Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-fscjk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T15:42:33.074Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Part III - The Phonetics and Phonology of the Bilingual Child

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 November 2024

Mark Amengual
Affiliation:
University of California, Santa Cruz
Get access
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2024

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

References

Abboub, N., Bijeljac-Babic, R., Serres, J., & Nazzi, T. (2015). On the importance of being bilingual: Word stress processing in a context of segmental variability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 132, 111120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Albereda-Castallot, B., Pons, F., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2010). The acquisition of phonetic categories in bilingual infants. Developmental Science, 14(2), 395401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Antovich, D. M. & Graf Estes, K. (2018). Learning across languages: Bilingual experience supports dual language statistical word segmentation. Developmental Science, 21(2), e12548.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bail, A., Morini, G., & Newman, R. S. (2015). Look at the gato! Code-switching in speech to toddlers. Journal of Child Language, 42(5), 10731101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bijeljac-Babic, R., Höhle, B., & Nazzi, T. (2016). Early prosodic acquisition in bilingual infants: The case of the perceptual trochaic bias. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bijeljac-Babic, R., Serres, J., Hohle, B., & Nazzi, T. (2012). Effect of bilingualism on lexical stress pattern discrimination in French-learning infants. PLoS ONE, 7, e30843.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bornstein, M. H., Haynes, M. O., & Painter, K. M. (1998). Sources of child vocabulary competence: A multivariate model. Journal of Child Language, 25, 367393.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bosch, L., Figueras, M., Teixidó, M., & Ramón-Casas, M. (2013). Rapid gains in segmenting fluent speech when words match the rhythmic unit: Evidence from infants acquiring syllable-timed languages. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bosch, L. & Ramón-Casas, M. (2011). Variability in vowel production by bilingual speakers: Can input properties hinder the early stabilization of contrastive categories? Journal of Phonetics, 39, 514526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bosch, L. & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (1997). Native-language recognition abilities in 4-month-old infants from monolingual and bilingual environments. Cognition, 65, 3369.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bosch, L. & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2001). Evidence of early language discrimination abilities in infants from bilingual environments. Infancy, 2, 2949.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bosch, L. & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2003a). Simultaneous bilingualism and the perception of a language-specific vowel contrast in the first year of life. Language and Speech, 46(2–3), 217243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bosch, L. & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2003b). Language experience and the perception of a voicing contrast in fricatives: Infant and adult data. In Solé, M. J., Recasens, D., & Romero, J., eds., International Congress of Phonetic Sciences. Barcelona: Causal Productions, pp. 19871990.Google Scholar
Bridges, K. & Hoff, E. (2014). Older sibling influences on the language environment and language development of toddlers in bilingual homes. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35(2), 225241.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burns, T. C., Yoshida, K. A., Hill, K., & Werker, J. F. (2007). The development of phonetic representation in bilingual and monolingual infants. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 455474.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byers-Heinlein, K. (2013). Parental language mixing: Its measurement and the relation of mixed input to young bilingual children’s vocabulary size. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16(1), 3248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byers-Heinlein, K., Burns, T. C., & Werker, J. F. (2010). The roots of bilingualism in newborns. Psychological Science, 21, 343348.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byers-Heinlein, K., Fennell, C. T., & Werker, J. F. (2013). The development of associative word learning in monolingual and bilingual infants. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16(1), 198205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byers-Heinlein, K., Morin-Lessard, E., & Lew-Williams, C. (2017). Bilingual infants control their languages as they listen. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114, 90329037.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carbajal, M. J., Fér, R., & Dupoux, E. (2016). Modeling language discrimination in infants using i-vector representations. In Papagragou, A., Grodner, D., Mirman, D., & Trueswell, J. C., eds., Proceedings of the 38th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, Recognizing and Representing Events. Philadelphia, PA: Cognitive Science Society, pp. 889894.Google Scholar
Cattani, A., Abbot‐Smith, K., Farag, R., et al. (2014). How much exposure to English is necessary for a bilingual toddler to perform like a monolingual peer in language tests? International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 49(6), 649671.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conboy, B., Rivera-Gaxiola, M., Klarman, L., Aksoylu, E., & Kuhl, P. K. (2005). Associations between native and nonnative speech sound discrimination and language development at the end of the first year. In Brugos, A., Clark-Cotton, M. R., & Ha, S., eds., Supplement to the Proceedings of the 29th Boston University Conference on Language Development. Boston, MA: BUCLD. www.bu.edu/bucld/files/2011/05/29-ConboyBUCLD2004.pdf.Google Scholar
Conboy, B. T. & Mills, D. L. (2006). Two languages, one developing brain: Effects of vocabulary size on bilingual toddlers’ event-related potentials to auditory words. Developmental Science, 9(1), F1F11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Curtin, S., Byers-Heinlein, K., & Werker, J. F. (2011). Bilingual beginnings as a lens for theory development: PRIMIR in focus. Journal of Phonetics, 39(4), 492504.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeAnda, S., Bosch, L., Poulin-Dubois, D., Zesiger, P., & Friend, M. (2016). The language exposure assessment tool: Quantifying language exposure in infants and children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 59(6), 13461356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeAnda, S., Hendrickson, K., Zesiger, P., Poulain-Dubois, D., & French, M. (2018). Lexical access in the second year: A study of monolingual and bilingual vocabulary development. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21, 314327.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Bree, E., Verhagen, J., Kerkhoff, A., Doedens, W., & Unsworth, S. (2017). Language learning from inconsistent input: Bilingual and monolingual toddlers compared. Infant & Child Development, 26, e1996.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Delattre, P. (1965). Comparing the Phonetic Features of English, French, German and Spanish: An Interim Report. Heidelberg: Julius Groos.Google Scholar
Durrant, S., Delle Luche, C., Cattani, A., & Floccia, C. (2015). Monodialectal and multidialectal infants’ representation of familiar words. Journal of Child Language, 42(2), 447465.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fecher, N. & Johnson, E. K. (2019). Bilingual infants excel at foreign-language talker recognition. Developmental Science, 22(4), e12778.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fecher, N. & Johnson, E. K. (2022). Revisiting the talker recognition advantage in bilingual infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 214, 105276.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fennell, C. T. & Byers-Heinlein, K. (2014). You sound like Mommy: Bilingual and monolingual infants learn words best from speakers typical of their language environments. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 38(4), 309316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fennell, C. T., Byers-Heinlein, K., & Werker, J. F. (2007). Using speech sounds to guide word learning: The case of bilingual infants. Child Development, 78(5), 15101525.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., et al. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59(5), 1185.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferjan Ramírez, N., Ramírez, R. R., Clarke, M., Taulu, S., & Kuhl, P. K. (2017). Speech discrimination in 11‐month‐old bilingual and monolingual infants: A magnetoencephalography study. Developmental Science, 20(1), e12427.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fibla, L., Sebastián-Gallés, N., & Cristia, A. (2021). Is there a bilingual disadvantage for word segmentation? A computational modeling approach. Journal of Child Language, 49(6), 128.Google Scholar
Floccia, C., Sambrook, T. D., Delle Luche, C., et al. (2018). Vocabulary of 2-year-olds learning English and an additional language: Norms and effects of linguistic distance. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 83, 328.Google Scholar
Friedrich, M., Herold, B., & Friederici, A. D. (2009). ERP correlates of processing native and non-native language word stress in infants with different language outcomes. Cortex, 45(5), 662676.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
García-Sierra, A., Rivera-Gaxiola, M., Percaccio, C. R., et al. (2011). Bilingual language learning: An ERP study relating early brain responses to speech, language input, and later word production. Journal of Phonetics, 39(4), 546557.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gasparini, L., Langus, A., Tsuji, S., & Boll-Avetisyan, N. (2021). Quantifying the role of rhythm in infants’ language discrimination abilities: A meta-analysis. Cognition, 213, 104757.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gathercole, V. C. M. & Thomas, E. M. (2009). Bilingual first-language development: Dominant language takeover, threatened minority language take-up. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12(2), 213237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Genesee, F. (1989). Early bilingual development: One language or two? Journal of Child Language, 16, 161180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gervain, J. & Werker, J. (2013). Prosody cues word order in 7‐month‐old bilingual infants. Nature Communications, 4, 14901496.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graf Estes, K. & Hay, J. F. (2015). Flexibility in bilingual infants’ word learning. Child Development, 86(5), 13711385.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, J. N. (1988). Spanish. In Harris, M. & Vincent, N., eds., The Romance Languages. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 79130.Google Scholar
Hart, B. & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful Differences in the Everyday Experience of Young American Children. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
Havy, M., Bouchon, C., & Nazzi, T. (2016). Phonetic processing when learning words: The case of bilingual infants. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 40(1), 4152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hay, J. F., Graf Estes, K., Wang, T., & Saffran, J. R. (2015). From flexibility to constraint: The contrastive use of lexical tone in early word learning. Child Development, 86, 1022.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heller, M. (2010). Codeswitching: Anthropological and Sociolinguistic Perspectives. Vol. 48 of Contributions to the Sociology of Language. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Höhle, B., Bijeljac-Babic, R., Herold, B., Weissenborn, J., & Nazzi, T. (2009). Language specific prosodic preferences during the first half year of life: Evidence from German and French infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 32, 262274.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, E. K. & Tyler, M. (2010). Testing the limits of statistical learning for word segmentation. Developmental Science, 13, 339345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jusczyk, P. W., Houston, D. M., & Newsome, M. (1999). The beginnings of word segmentation in English-learning infants. Cognitive Psychology, 39(3–4), 159207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kisilevsky, B. S., Hains, S. M., Brown, C. A., et al. (2009). Fetal sensitivity to properties of maternal speech and language. Infant Behavior & Development, 32(1), 5971.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kovács, A. M. & Mehler, J. (2009). Flexible learning of multiple speech structures in bilingual infants. Science, 325, 611612.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kremin, L., Alves, J., Orena, A., Polka, L., & Byers-Heinlein, K. (2022). Code-switching in parents’ everyday speech to bilingual infants. Journal of Child Language, 49(4), 714740.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhl, P. K., Conboy, B. T., Coffey-Corina, S., Padden, D., & Rivera-Gaxiola, M. (2008). Phonetic learning as a pathway to language: New data and native language magnet theory expanded (NLM-e). Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 363, 9791000.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Legacy, J., Zesiger, P., Friend, M., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2018). Vocabulary size and speed of word recognition in very young French-English bilinguals: A longitudinal study. Bilingualism, 21(1), 137149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, L. & Kager, R. (2014). Perception of tones by infants learning a non-tone language. Cognition, 133(2), 385394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, L. & Kager, R. (2016). Perception of a native vowel contrast by Dutch monolingual and bilingual infants: A bilingual perceptual lead. International Journal of Bilingualism, 20(3), 335345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, L. & Kager, R. (2017). Perception of tones by bilingual infants learning non-tone languages. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20(3), 561575.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marchman, V. A., Fernald, A., & Hurtado, N. (2010). How vocabulary size in two languages relates to efficiency in spoken word recognition by young Spanish-English bilinguals. Journal of Child Language, 37, 817840.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marchman, V. A., Martínez, L. Z., Hurtado, N., Grüter, T., & Fernald, A. (2017). Caregiver talk to young Spanish-English bilinguals: Comparing direct observation and parent-report measures of dual-language exposure. Developmental Science, 20, e12425.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mateu, V. E. & Sundara, M. (2022). Spanish input accelerates bilingual infants’ segmentation of English words. Cognition, 218, 104936.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mattock, K. & Burnham, D. (2006). Chinese and English infants’ tone perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization. Infancy, 10, 241265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mattock, K., Molnar, M., Polka, L., & Burnham, D. (2008). The developmental course of lexical tone perception in the first year of life. Cognition, 106(3), 13671381.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mattock, K., Polka, L., Rvachew, S., & Krehm, M. (2010). The first steps in word learning are easier when the shoes fit: Comparing monolingual and bilingual infants. Developmental Science, 13, 229243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mehler, J., Jusczyk, P. W., Lambertz, G., et al. (1988). A precursor of language acquisition in young infants. Cognition, 29, 143178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meisel, J. M. (2001). The simultaneous acquisition of two first languages: Early differentiation and subsequent development of grammars. In Cenoz, J. & Genesee, F., eds., Trends in Bilingual Acquisition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 1141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meisel, J. M. (2007). On autonomous syntactic development in multiple first language acquisition. In Caunt-Nulton, H., Kulatilake, S., & Woo, I., eds., Proceedings of the 31st Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press, pp. 126.Google Scholar
Minai, U., Gustafson, K., Fiorentino, R., Jongman, A., & Sereno, J. (2017). Fetal rhythm-based language discrimination: A biomagnetometry study. Neuroreport, 28(10), 561564.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Molnar, M., Gervain, J., & Carreira, M. (2014). Within-rhythm class native language discrimination abilities of Basque-Spanish monolingual and bilingual infants at 3.5 months of age. Infancy, 19, 326337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moon, C., Cooper, R. P., & Fifer, W. P. (1993). Two-day-olds prefer their native language. Infant Behavior and Development, 16, 495500.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mora, J. C. & Nadeu, M. (2012). L2 effects on the perception and production of a native vowel contrast in early bilinguals. International Journal of Bilingualism, 16(4), 484500.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Müller, N. (2017). Different sources of delay and acceleration in early child bilingualism. Zeitschrift für Sprachwissenschaft, 36(1), 730.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nicoladis, E. & Secco, G. (2000). The role of a child’s productive vocabulary in the language choice of a bilingual family. First Language, 20(58), 328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orena, A. J., Byers-Heinlein, K., & Polka, L. (2019). Reliability of the language environment analysis (LENA) in a French-English bilingual speech context. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 62(7), 24912500.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orena, A. J. & Polka, L. (2019). Monolingual and bilingual infants’ word segmentation abilities in an inter‐mixed dual‐language task. Infancy, 24(5), 718737.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J. & Genesee, F. (1996). Syntactic acquisition in bilingual children: Autonomous or interdependent? Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 18(1), 125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petitto, L. A., Berens, M. S., Kovelman, I., et al. (2012). The “Perceptual Wedge Hypothesis” as the basis for bilingual babies’ phonetic processing advantage: New insights from fNIRS brain imaging. Brain and Language, 121(2), 130143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Polka, L., Orena, A. J., Sundara, M., & Worrall, J. (2017). Segmenting words from fluent speech in infancy: Challenges and opportunities in the bilingual context. Developmental Science, 20(1), e12419.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Potter, C., Fourakis, E., Morin-Lessard, E., Byers-Heinlein, K., & Lew-Williams, C. (2019). Bilingual toddlers’ comprehension of mixed sentences is asymmetrical across their two languages. Developmental Science, 22, e12794.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Querleu, D., Renard, X., & Crepin, G. (1981). Bruit intra-utérin et perceptions auditives du foetus [Intra-uterine sound and fetal auditory perception]. Bulletin de l’Academie nationale de medecine, 165(5), 581588.Google Scholar
Ramírez-Esparza, N., García-Sierra, A., & Kuhl, P. K. (2010). Naturalistic social communication and speech development in monolingual and bilingual infants. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 128, 2459.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ramón-Casas, M. & Bosch, L. (2010). Are non-cognate words phonologically better specified than cognates in the early lexicon of bilingual children? In Ortega-Llebaria, M., ed., Proceedings of the 4th Conference on Laboratory Approaches to Spanish Phonology. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press, pp. 3136.Google Scholar
Ramón-Casas, M., Fennell, C. T., & Bosch, L. (2017). Minimal-pair word learning by bilingual toddlers: The Catalan/e/-/ɛ/contrast revisited. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20(3), 649656.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ramón-Casas, M., Swingley, D., Sebastián-Gállés, N., & Bosch, L. (2009). Vowel categorization during word recognition in bilingual toddlers. Cognitive Psychology, 59, 96121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramus, F., Hauser, M. D., Miller, C., Morris, D., & Mehler, J. (2000). Language discrimination by human newborns and cotton-top tamarin monkeys. Science, 288, 349351.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rivera-Gaxiola, M., Silva-Pereyra, J., & Kuhl, P. K. (2005). Brain potentials to native and non-native speech contrasts in 7- and 11-month-old American infants. Developmental Science, 8, 162172.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roark, B. & Demuth, K. (2000). Prosodic constraints and the learner’s environment: A corpus study. In Howell, S. C., Fish, S. A., & Keith-Lucas, T., eds., Proceedings of the 24th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, Vol. 2. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press, pp. 597608.Google Scholar
Saffran, J. R., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L. (1996). Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science, 274(5294), 19261928.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Santolin, C. & Saffran, J. R. (2018). Constraints on statistical learning across species. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(1), 5263.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schott, E., Mastroberardino, M., Fourakis, E., Lew-Williams, C., & Byers-Heinlein, K. (2021). Fine-tuning language discrimination: Monolingual and bilingual infants’ detection of language switching. Infancy, 26, 10371056.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sebastián-Gallés, N., Albareda-Castellot, B., Weikum, W. M., & Werker, J. F. (2012). A bilingual advantage in visual language discrimination in infancy. Psychological Science, 23(9), 994999.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sebastián-Gallés, N. & Bosch, L. (2002). Building phonotactic knowledge in bilinguals: Role of early exposure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28(4), 974989.Google ScholarPubMed
Sebastián-Gallés, N. & Bosch, L. (2009). Developmental shift in the discrimination of vowel contrasts in bilingual infants: Is the distributional account all there is to it? Developmental Science, 12, 874887.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shafer, V. L., Yu, Y. H., & Datta, H. (2011). The development of English vowel perception in monolingual and bilingual infants: Neurophysiological correlates. Journal of Phonetics, 39(4), 527545.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shi, R., Santos, E., Gao, J., & Li, A. (2017). Perception of similar and dissimilar lexical tones by non‐tone‐learning infants. Infancy, 22, 790800.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silvén, M., Voeten, M., Kouvo, A., & Lundén, M. (2014). Speech perception and vocabulary growth: A longitudinal study of Finnish-Russian bilinguals and Finnish monolinguals from infancy to three years. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 38(4), 323332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singh, L. (2018). Bilingual infants demonstrate advantages in learning words in a third language. Child Development, 89(4), e397e413.Google Scholar
Singh, L. (2019). Does infant speech perception predict later vocabulary development in bilingual infants? Journal of Phonetics, 76, 100914.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singh, L. & Foong, J. (2012). Influences of lexical tone and pitch on word recognition in bilingual infants. Cognition, 124, 128142.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singh, L., Fu, C. S. L., Seet, X. H., et al. (2018). Developmental change in tone perception in Mandarin monolingual, English monolingual, and Mandarin-English bilingual infants: Divergences between monolingual and bilingual learners. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 173, 5977.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singh, L., Fu, C. S. L., Tay, Z. W., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2018). Novel word learning in bilingual and monolingual infants: Evidence for a bilingual advantage. Child Development, 89(3), e183e198.Google ScholarPubMed
Singh, L., Poh, F. L. S., & Fu, C. S. L. (2016). Limits on monolingualism? A comparison of monolingual and bilinguals infants’ abilities to integrate lexical tone in novel word learning. Frontiers in Psychology, 7.Google ScholarPubMed
Singh, L. & Tan, A. (2021). Beyond perceptual narrowing: Monolingual and bilingual infants discriminate Hindi contrasts when learning words in the second year of life. Developmental Psychology, 57(1), 1932.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skoruppa, K., Cristià, A., Peperkamp, S., & Seidl, A. (2011). English-learning infants’ perception of word stress patterns. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 130, EL50–EL55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skoruppa, K., Pons, F., Christophe, A., et al. (2009). Language-specific stress perception by 9-month-old French and Spanish infants. Developmental Science, 12, 914919.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sundara, M. & Mateu, V. E. (2018). Lexical stress constrains English-learning infants’ segmentation in a non-native language. Cognition, 181, 105116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sundara, M. & Mayer, C. (2018). Modeling developmental changes in infants’ discrimination of English vowels. Poster presented at the 43rd Boston University Conference on Language Development [BUCLD 43].Google Scholar
Sundara, M., Polka, L., & Baum, S. (2006). Production of coronal stops by simultaneous bilingual adults. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 9(1), 97114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sundara, M., Polka, L., & Molnar, M. (2008). Development of coronal stop perception: Bilingual infants keep pace with their monolingual peers. Cognition, 108(1), 232242.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sundara, M. & Scutellaro, A. (2011). Rhythmic distance between languages affects the development of speech perception in bilingual infants. Journal of Phonetics, 39(4), 505513.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tare, M. & Gelman, S. A. (2011). Bilingual parents’ modeling of pragmatic language use in multiparty interactions. Applied Psycholinguistics, 32(4), 761780.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thordardottir, E., Rothenberg, A., Rivard, M., & Naves, R. (2006). Bilingual assessment: Can overall proficiency be estimated from separate measurements of two languages? Journal of Multilingual Communication Disorders, 4, 121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Toro, J. M., Sinnett, S., & Soto-Faraco, S. (2005). Speech segmentation by statistical learning depends on attention. Cognition, 97(2), B25B34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toro, J. M., Trobalon, J. B., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2003). The use of prosodic cues in language discrimination tasks by rats. Animal Cognition, 6, 131136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tsui, A. S. M., Erickson, L. C., Mallikarjunn, A., Thiessen, E. D., & Fennell, C. T. (2021). Dual language statistical word segmentation in infancy: Simulating a language‐mixing bilingual environment. Developmental Science, 24(3), e13050.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van der Feest, S. V. H., Rose, M. C., & Johnson, E. K. (2022). Showing strength through flexibility: Multi-accent toddlers recognize words quickly and efficiently. Brain and Language, 227, 105083.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Volterra, V. & Taeschner, T. (1978). The acquisition and development of language by bilingual children. Journal of Child Language, 5, 311326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Von Holzen, K., Fennell, C. T., & Mani, N. (2019). The impact of cross-language phonological overlap on bilingual and monolingual toddlers’ word recognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 22(3), 476499.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weikum, W. M., Vouloumanos, A., Navarra, J., et al. (2007). Visual language discrimination in infancy. Science, 316(5828), 1159.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiss, D., Gerfen, C., & Mitchel, A. (2010). Colliding cues in word segmentation: The role of cue strength and general cognitive processes. Language and Cognitive Processes, 25(3), 402422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Werker, J. F., Fennell, C. T., Corcoran, K., & Stager, C. L. (2002). Infants’ ability to learn phonetically similar words: Effects of age and vocabulary size. Infancy, 3(1), 130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wewalaarachchi, T. D., Wong, L. H., & Singh, L. (2017). Vowels, consonants and lexical tones: Sensitivity to phonological variation in monolingual Mandarin and bilingual English-Mandarin toddlers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 159, 1633.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yeung, H. H., Chen, K. H., & Werker, J. F. (2013). When does native language input affect phonetic perception? The precocious case of lexical tone. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(2), 123139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yoshida, H., Tran, D. N., Benitez, V., & Kuwabara, M. (2011). Inhibition and adjective learning in bilingual and monolingual children. Developmental Psychology, 2, 210.Google ScholarPubMed
Zacharaki, K. & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2021). The ontogeny of early language discrimination: Beyond rhythm. Cognition, 213, 104628.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

References

Almeida, L., Rose, Y., & Freitas, J. (2012). Prosodic influence in bilingual phonological development: Evidence from a Portuguese-French first language learner. In Biller, A., Chung, E., & Kimball, A., eds., Proceedings of the 36th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press, pp. 4252.Google Scholar
Brysbaert, M. (2021). Power considerations in bilingualism research: Time to step up our game. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 24(5), 813818.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bunta, F. & Ingram, D. (2007). The acquisition of speech rhythm by bilingual Spanish- and English-speaking 4- and 5-year-old children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 9991014.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooperson, S., Bedore, L., & Peña, E. (2013). The relationship of phonological skills to language skills in Spanish-English-speaking bilingual children. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 27, 371389.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Curtin, S., Byers-Heinlein, K., & Werker, J. (2011). Bilingual beginnings as a lens for theory development: PRIMIR in focus. Journal of Phonetics, 39, 492504.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dodane, C. & Bijeljac-Babic, R. (2017). Cross-language influences in the productions of bilingual children: Separation or interaction. In Yavas, M., Kehoe, M., & Cardoso, W., eds., Romance-Germanic Bilingual Phonology. Sheffield: Equinox, pp. 3855.Google Scholar
Fabiano, L. & Goldstein, B. (2005). Phonological cross-linguistic effects in bilingual Spanish-English children. Journal of Multilingual Communication Disorders, 3, 5663.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fabiano-Smith, L. & Barlow, J. (2010). Interaction in bilingual phonological acquisition: Evidence from phonetic inventories. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 13, 117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fabiano-Smith, L. & Bunta, F. (2012). Voice onset time of voiceless bilabial and velar stops in 3-year-old bilingual children and their age-matched monolingual peers. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 26, 148163.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fabiano-Smith, L. & Goldstein, B. (2010a). Early, middle, and late-developing sounds in monolingual and bilingual children: An exploratory study. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 19, 112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fabiano-Smith, L. & Goldstein, B. (2010b). Phonological acquisition in bilingual Spanish-English speaking children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53, 119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fabiano-Smith, L., Oglivie, T., Maiefski, O., & Schertz, J. (2015). Acquisition of the stop-spirant alternation in bilingual Mexican Spanish-English speaking children: Theoretical and clinical implications. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 29, 126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fennell, C. T., Byers-Heinlein, K., & Werker, J. F. (2007). Using speech sounds to guide word learning: The case of bilingual infants. Child Development, 78, 15101525.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flege, J. E. (1987). A critical period for learning to pronounce foreign languages? Applied Linguistics, 8, 162177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flege, J. E. (1995). Second-language speech learning: Theory, findings, and problems. In Strange, W., ed., Speech Perception and Linguistic Experience: Issues in Cross-Language Research. Timonium, MD: York Press, pp. 233273.Google Scholar
Flege, J. E. & Bohn, O.-S. (2021). The revised Speech Learning Model (SLM-r). In Wayland, R., ed., Second Language Speech Learning: Theoretical and Empirical Progress. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Genesee, F. (1989). Early bilingual development: One language or two? Journal of Child Language, 6, 161179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gildersleeve-Neumann, C., Kester, E., Davis, B., & Peña, E. (2008). English speech sound development in preschool-aged children from bilingual English-Spanish environments. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 39, 314328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gildersleeve-Neumann, C., Peña, E., Davis, B., & Kester, E. (2009). Effects of L1 during early acquisition of L2: Speech changes in Spanish at first contact. Bilingualism: Language & Cognition, 12, 259272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gildersleeve-Neumann, C. & Wright, K. (2010). English speech acquisition in 3- to 5-year-old children learning Russian and English. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 41, 429444.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldstein, B., Bunta, F., Lange, J., Rodriguez, J., & Burrows, L. (2010). The effects of measures of language experience and language ability on segmental accuracy in bilingual children. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 19, 238247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldstein, B., Fabiano, L., & Washington, P. (2005). Phonological skills in predominantly English-speaking, predominantly Spanish-speaking, and Spanish-English bilingual children. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 36, 201218.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldstein, B. & Washington, P. (2001). An initial investigation of phonological patterns in typically developing 4-year-old Spanish English bilingual children. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 32, 153164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grabe, E. & Low, E. (2002). Durational variability in speech and the rhythm class hypothesis. In Gussenhoven, C. & Warner, N., eds., Papers in Laboratory Phonology 7. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, pp. 515546.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grabe, E., Post, B., & Watson, I. (1999). The acquisition of rhythm in English and French. In Proceedings of the 14th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (ICPhS99), 2, London: International Phonetic Association, pp. 12011204. www.internationalphoneticassociation.org/icphs-proceedings/ICPhS1999/papers/p14_1201.pdf.Google Scholar
Grech, H. & Dodd, B. (2008). Phonological acquisition in Malta: A bilingual language learning context. International Journal of Bilingualism, 12, 155–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hambly, H., Wren, Y., McLeod, S., & Roulstone, S. (2013). The influence of bilingualism on speech production: A systematic review. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 48, 124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holm, A., Dodd, B., Stow, C., & Pert, S. (1999). Identification and differential diagnosis of phonological disorder in bilingual children. Language Testing, 16, 271292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, C. & Wilson, I. (2002). Phonetic evidence for early language differentiation: Research issues and some preliminary data. International Journal of Bilingualism, 6, 271289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kartushina, N. & Frauenfelder, U. (2014). On the effects of L2 perception and of individual differences in L1 production on L2 pronunciation. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1246. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keffala, B., Barlow, J., & Rose, S. (2018). Interaction in Spanish-English bilinguals’ acquisition of syllable structure. International Journal of Bilingualism, 22, 1637.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keffala, B., Scarpino, S., Hammer, C., et al. (2020). Vocabulary and phonological abilities affect dual-language learners’ consonant production accuracy within and across languages: A large-scale study of 3- to 6-year-old Spanish-English dual language learners. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 29, 11961211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kehoe, M. (2015a). Cross-linguistic interaction: A retrospective and prospective view. In Babatsouli, E. & Ingram, D., eds., Proceedings of the International Symposium on Monolingual and Bilingual Speech 2015. Chania, Greece: Institute of Monolingual and Bilingual Speech, pp. 141167. http://ismbs.eu/publications.Google Scholar
Kehoe, M. (2015b). Lexical-phonological interactions in bilingual children. First Language, 35, 93125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kehoe, M. (2018). The development of rhotics: A comparison of monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21, 710731.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kehoe, M. (2022). The prosody of two-syllable words in French-speaking monolingual and bilingual children: A focus on initial accent and final accent. Language and Speech, 65, 444471.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kehoe, M., Friend, M., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2021). Relations between phonological production, grammar and the lexicon in bilingual French-English children. International Journal of Bilingualism, 25, 15761596.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kehoe, M. & Girardier, C. (2020). What factors influence phonological production in French-speaking bilingual children, aged three to six years? Journal of Child Language, 47, 945981.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kehoe, M. & Havy, M. (2019). Bilingual phonological acquisition: The influence of language-internal, language-external and lexical factors. Journal of Child Language, 46, 292333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kehoe, M., Lleó, C., & Rakow, M. (2004). Voice onset time in bilingual German-Spanish children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 7, 7188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kehoe, M., Lleó, C., & Rakow, M. (2011). Speech rhythm in the pronunciation of German and Spanish monolingual and German-Spanish bilingual 3-year-olds. Linguistische Berichte, 227, 323351.Google Scholar
LaBelle, C. (2000). A longitudinal study of lexical and prosodic differentiation by a simultaneous French/English bilingual child (1;5–2;3). In Howell, S. C., Fish, S., & Keith-Lucas, T., eds., Proceedings of the 24th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press, pp. 474485.Google Scholar
Lanza, E. (1992). Can bilingual two-year-olds code-switch? Journal of Child Language, 19, 633658.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, S. & Iverson, G. (2012a). Stop consonant productions of Korean-English bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15, 275287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, S. & Iverson, G. (2012b). Vowel category formation in Korean-English bilingual children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 55, 14491462.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, S. & Iverson, G. (2017). The emergence of phonetic categories in Korean-English bilingual children. Journal of Child Language, 44, 14851515.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lisker, L. & Abramson, A. S. (1964). A cross-language study of voicing in initial stops: Acoustical measurements. Word, 20, 384422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lleó, C., Kuchenbrandt, I., Kehoe, M., & Trujillo, C. (2003). Syllable final consonants in Spanish and German monolingual and bilingual acquisition. In Müller, N., ed., (Non)Vulnerable Domains in Bilingualism. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 191220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lleó, C. & Rakow, M. (2005). Markedness effects in voiced stop spirantization in bilingual German-Spanish children. In Cohen, J., McAlister, K., Rolstad, K., & McSwan, J., eds., Proceedings of the 4th International Symposium on Bilingualism (ISB4), Vol. 81. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press, pp. 13531371.Google Scholar
Mack, M. (1990). Phonetic transfer in a French-English bilingual child. In Nelde, P., ed., Language Attitudes and Language Conflict. Bonn: Dümmler, pp. 107124.Google Scholar
Macken, M. A. & Barton, D. (1980a). The acquisition of the voicing contrast in English: A study of voice onset time in world-initial stop consonants. Journal of Child Language, 7, 4174.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Macken, M. A. & Barton, D. (1980b). The acquisition of the voicing contrast in Spanish: A phonetic and phonological study of word-initial stop consonants. Journal of Child Language, 7, 433458.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacLeod, A., Laukys, K., & Rvachew, S. (2011). The impact of bilingual language learning on whole-word complexity and segmental accuracy among children aged 18 and 36 months. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 13, 490499.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacWhinney, B. (2005). A unified model of language acquisition. In Kroll, J. & De Groot, A., eds., Handbook of Bilingualism: Psycholinguistic Approaches. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 4967.Google Scholar
Marecka, M., Wrembel, M., Otwinowska, A., et al. (2020). Bilingual children’s phonology shows evidence of transfer, but not deceleration in their L1. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 42, 89114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayr, R., Howells, G., & Lewis, R. (2015). Asymmetries in phonological development: The case of word-final cluster acquisition in Welsh-English bilingual children. Journal of Child Language, 42, 146179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayr, R. & Montanari, S. (2015). Cross-linguistic interaction in trilingual phonological development: The role of the input in the acquisition of the voicing contrast. Journal of Child Language, 42, 10061035.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayr, R. & Siddika, A. (2018). Inter-generational transmission in a minority language setting: Stop consonant production by Bangladeshi heritage children and adults. International Journal of Bilingualism, 22, 255284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCarthy, K., Mahon, M., Rosen, S., & Evans, B. (2014). Speech perception and production by sequential bilingual children: A longitudinal study of voice onset time acquisition. Child Development, 85, 19651980.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meinhold, G. & Stock, E. (1980). Phonologie der deutschen Gegenwartssprache. Leipzig : Bibliographisches Institut.Google Scholar
Meziane, R. & MacLeod, A. (2017). L’acquisition de la phonologie en français langue seconde : le profile phonologique d’enfants allophones en maternelle. Canadian Journal of Applied Linguistics, 20, 117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mok, P. (2011). The acquisition of rhythm by three-year-old bilingual and monolingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14, 458472.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mok, P. (2013). Speech rhythm of monolingual and bilingual children at age 2;6: Cantonese and English. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16, 693703.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montanari, S., Mayr, R., & Subrahmanyam, K. (2018). Bilingual speech sound development during the preschool years: The role of language proficiency and cross-linguistic relatedness. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 61, 24672486.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morrow, A., Goldstein, B., Gilhool, A., & Paradis, J. (2014). Phonological skills in English language learners. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 45, 2639.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paradis, J. & Genesee, F. (1996). Syntactic acquisition in bilingual children: Autonomous or interdependent? Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 18, 125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Payne, E., Post, B., Astruc, A., Prieto, P., & Vanrell, M. (2012). Measuring child rhythm. Language and Speech, 55, 203229.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramus, F., Nespor, M., & Mehler, J. (1999). Correlates of linguistic rhythm in the speech signal. Cognition, 73, 265292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Redlinger, W. & Park, T. (1980). Language mixing in young bilinguals. Journal of Child Language, 7, 337–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rose, Y. & Champdoizeau, C. (2007). There is no innate trochaic bias: Acoustic evidence in favour of the neutral start hypothesis. In Gavarro, A. & Freitas, M. J., eds., Proceedings of the Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition Conference 2007. Newcastle-upon-Tyne: Cambridge Scholars, pp. 357367.Google Scholar
Ruiz-Felter, R., Cooperson, S., Bedore, L., & Peña, E. (2016). Influence of current input-output and age of first exposure on phonological acquisition in early bilingual Spanish-English-speaking kindergarteners. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 51, 36383.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scarpino, S., Hammer, C., Goldstein, B., Rodriguez, B., & Lopez, L. (2019). Effects of home language, oral language skills, and cross-linguistic phonological abilities on whole-word proximity in Spanish-English speaking children. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 28, 174187.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmidt, E. & Post, B. (2015). Language interaction in the development of speech rhythm in simultaneous bilinguals. In Delais-Roussarie, E., Avanzi, M., & Herment, S., eds., Prosody and Language in Contact. New York: Springer, pp. 271291.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schnitzer, M. & Krasinski, E. (1994). The development of segmental phonological production in a bilingual child. Journal of Child Language, 21, 585622.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schnitzer, M. & Krasinski, E. (1996). The development of segmental phonological production in a bilingual child: A contrasting second case. Journal of Child Language, 23, 547571.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sim, J. & Post, B. (2022). Variation in quality of maternal input and development of coda stops in English-speaking children in Singapore. Journal of Child Language, 49, 11471172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sorenson Duncan, T. & Paradis, J. (2016). English language learners’ nonword repetition performance: The influence of L2 vocabulary size, length of L2 exposure and L1 phonology. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 59, 3948.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stoehr, A., Benders, T., van Hell, J., & Fikkert, P. (2018). Heritage language exposure impacts voice onset time of Dutch–German simultaneous bilingual preschoolers. Bilingualism: Language & Cognition, 21, 598617.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stoehr, A., Benders, T., van Hell, J., & Fikkert, P. (2019). Bilingual preschoolers’ speech is associated with non-native maternal language input. Language Learning and Development, 15, 75100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stoel-Gammon, C. (2011). Relationships between lexical and phonological development in young children. Journal of Child Language, 38, 134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tamburelli, M., Sanoudaki, E., Jones, G., & Sowinska, M. (2015). Acceleration in the bilingual acquisition of phonological structure: Evidence from Polish-English children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18, 713725.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vihman, M. M. (2002). Getting started without a system: From phonetics to phonology in bilingual development. International Journal of Bilingualism, 6, 239254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vihman, M. M. (2016). Prosodic structures and templates in bilingual phonological development. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19, 6988.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Volterra, V. & Taeschner, T. (1978). The acquisition and development of language by bilingual children. Journal of Child Language, 5, 311326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Werker, J. & Curtin, S. (2005). PRIMIR: A developmental framework of infant speech processing. Language Learning and Development, 1, 197234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yang, J. (2021). Vowel development in young Mandarin-English bilingual children. Phonetica, 78, 241272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, J. & Fox, R. (2017). L1–L2 interactions of vowel systems in young bilingual Mandarin-English children. Journal of Phonetics, 65, 6076.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yang, J., Fox, R., & Jacewicz, E. (2015). Vowel development in an emergent Mandarin-English bilingual child: A longitudinal study. Journal of Child Language, 42, 11251143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

References

Albrecht, K. M. (2017). The identification of typical and atypical phonological acquisition in Turkish-German bilingual children. [PhD thesis, University of Sheffield].Google Scholar
ASHA Archives. (2022). Photograph of minutes from first meeting. [Website]. https://ashaarchives.omeka.net/items/show/8.Google Scholar
Austin, L. (2010). Dynamic assessment: Whys and hows. Perspectives on School-Based Issues, 11(3), 8087.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Babatsouli, E. (2013). Phonological development of a child’s L2 English in bilingualism. [Doctoral dissertation, University of Crete].Google Scholar
Babatsouli, E. (2016). Chaos in monolingual and bilingual speech. In Skiadas, C. H. & Skiadas, C., eds., Handbook of Applications of Chaos Theory. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press, pp. 789890.Google Scholar
Babatsouli, E. (2019a). Linguistics. In Damico, J. S. & Ball, M. J., eds., The SAGE Encyclopedia of Communication Sciences and Disorders. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications, pp. 11091118.Google Scholar
Babatsouli, E. (2019b). A phonological assessment test for child Greek. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 33(7), 601607.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Babatsouli, E. (2020a). Prolegomenon. In Babatsouli, E., ed., On Under-Reported Monolingual Child Phonology. Bristol, UK: Multilingual Matters, pp. 117.Google Scholar
Babatsouli, E. (2020b). Enhanced phonology in a child’s weaker language in bilingualism: A portrait. In Babatsouli, E. & Ball, M. J., eds., An Anthology of Bilingual Child Phonology. Bristol, UK: Multilingual Matters, pp. 117139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Babatsouli, E. (2021a). Diversity considerations in speech and language disorders: A focus on training. In Damico, J. S., Müller, N., & Ball, M. J., eds., Handbook of Language and Speech Disorders, 2nd ed. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Blackwell, pp. 3352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Babatsouli, E. (2021b). Correlation between the measure for cluster proximity (MCP) and the percentage of consonants correct (PCC). Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 35(1), 6583.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Babatsouli, E. (2022). Speech errors Greeks make in English: A tutorial. In Kkese, E., ed., Speech Perception and Production in L2. Newcastle-upon-Tyne: Cambridge Scholars, pp. 78104.Google Scholar
Babatsouli, E. (2023). Ecosystemic clinical assessment of linguistic diversity: Greek-dominant speech. Perspectives of the ASHA Special Interest Groups, 123. https://doi.org/10.1044/2023_PERSP-23-00088.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Babatsouli, E. (2024). Multilingual Acquisition and Learning: An Ecosystemic View of Diversity. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Babatsouli, E. (in press). English-Greek speech development. In. S. McLeod, ed., The Oxford Handbook of Speech Development in Languages of the World. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Babatsouli, E. & Geronikou, E. (2022). Phonological delay of segmental sequences in a Greek child’s speech. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 36(7), 642656.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Babatsouli, E. & Ingram, D. (2018). Prologue. In Babatsouli, E. & Ingram, D., eds., Phonology in Protolanguage and Interlanguage. Sheffield: Equinox, pp. 123.Google Scholar
Babatsouli, E. & Pollock, K. (2020). History of the International Child Phonology Conference. In Babatsouli, E., ed., On Under-Reported Monolingual Child Phonology. Bristol, UK: Multilingual Matters, pp. 1824.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Babatsouli, E. & Sotiropoulos, D. (2018). A measure for cluster proximity (MCP) in child speech. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 32(12), 10711089.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baker, E., McCauley, R. J., Williams, A. L., & McLeod, S. (2020). Elements in phonological intervention: A comparison of three approaches using the phonological intervention taxonomy. In Babatsouli, E., ed., On Under-Reported Monolingual Child Phonology. Bristol, UK: Multilingual Matters, pp. 375399.Google Scholar
Ball, M. J. (2016). Principles of Clinical Phonology: Theoretical Approaches. Abingdon: Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
Ball, M. J. & Munro, S. M. (1981). Language assessment procedures for linguistic minorities: An example. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 2(4), 231241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Battle, B., ed. (2012). Communication Disorders and Development in Multicultural Populations. Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Bernhardt, B. M. & Stemberger, J. P. (2020). Phonological development: Research in multilingual and cross-cultural contexts. In Li, F., Pollock, K., & Gibb, R., eds., Child Bilingualism and Second Language Learning: Multidisciplinary Perspectives. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 223248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernhardt, B. M., Stemberger, J. P., Bérubé, D., et al. (2020). Identification of protracted phonological development across languages: The whole word match and basic mismatch measures. In Babatsouli, E. & Ball, M. J., eds., An Anthology of Bilingual Child Phonology. Bristol, UK: Multilingual Matters, pp. 274308.Google Scholar
Bernthal, J., Bankson, N., & Flipsen, R. (2013). Articulation and Phonological Disorders. London: Pearson.Google Scholar
Bérubé, D., Bernhardt, B. M., Stemberger, J. P., & de Almeida, L. (2014). Un test de phonologie du français (canadien et standard): Construction et utilisation. Orthophonie, 16, 172.Google Scholar
Bloom, L. & Lahey, M. (1978). Language Development and Language Disorders. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Bowen, C. (2015). Children’s Speech Sound Disorders. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brice, A. E., Carson, C. K., & O’Brien, J. (2009). Spanish-English articulation and phonology of 4- and 5-year-old preschool children: An initial investigation. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 31(1), 314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A First Language: The Early Stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burrows, L. & Goldstein, B. A. (2010). Whole-word measures in bilingual children with speech sound disorders. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 24, 357368.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic Structures. Boston, MA: De Gruyter Mouton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Combiths, P., Barlow, J. A., & Sanchez, E. (2019). Quantifying phonological knowledge in children with phonological disorder. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 33(10–11), 885898.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Combiths, P., Escobedo, A., Barlow, J. A., & Pruitt-Lord, S. (2022). Complexity and cross-linguistic transfer in intervention for Spanish-English bilingual children with speech sound disorder. Journal of Monolingual and Bilingual Speech, 4(3), 234270.Google ScholarPubMed
Corinne, J., Williams, L. A., & McLeod, S. (2012). Speech-language pathologists’ assessment and intervention practices with multilingual children. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 14(3), 292305.Google Scholar
De Lamo White, C. & Jin, L. (2011). Evaluation of speech and language assessment approaches with bilingual children. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 46, 613627.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dinnsen, D. A. & Chin, S. B. (1993). Individual differences in phonological disorders and implications for a theory of acquisition. In Eckman, F. R., ed., Confluence: Linguistics, L2 Acquisition, and Speech Pathology. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 137152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dinnsen, D. A., Chin, S. B., Elbert, M., & Powel, T. W. (1990). Some constraints on functionally-disordered phonologies: Phonetic inventories and phonotactics. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 33, 2837.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dodd, B. (2014). Differential diagnosis of pediatric speech sound disorder. Current Developmental Disorders Reports, 1, 189196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dodd, B., Holm, A., & Wei, L. (1997). Speech disorder in preschool children exposed to Cantonese and English. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 11(3), 229243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eckman, F. R., ed. (1993). Confluence: Linguistics, L2 Acquisition, and Speech Pathology. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edwards, M. L. & Shriberg, L. D. (1983). Phonology: Applications in Communicative Disorders. Providence, RI: College-Hill Press.Google Scholar
Elbert, M. & Gierut, J. A. (1986). Handbook of Clinical Phonology: Approaches to Assessment and Treatment. Providence, RI: College-Hill Press.Google Scholar
Elleseff, T. (2014). Russian Articulation Screener [Informal Assessment Instrument]. Smart Speech Therapy. www.smartspeechtherapy.com/shop/russian-articulation-screener/.Google Scholar
Fabiano-Smith, L. (2007). Evidence-based phonological assessment of bilingual children. Perspectives on Communication Disorders and Sciences in Culturally and Linguistically Diverse (CLD) Populations, 14(2), 2123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernandes, B., Kester, E., Bauman, M., & Prath, S. (2012). Bilingual Articulation Phonology Assessment: English/Spanish. Rowlett, TX: Smarty Ears.Google Scholar
Flipsen, P. (2015). Severity and SSD: A continuing puzzle. In Bowen, C., ed., Children’s Speech Sound Disorders. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Blackwell, pp. 94100.Google Scholar
Geronikou, E. & Babatsouli, E. (2024). Child speech developmental norms in Greek monolinguals: Whole word and consonant accuracy. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699206.2024.2329975. Epub ahead of print. PMID: 38560915.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gierut, J. (1998). Treatment efficacy: Functional phonological disorders in children. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 32, 229241.Google Scholar
Gildersleeve-Neumann, C. & Goldstein, B. A. (2015). Crosslinguistic generalization in the treatment of two sequential Spanish-English bilingual children with SSDs. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 17(1), 2640.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gildersleeve-Neumann, C. & Goldstein, B. A. (2022). Speech sound development and disorders. In Goldstein, B. A., ed., Bilingual Language Development and Disorders in Spanish-English Speakers, 3rd ed. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes, pp. 307347.Google Scholar
Glaspey, A. M., Wilson, J. J., Reeder, J. D., Tseng, W. C., & MacLeod, A. N. (2022). Moving beyond single word acquisition of speech sounds to connected speech development with dynamic assessment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 65(2), 508524.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glaspey, A. & Stoel-Gammon, C. (2007). A dynamic approach to phonological assessment. Advances in Speech Language Pathology, 9(4), 286296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldman, R. & Fristoe, M. (2000). Goldman Fristoe Test of Articulation 2. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service (AGS).Google Scholar
Goldstein, B. A. (2000). Cultural and Linguistic Diversity Resource Guide for Speech-Language Pathologists. Norwich: Singular.Google Scholar
Goldstein, B. A., ed. (2022). Bilingual Language Development and Disorders in Spanish-English Speakers, 3rd ed. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
Goldstein, B. A. & Gildersleeve-Neumann, C. E. (2015). Bilingualism and speech sound disorders. Current Developmental Disorders Reports, 2, 237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldstein, B. & Kohnert, K. (2005). Speech, language, and hearing in developing bilingual children: Current findings and future directions. Language Speech and Hearing Sciences in Schools, 36(3), 264267.Google ScholarPubMed
Goldstein, B. A. & McLeod, S. (2012). Typical and atypical multilingual speech acquisition. In McLeod, S. & Goldstein, B. A., eds., Multilingual Aspects of Speech Sound Disorders in Children. Bristol, UK: Multilingual Matters, pp. 84100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gorman, B. K. (2015). Dynamic assessment with bilinguals: A focus on increasing clinicians’ confidence. Perspectives on Communication Disorders and Sciences in Culturally and Linguistically Diverse Populations, 22, 112121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grech, H. & Dodd, B. (2007). Assessment of speech and language skills in bilingual children: An holistic approach. Stem-, Spraak- en Taalpathologie, 15(2), 8492.Google Scholar
Grech, H., Dodd, B., & Franklin, S. (2015). The development and standardisation of the bilingual Maltese-English Speech Assessment (MESA). In Babatsouli, E. & Ingram, D., eds., Proceedings of the International Symposium on Monolingual and Bilingual Speech. Chania, Greece: ISMBS, pp. 7585.Google Scholar
Grech, H., Dodd, B., & Franklin, S. (2017). Bilingual speech assessment for Maltese children. In Babatsouli, E, Ingram, D., & Müller, N., eds., Crosslinguistic Encounters in Language Acquisition. Bristol, UK: Multilingual Matters, pp. 109210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grosjean, F. (2010). Bilingual: Life and Reality. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grunwell, P. (1981). The development of phonology: A descriptive profile. First Language, 2(6), 161191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guiberson, M. (2013). Bilingual myth-busters series language confusion in bilingual children. Perspectives on Communication Disorders and Sciences in Culturally and Linguistically Diverse (CLD) Populations, 20(1), 514.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hambly, H., Wren, Y., McLeod, S., & Roulstone, S. (2013). The influence of bilingualism on speech production: A systematic review. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 48(1), 124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hasson, N., Camilleri, B., Jones, C., Smith, J., & Dodd, B. (2013). Discriminating disorder from difference using dynamic assessment with bilingual children. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 29, 5775.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hemsley, G., Holm, A., & Dodd, B. (2014). Identifying language difference versus disorder in bilingual children. Speech, Language and Hearing, 17(2), 101115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hodson, B. W. (1980). The Assessment of Phonological Processes. Sedalia, MO: Interstate.Google Scholar
Holm, A. & Dodd, B. (1999). Differential diagnosis of phonological disorder in two bilingual children acquiring Italian and English. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 13(2), 113129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holm, A. & Dodd, B. (2001). Comparison of cross-language generalisation following speech therapy. Folia Phoniatrica et Logopaedica, 53(3), 166172.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holm, A., Dodd, B., & Ozanne, A. (1997). Case report: Efficacy of intervention for a bilingual child making articulation and phonological errors. International Journal of Bilingualism, 1, 5569.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hua, Z. & Dodd, B. (2006). A multilingual perspective on phonological development and disorders. In Hua, Z. & Dodd, B., eds., Phonological Development and Disorders in Children: A Multilingual Perspective. Bristol, UK: Multilingual Matters, pp. 113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hyter, Y. D. & Salas-Provance, M. B. (2019). Culturally Responsive Practices in Speech Language and Hearing Sciences. San Diego, CA: Plural.Google Scholar
Ingram, D. (1976). Phonological Disability in Children. London: Edward Arnold.Google Scholar
Ingram, D. (2002). The measurement of whole-word productions. Journal of Child Language, 29, 713733.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ingram, D. (2015). The role of theory in speech sound disorders. In Bowen, C., ed., Children’s Speech Sound Disorders. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Blackwell, pp. 100104.Google Scholar
Ingram, D. & Babatsouli, E. (2024). Cross-linguistic phonological acquisition. In Ball, M. J., Müller, N., & Spencer, L., eds., The Handbook of Clinical Linguistics, 2nd ed. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Blackwell, pp. 407420.Google Scholar
Ingram, D. & Dubasik, V. L. (2011). Multidimensional assessment of phonological similarity within and between children. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 25(12), 962967.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ingram, D., Williams, L. A., & Scherer, N. (2018). Are speech sound disorders phonological or articulatory? A spectrum approach. In Babatsouli, E. & Ingram, D., eds., Phonology in Protolanguage and Interlanguage. Sheffield: Equinox, pp. 2748.Google Scholar
Jakobson, R. ([1941] 1968). Child Language, Phonological Universals and Aphasia. Trans. A. R. Keiler. The Hague: Mouton. (Original work published in 1941 as Kindersprache, aphasie und allgemeine Lautgesetze.)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jordaan, H. (2008). Clinical intervention for bilingual children: An international survey. Folia Phoniatrica et Logopaedica, 60(2), 97105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kamhi, A. G. & Pollock, K. E., eds. (2005). Phonological Disorders in Children: Clinical Decision Making and Intervention. Baltimore, MD: Paul. H. Brookes.Google Scholar
Kehoe, M., Friend, M., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2021). Relations between phonological production, grammar and the lexicon in bilingual French-English children. International Journal of Bilingualism, 25(6), 15761596.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kehoe, M. & Matrat, M. (2024). Dynamic assessment in phonology: A review and its application to French monolingual and bilingual children. In Babatsouli, E., ed., Multilingual Acquisition and Learning: An Ecosystemic View to Diversity. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 295323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kehoe, M., Niederberger, N., & Bouchut, A. (2021). The development of a speech sound screening test for European French-speaking monolingual and bilingual children. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 23(2), 135144.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kester, E. S. (2014). Difference or Disorder? Understanding Speech and Language Patterns in Culturally and Linguistically Diverse Students. Austin, TX: Bilinguistics.Google ScholarPubMed
Kohnert, K. (2007). Evidence-based practice and treatment of speech sound disorders in bilingual children. Perspectives on Language Learning and Education, 14(2), 1821.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kohnert, K., Ebert, K., & Pham, G. (2021). Language Disorders in Bilingual Children and Adults, 3rd ed. San Diego, CA: Plural.Google Scholar
Kohnert, K. & Medina, A. (2009). Bilingual children and communication disorders: A 30-year research retrospective. Seminars in Speech and Language, 30(4), 219233.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kwiatkowski, J. & Shriberg, L. D. (1992). Intelligibility assessment in developmental phonological disorders: Accuracy of caregiver gloss. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 35(5), 10951104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lammer, V. & Kalmár, M. (2004). Wiener Lautprüfverfahren für Türkisch sprechende Kinder (WIELAU-T). [German] Vienna: Lernen mit Pfiff.Google Scholar
Macleod, A. N. & Pollock, K. E. (2020). Global perspectives in child phonology. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 22(6), 611613.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mamdouh, H. (2008). Management of delayed language affecting phonology in a bilingual child. El Minia Medical Bulletin, 19(1), 3845.Google Scholar
Marian, V. & Hayakawa, S. (2020). Measuring bilingualism: The quest for a “bilingualism quotient.Applied Psycholinguistics, 42, 527548.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLeod, S. (2022). Multilingual Speech Assessments. Bathurst, NSW: Charles Sturt University. www.csu.edu.au/research/multilingual-speech/speech-assessments.Google Scholar
McLeod, S. & Baker, E. (2017). Children’s Speech: An Evidence-Based Approach to Assessment and Intervention. London: Pearson.Google Scholar
McLeod, S. & Crowe, K. (2018). Children’s consonant acquisition in 27 languages: A cross-linguistic review. American Journal of Speech Language Pathology, 27(4), 15461571.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLeod, S. & Goldstein, B., eds. (2012). Multilingual Aspects of Speech Sound Disorders in Children. Bristol, UK: Multilingual Matters.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McLeod, S., Harrison, L. J., & McCormack, J. (2012). Intelligibility in Context Scale. Bathurst, NSW: Charles Sturt University. www.csu.edu.au/research/multilingual-speech/ics.Google ScholarPubMed
McLeod, S., Verdon, S., Bowen, C., & International Expert Panel on Multilingual Children’s Speech. (2013). International aspirations for speech-language pathologists’ practice with multilingual children with speech sound disorders: Development of a position paper. Journal of Communication Disorders, 46(4), 375387.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meziane, R. S., Bérubé, D., & MacLeod, A. A. (2024). Towards an ecosystemic view of bilingual phonological development. In Babatsouli, E., ed., Multilingual Acquisition and Learning: An Ecosystemic View to Diversity. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 246271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mohanan, K. P. (1992). Emergence of complexity in phonological development. In Ferguson, C. A., Menn, L., & Stoel-Gammon, C., eds., Phonological Development: Models, Research, Implications. Timonium, MD: York Press, pp. 635662.Google Scholar
Nice, M. (1925). Length of sentence as a criterion of child progress in speech. Journal of Educational Psychology, 16, 370379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noveen, S., Masood, T., & Habibullah, S. (2017). Establishment of validity of a test for articulation and phonological disorders among bilingual children in Punjab and Baluchistan. International Journal of Rehabilitation Sciences, 6(2), 3742.Google Scholar
Nye, C. D. (2019). Cross-language treatment of speech sounds disorders in bilingual children. [Doctoral dissertation, University of Texas at Austin].Google Scholar
Oetting, J. B. (2018). Prologue: Toward accurate identification of developmental language disorder within linguistically diverse schools. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 49(2), 213217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oetting, J. B. (2024). Approaching developmental language disorder from a Disorder within Dialects framework: A focus on dialect-informed terms, materials, and strategic scoring. In Babatsouli, E., ed., Multilingual Acquisition and Learning: An Ecosystemic View to Diversity. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 116142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J. (2017). Parent report data on input and experience reliably predict bilingual development and this is not trivial. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21(1), 2728.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J., Genesee, F., & Crago, M. B. (2021). Dual Language Development and Disorders: A Handbook on Bilingualism and Second Language Learning, 3rd ed. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
Parker, M. D. & Brorson, K. (2005). A comparative study between mean length of utterance in morphemes (MLUm) and mean length of utterance in words (MLUw). First Language, 25, 365376.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pascoe, M. & Jeggo, Z. (2019). Speech acquisition in monolingual children acquiring isiZulu in rural KwaZulu-Natal, South Africa. Journal of Monolingual and Bilingual Speech, 1(1), 94117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pascoe, M., Rossouw, K., & Mahura, O. (2018). Core vocabulary intervention for an isiXhosa-English speaking child with speech sound difficulties. Southern African Linguistics and Applied Language Studies, 36(4), 313328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peña, E. D., Gutiérrez-Clellen, V. F., Iglesias, A., Goldstein, B. A., & Bedore, L. M. (2014). BESA: Bilingual English-Spanish Assessment. San Rafael, CA: AR-Clinical Publications.Google Scholar
Potapova, I., Combiths, P., Pruitt-Lord, S., & Barlow, S. (2023). Word-final complexity in speech sound intervention: Two case studies. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 37(4–6), 363384.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Potter, S. (1882). Speech and Its Defects. Philadelphia, PA: Blakiston Son & Co.Google Scholar
Prater, R. J. & Swift, R. W. (1982). Phonological process development with MLU-referenced guidelines. Journal of Communication Disorders, 15(5), 395410.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prath, S. (2016). Red flags for speech-language impairment in bilingual children. ASHA Leader. https://doi.org/10.1044/leader.SCM.21112016.32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Preston, J. L. & Seki, A. (2011). Identifying residual speech sound disorders in bilingual children: A Japanese-English case study. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 20(2), 7385.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramos, E. & Mead, J. (2014). Dual language intervention in a case of severe speech sound disorder. Revista de Investigación en Logopedia, 4(2), 93111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ray, J. (2002). Treating phonological disorders in a multilingual child: A case study. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 11, 305315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rice, K. & Avery, P. (1995). Variability in a deterministic model of language acquisition: A theory of segmental elaboration. In Archibald, J. A., ed., Phonological Acquisition and Phonological Theory. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 2342.Google Scholar
Rose, Y. (2022). Phon functions for the study of phonology and phonetics in multilingual speakers. In Babatsouli, E., ed., Book of Abstracts of the International Symposium on Monolingual and Bilingual Speech (ISMBS) 2022. Lafayette: University of Louisiana at Lafayette.Google Scholar
Rose, Y. (2024). Multilingual data coding and analysis with Phon: A practical demonstration. In Babatsouli, E., ed., Multilingual Acquisition and Learning: An Ecosystemic View to Diversity. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 169193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rossouw, K. & Pascoe, M. (2018). Intervention for bilingual speech sound disorders: A case study of an isiXhosa-English-speaking child. South African Journal of Communication Disorders, 65(1), https://doi.org/10.4102/sajcd.v65i1.566.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rvachew, S. & Brosseau-Lapré, F. (2012). Developmental Phonological Disorders: Foundations of Clinical Practice. Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Santhanam, S. P. & Parveen, S. (2018). Serving culturally and linguistically diverse clients: A review of changing trends in speech-language pathologists’ self-efficacy and implications for stakeholders. Clinical Archives of Communication Disorders, 3(3), 165177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schleif, E. P., Mason, K., & Perry, J. L. (2021). English-only treatment of compensatory speech errors in a bilingual adoptee with repaired cleft palate: A descriptive case study. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 30(3), 9931007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Selinker, L., Swain, M., & Dumas, G. (1975). The interlanguage hypothesis extended to children. Language Learning, 25(1), 139152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shriberg, L. D. (1982). Towards a classification of developmental phonological disorders. In Lass, N., ed., Speech and Language: Advances in Basic Research and Practice. New York: Academic Press, pp. 117.Google Scholar
Shriberg, L. D., Lewis, B. A., & McSweeny, J. L. (1997a). The percentage of consonants correct (PCC) metric: Extensions and reliability data. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40(4), 708722.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shriberg, L. D., Austin, D., Lewis, B. A., McSweeny, J. L., & Wilson, D. L. (1997b). The speech disorders classification system (SDCS): Extensions and lifespan reference data. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40(4), 723740.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shriberg, L. D., Potter, N. L., & Strand, E. A. (2011). Prevalence and phenotype of childhood apraxia of speech in youth with galactosemia. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 54(2), 487519.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sim, J. H. & Post, B. (2024). Early phonological acquisition in multi-accent contexts. In Babatsouli, E., ed., Multilingual Acquisition and Learning: An Ecosystemic View to Diversity. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 194216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smit, A. B., Hand, L., Freilinger, J. J., Bernthal, J. E., & Bird, A. (1990). The Iowa articulation norms project and its Nebraska replication. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 55, 779798.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, N. V. (1973). The Acquisition of Phonology: A Case Study. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
So, L. K. H. & Dodd, B. J. (1995). The acquisition of phonology by Cantonese-speaking children. Journal of Child Language, 22(3), 473–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stemberger, J. P. & Bernhardt, B. M. (2022). Individual profiles in protracted phonological development across languages: Introduction to the special issue. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 36(7), 597616.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stoel-Gammon, C. (2010). The Word Complexity Measure: Description and application to developmental phonology and disorders. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 24(4–5), 271282.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stoel-Gammon, C. & Dunn, C. (1985). Normal and Disordered Phonology in Children. Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Stow, C. & Pert, S. (1998). Rochdale Assessment of Mirpuri Phonology with Punjabi, Urdu and English: A Speech and Language Therapy Resource for the Phonological Assessment of Bilingual Children. Rochdale, UK: Pert.Google Scholar
Stow, C. & Pert, S. (2006). BiSSS: Bilingual Speech Sound Screen: Pakistani Heritage Languages. Milton Keynes: Speechmark.Google Scholar
Stow, C. & Pert, S. (2015). SLT assessment and intervention: Best practice for CYP in bilingual settings – and in particular Gaelic medium education (GME). https://bit.ly/3ZMZusk.Google Scholar
Taylor, O., ed. (1986). Nature of Communication Disorders in Culturally and Linguistically Diverse Populations. Providence, RI: College-Hill Press.Google Scholar
Templin, M. (1957). Certain Language Skills in Children: Their Development and Interrelationships. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tzivinikou, S. (2005). Identification and differential diagnosis of developmental speech problems in a bilingual child. Early Child Development and Care, 175, 165178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Borsel, J. & D’haeseleer, L. (2019). The process density index as a measure of phonological development: Data from Dutch. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 40(4), 220227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vasanta, D. & Dodd, B. (2007). Perceptual factors in phonological disorders: A tool for assessing input phonological processing in Telugu-English bilinguals. Osmania Papers in Linguistics, 33, 5572.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in Society: The Development of Higher Psychological Processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Wagner, L. (2008). Screemik Version 2. Screening der Erstsprachefähigkeit bei Migrantenkindern: Russisch-Deutsch. Türkisch-Deutsch [German]. Munich: Eugen Wagner.Google Scholar
Wagner, L. (2009). Screemik Version 2. Screening der Erstsprachefähigkeit bei Migrantenkindern. [German]. Munich: Eugen Wagner.Google Scholar
Watson, I. (1991). Phonological processing in two languages. In Bialystok, E., ed., Language Processing in Bilingual Children. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 2548.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, A. L. (2015). Assessment and intervention from a systemic perspective. In Bowen, C., ed., Children’s Speech Sound Disorders, 2nd ed. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell, pp. 199203.Google Scholar
Williams, C. J. & McLeod, S. (2012). Speech-language pathologists’ assessment and intervention practices with multilingual children. International Journal of Speech–Language Pathology, 14(3), 292305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
World Health Organization. (2001). ICF: International Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health. Geneva: WHO. www.who.int/standards/classifications/international-classification-of-functioning-disability-and-health.Google Scholar
Yavaş, M. (1991). Phonological Disorders in Children: Theory, Research and Practice. Abingdon: Routledge.Google Scholar
Yavaş, M. & Goldstein, B. (1998). Phonological assessment and treatment of bilingual speakers. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 7, 4960.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zydorowicz, P. (2022). Word structure produced, but clusters reduced, coronals developing: A Polish child with protracted phonological development. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 36(9), 751776.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

References

Almeida, L., Rose, Y., & Freitas, J. (2012). Prosodic influence in bilingual phonological development: Evidence from a Portuguese-French first language learner. In Proceedings of the 36th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press, pp. 4252.Google Scholar
Amengual, M. (2016). The perception of language-specific phonetic categories does not guarantee accurate phonological representations in the lexicon of early bilinguals. Applied Psycholinguistics, 37(5), 12211251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Babatsouli, E. & Ingram, D. (2015). What bilingualism tells us about phonological acquisition. In Bahr, R. H. & Silliman, E. R., eds., Routledge Handbook of Communication Disorders. New York: Routledge, pp. 197206.Google Scholar
Bosch, L. (2018). Language proximity and speech perception in young bilinguals. In Gibson, M. & Gil, J., eds., Romance Phonetics and Phonology. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 353366.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bosch, L. & Ramon-Casas, M. (2011). Variability in vowel production by bilingual speakers: Can input properties hinder the early stabilization of contrastive categories? Journal of Phonetics, 39(4), 514526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bosch, L. & Ramon-Casas, M. (2014). First translation equivalents in bilingual toddlers’ expressive vocabulary: Does form similarity matter? International Journal of Behavioral Development, 38(4), 317322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bosch, L. & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (1997). Native-language recognition abilities in 4-month-old infants from monolingual and bilingual environments. Cognition, 65(1), 3369.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bosch, L. & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2001). Evidence of early language discrimination abilities in infants from bilingual environments. Infancy, 2(1), 2949.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooperson, S. J., Bedore, L. M., & Peña, E. D. (2013). The relationship of phonological skills to language skills in Spanish-English-speaking bilingual children. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 27(5), 371389.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Curtin, S., Byers-Heinlein, K., & Werker, J. F. (2011). Bilingual beginnings as a lens for theory development: PRIMIR in focus. Journal of Phonetics, 39(4), 492504.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, B. L. & Bedore, L. M. (2013). An Emergence Approach to Speech Acquisition: Doing and Knowing. New York: Psychology Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
deAnda, S., Bosch, L., Poulin-Dubois, D., Zesiger, P., & Friend, M. (2016). The language exposure assessment tool: Quantifying language exposure in infants and children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 59(6), 13461356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Boysson-Bardies, B. & Vihman, M. M. (1991). Adaptation to language: Evidence from babbling and first words in four languages. Language, 67(2), 297319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dodd, B. & McIntosh, B. (2010). Two-year-old phonology: Impact of input, motor and cognitive abilities on development. Journal of Child Language, 37(5), 10271046.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fabiano-Smith, L. & Barlow, J. A. (2010). Interaction in bilingual phonological acquisition: Evidence from phonetic inventories. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 13(1), 8197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fabiano-Smith, L. & Bunta, F. (2012). Voice onset time of voiceless bilabial and velar stops in 3-year-old bilingual children and their age-matched monolingual peers. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 26(2), 148163.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fabiano-Smith, L. & Goldstein, B. A. (2010). Phonological acquisition in bilingual Spanish-English speaking children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53(1), 160178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fennell, C. T., Byers‐Heinlein, K., & Werker, J. F. (2007). Using speech sounds to guide word learning: The case of bilingual infants. Child Development, 78(5), 15101525.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Floccia, C., Sambrook, T. D., Delle Luche, C., et al. (2018). Vocabulary of 2-year-olds learning English and an additional language: Norms and effects of linguistic distance. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 83(1), 729.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gildersleeve-Neumann, C. E., Kester, E. S., Davis, B. L., & Peña, E. D. (2008). English speech sound development in preschool-aged children from bilingual English–Spanish environments. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 39(3), 314328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldstein, B. A. & Bunta, F. (2012). Positive and negative transfer in the phonological systems of bilingual speakers. International Journal of Bilingualism, 16(4), 388401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grech, H. & McLeod, S. (2012). Multilingual speech and language development and disorders. In Battle, D. E., ed., Communication Disorders in Multicultural and International Populations. St. Louis, MO: Elsevier, pp. 120140.Google Scholar
Grosjean, F. (2021). Life as a Bilingual: Knowing and Using Two or More Languages. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hambly, H., Wren, Y., McLeod, S., & Roulstone, S. (2013). The influence of bilingualism on speech production: A systematic review. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 48(1), 124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ingram, D. (1981). The emerging phonological system of an Italian-English bilingual child. Journal of Italian Linguistics Amsterdam, 6(2), 95113.Google Scholar
Ingram, D. (2002). The measurement of whole-word productions. Journal of Child Language, 29(4), 713733.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kail, M. (2015). L’acquisition de plusieurs langues. Paris : Presses universitaires de France.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keffala, B., Barlow, J. A., & Rose, S. (2018). Interaction in Spanish-English bilinguals’ acquisition of syllable structure. International Journal of Bilingualism, 22(1), 1637.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keffala, B., Scarpino, S., Hammer, C. S., et al. (2020). Vocabulary and phonological abilities affect dual language learners’ consonant production accuracy within and across languages: A large-scale study of 3-to 6-year-old Spanish-English dual language learners. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 29(3), 11961211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kehoe, M. (2002). Developing vowel systems as a window to bilingual phonology. International Journal of Bilingualism, 6(3), 315334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kehoe, M. (2011). Relationships between lexical and phonological development: A look at bilingual children – A commentary on Stoel-Gammon’s “Relationships between lexical and phonological development in young children.” Journal of Child Language, 38(1), 7581.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kehoe, M. (2015). Lexical-phonological interactions in bilingual children. First Language, 35(2), 93125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kehoe, M., Friend, M., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2021). Relations between phonological production, grammar and the lexicon in bilingual French-English children. International Journal of Bilingualism, 25(6), 15761596.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kehoe, M. & Girardier, C. (2020). What factors influence phonological production in French-speaking bilingual children, aged three to six years? Journal of Child Language, 47(5), 945981.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kehoe, M. & Havy, M. (2019). Bilingual phonological acquisition: The influence of language-internal, language-external, and lexical factors. Journal of Child Language, 46(2), 292333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kehoe, M., Lleó, C., & Rakow, M. (2004). Voice onset time in bilingual German-Spanish children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 7(1), 7188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keshavarz, M. H. & Ingram, D. (2002). The early phonological development of a Farsi-English bilingual child. International Journal of Bilingualism, 6(3), 255269.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kremin, L. V., Alves, J., Orena, A. J., Polka, L., & Byers-Heinlein, K. (2022). Code-switching in parents’ everyday speech to bilingual infants. Journal of Child Language, 49(4), 714740.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhl, P. K., Conboy, B. T., Coffey-Corina, S., et al. (2008). Phonetic learning as a pathway to language: New data and native language magnet theory expanded (NLM-e). Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 363, 9791000.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhl, P. K., Ramírez, R. R., Bosseler, A., Lin, J. F. L., & Imada, T. (2014). Infants’ brain responses to speech suggest analysis by synthesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(31), 1123811245.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kupisch, T., Kolb, N., Rodina, Y., & Urek, O. (2021). Foreign accent in pre-and primary school heritage bilinguals. Languages, 6(2), 96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Law, N. C. & So, L. K. (2006). The relationship of phonological development and language dominance in bilingual Cantonese-Putonghua children. International Journal of Bilingualism, 10(4), 405427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewkowicz, D. J. & Hansen-Tift, A. M. (2012). Infants deploy selective attention to the mouth of a talking face when learning speech. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(5), 14311436.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lleó, C. (2002). The role of markedness in the acquisition of complex prosodic structures by German-Spanish bilinguals. International Journal of Bilingualism, 6(3), 291313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lleó, C. & Kehoe, M. (2002). On the interaction of phonological systems in child bilingual acquisition. International Journal of Bilingualism, 6(3), 233237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lleó, C., Kuchenbrandt, I., Kehoe, M., & Trujillo, C. (2003). Syllable final consonants in Spanish and German monolingual and bilingual acquisition. In Müller, N., ed., (In)vulnerable Domains in Multilingualism. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 191220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayr, R., Howells, G., & Lewis, R. (2015). Asymmetries in phonological development: The case of word-final cluster acquisition in Welsh–English bilingual children. Journal of Child Language, 42(1), 146179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meisel, J. M. (2020). Shrinking structures in heritage languages: Triggered by reduced quantity of input? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 23(1), 3334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Menke, M. R. (2018). Development of Spanish rhotics in Spanish-English bilingual children in the United States. Journal of Child Language, 45(3), 788806.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meziane, R. S. & MacLeod, A. A. (2021). Evidence of phonological transfer in bilingual preschoolers who speak Arabic and French. International Journal of Bilingualism, 25(6), 16801695.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitra, A. & Dutta, I. (2023). Mixed language processing increases cross-language phonetic transfer in Bengali–English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 26(5), 896909.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Molnar, M., Gervain, J., & Carreiras, M. (2014). Within‐rhythm class native language discrimination abilities of Basque‐Spanish monolingual and bilingual infants at 3.5 months of age. Infancy, 19(3), 326337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mora, J. C. & Nadeu, M. (2012). L2 effects on the perception and production of a native vowel contrast in early bilinguals. International Journal of Bilingualism, 16(4), 484500.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morrow, A., Goldstein, B. A., Gilhool, A., & Paradis, J. (2014). Phonological skills in English language learners. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 45(1), 2639.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paradis, J. (2001). Do bilingual two-year-olds have separate phonological systems? International Journal of Bilingualism, 5(1), 1938.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J. & Genesee, F. (1996). Syntactic acquisition in bilingual children: Autonomous or interdependent? Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 18(1), 125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pons, F., Bosch, L., & Lewkowicz, D. J. (2015). Bilingualism modulates infants’ selective attention to the mouth of a talking face. Psychological Science, 26(4), 490498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ramon-Casas, M., Cortes, S., Benet, A., Lleó, C., & Bosch, L. (2021). Connecting perception and production in early Catalan–Spanish bilingual children: Language dominance and quality of input effects. Journal of Child Language, 50(1), 155176.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramon-Casas, M., Swingley, D., Sebastián-Gallés, N., & Bosch, L. (2009). Vowel categorization during word recognition in bilingual toddlers. Cognitive Psychology, 59(1), 96121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scarpino, S. E. (2011). The effects of language environment and oral language ability on phonological production proficiency in bilingual Spanish-English speaking children. [Doctoral dissertation, Pennsylvania State University].Google Scholar
Schnitzer, M. L. & Krasinski, E. (1994). The development of segmental phonological production in a bilingual child. Journal of Child Language, 21(3), 585622.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schnitzer, M. L. & Krasinski, E. (1996). The development of segmental phonological production in a bilingual child: A contrasting second case. Journal of Child Language, 23(3), 547571.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shriberg, L. D., Austin, D., Lewis, B. A., McSweeny, J. L., & Wilson, D. L. (1997). The percentage of consonants correct (PCC) metric: Extensions and reliability data. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40(4), 708722.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stoehr, A., Benders, T., Van Hell, J. G., & Fikkert, P. (2019). Bilingual preschoolers’ speech is associated with non-native maternal language input. Language Learning and Development, 15(1), 75100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stoel-Gammon, C. (2011). Relationships between lexical and phonological development in young children. Journal of Child Language, 38(1), 134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sundara, M., Ward, N., Conboy, B., & Kuhl, P. K. (2020). Exposure to a second language in infancy alters speech production. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 23(5), 978991.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tamburelli, M., Sanoudaki, E., Jones, G., & Sowinska, M. (2015). Acceleration in the bilingual acquisition of phonological structure: Evidence from Polish–English bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(4), 713725.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vihman, M. M. (2014). Phonological Development: The First Two Years, 2nd ed. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weisleder, A. & Fernald, A. (2013). Talking to children matters: Early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychological Science, 24(11), 21432152.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Werker, J. F. & Byers-Heinlein, K. (2008). Bilingualism in infancy: First steps in perception and comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(4), 144151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Werker, J. F. & Curtin, S. (2005). PRIMIR: A developmental framework of infant speech processing. Language Learning and Development, 1(2), 197234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

References

Abboub, N., Bijeljac-Babic, R., Serres, J., & Nazzi, T. (2015). On the importance of being bilingual: Word stress processing in a context of segmental variability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 132, 111120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allen, G. & Hawkins, S. (1980). Phonological rhythm: Definition and development. In Yeni-Komshian, G., Kavanagh, J., & Ferguson, C., eds., Child Phonology: Production. New York: Academic Press, pp. 227256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arias, J. & Lleó, C. (2009). Comparing the representation of iambs by monolingual German, monolingual Spanish and bilingual German-Spanish children. In Braunmüller, K. & House, J., eds., Convergence and Divergence in Language Contact Situations. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 205234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Astruc, L., Payne, E., Post, B., Vanrell, M., & Prieto, P. (2013). Tonal targets in early child English, Spanish, and Catalan. Language and Speech, 56(2), 229253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bhatara, A., Boll-Avetisyan, N., Höhle, B., & Nazzi, T. (2018). Early sensitivity and acquisition of prosodic patterns at the lexical level. In Prieto, P. & Esteve-Gibert, N., eds., The Development of Prosody in First Language Acquisition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 3757.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bijeljac-Babic, R., Serres, J., Höhle, B., & Nazzi, T. (2012). Effect of bilingualism on lexical stress pattern discrimination in French-learning infants. PLoS ONE, 7(2), e30843.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bosch, L. & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (1997). Native-language recognition abilities in 4-month-old infants from monolingual and bilingual environments. Cognition, 65(1), 3369.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bunta, F. & Ingram, D. (2007). The acquisition of speech rhythm by bilingual Spanish- and English-speaking 4- and 5-year-old children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50(4), 9991014.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byers-Heinlein, K., Burns, T. C., & Werker, J. F. (2010). The roots of bilingualism in newborns. Psychological Science, 21(3), 343348.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, Ao & Kager, R. (2016). Discrimination of lexical tones in the first year of life. Infant and Child Development, 25(5), 426439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, Aoju, Esteve-Gibert, N., Prieto, P., & Redford, M. A. (2020). Development of phrasal prosody from infancy to late childhood. In Gussenhoven, C. & Chen, A., eds., The Oxford Handbook of Language Prosody. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 553562.Google Scholar
Chen, Aoju, & Fikkert, P. (2007). Intonation of early two-word utterances in Dutch. In Trouvain, J. & Barry, W. J., eds., Proceedings of the 16th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (ICPhS 2007). Dudweiler: Pirrot, pp. 315320.Google Scholar
Chong, A. J., Vicenik, C., & Sundara, M. (2018). Intonation plays a role in language discrimination by infants. Infancy, 23(6), 795819.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christophe, A. & Morton, J. (1998). Is Dutch native English? Linguistic analysis by 2-month-olds. Developmental Science, 1(2), 215219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Carvalho, A., Dautriche, I., Millotte, S., & Christophe, A. (2018). Early perception of phrasal prosody and its role in syntactic and lexical acquisition. In Prieto, P. & Esteve-Gibert, N., eds., The Development of Prosody in First Language Acquisition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 1735.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Delattre, P. (1969). An acoustic and articulatory study of vowel reduction in four languages. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 7, 295325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dodane, C. & Bijeljac-Babic, R. (2017). Cross-language influences in the productions of French-English bilingual children: Separation or interaction? In Yavas, M., Kehoe, M., & Cardoso, W., eds., Romance-Germanic Bilingual Phonology. Sheffield: Equinox, pp. 3855.Google Scholar
Fikkert, P. (1994). On the acquisition of prosodic structure. [Doctoral dissertation, University of Leiden].Google Scholar
Fikkert, P., Liu, L., & Ota, M. (2021). The acquisition of word prosody. In Gussenhoven, C. & Chen, A., eds., The Oxford Handbook of Language Prosody. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 541552.Google Scholar
Frota, S. & Butler, J. (2018). Early development of intonation. In Prieto, P. & Esteve-Gibert, N., eds., The Development of Prosody in First Language Acquisition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 145164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frota, S., Matos, N., Cruz, M., & Vigário, M. (2016). Early prosodic development: Emerging intonation and phrasing in European Portuguese. In Armstrong, M., Henriksen, N. C., & Vanrell, M., eds., Intonational Grammar in Ibero-Romance: Approaches across Linguistic Subfields. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 277294.Google Scholar
Götz, A., Yeung, H. H., Krasotkina, A., Schwarzer, G., & Höhle, B. (2018). Perceptual reorganization of lexical tones: Effects of age and experimental procedure. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 477.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grabe, E., Post, B., & Watson, I. (1999). The acquisition of rhythmic patterns in French and English. In Ohala, J. J., Hasegawa, Y., Ohala, M., Granville, D., & Bailey, A. C., eds., Proceedings of the 14th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences. Berkeley: University of California, Berkeley, 12011204. www.internationalphoneticassociation.org/icphs-proceedings/ICPhS1999/papers/p14_1201.pdf.Google Scholar
Höhle, B., Bijeljac-Babic, R., Herold, B., Weissenborn, J., & Nazzi, T. (2009). Language specific prosodic preferences during the first half year of life: Evidence from German and French infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 32(3), 262274.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Höhle, B., Bijeljac-Babic, R., & Nazzi, T. (2020). Variability and stability in early language acquisition: Comparing monolingual and bilingual infants’ speech perception and word recognition. Bilingualism, 23(1), 5671.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holm, A. & Dodd, B. (2006). Phonological development and disorder of bilingual children acquiring Cantonese and English. In Zhu, H. & Dodd, B., eds., Phonological Development and Disorders in Children: A Multilingual Perspective. Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters, pp. 286325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hua, Z. & Dodd, B. (2000). The phonological acquisition of Putonghua (modern standard Chinese). Journal of Child Language, 27(1), 342.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kan, R. T. Y. & Schmid, M. S. (2019). Development of tonal discrimination in young heritage speakers of Cantonese. Journal of Phonetics, 73, 4054.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karpava, S. (2020). Lexical stress assignment and reading skills of Russian heritage children. In Proceedings of the International Conference on Speech Prosody 2020, pp. 9195. https://doi.org/10.21437/SpeechProsody.2020-19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kehoe, M. (2001). Prosodic patterns in children’s multisyllabic word productions. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 32(4), 284294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kehoe, M. (2018). Prosodic phonology in acquisition. In Prieto, P. & Esteve-Gibert, N., eds., The Development of Prosody in First Language Acquisition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 165184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kehoe, M. (2021). The prosody of two-syllable words in French-speaking monolingual and bilingual children: A focus on initial accent and final accent. Language and Speech, 65(2), 444471.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kehoe, M., Lleó, C., & Rakow, M. (2011). Speech rhythm in the pronunciation of German and Spanish monolingual and German-Spanish bilingual 3-year-olds. Linguistische Berichte, 2011(227), 323352.Google Scholar
Li, C. N. & Thompson, S. A. (1977). The acquisition of tone in Mandarin-speaking children. Journal of Child Language, 4(2), 185199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, J., & Mok, P. P. K. (2014). The acquisition of English lexical stress by Cantonese-English bilingual children at 2;06 and 3;0. In Campbell, N., Gibbon, D., & Hirst, D., eds., Proceedings of the International Conference on Speech Prosody 7, pp. 688692. https://bit.ly/3UiiOx3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, L. & Kager, R. (2017a). Perception of tones by bilingual infants learning non-tone languages. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20(3), 561575.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, L. & Kager, R. (2017b). Enhanced music sensitivity in 9-month-old bilingual infants. Cognitive Processing, 18(1), 5565.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lleó, C. (2016). A preliminary study of wh-questions in German and Spanish child language. In Armstrong, M., Henriksen, N., & Vanrell, M., eds., Intonational Grammar in Ibero-Romance: Approaches across Linguistic Subfields. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 325350.Google Scholar
Lleó, C. (2018). Bilingual children’s prosodic development. In Prieto, P. & Esteve-Gibert, N., eds., The Development of Prosody in First Language Acquisition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 317342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lleó, C. & Rakow, M. (2011). Intonation targets of yes/no questions by Spanish and German monolingual and bilingual children. In Rinke, E. & Kupisch, T., eds., The Development of Grammar: Language Acquisition and Diachronic Change. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 263286.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lleó, C., Rakow, M., & Kehoe, M. (2004). Acquisition of language-specific pitch accent by Spanish and German monolingual and bilingual children. In Face, T., ed., Laboratory Approaches to Spanish Phonology. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton, pp. 327.Google Scholar
Lleó, C., Rakow, M., & Kehoe, M. (2007). Acquiring rhythmically different languages in a bilingual context. In Trouvain, J. & Barry, W. J., eds., Proceedings of the 16th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, pp. 15451548. www.icphs2007.de/conference/Papers/1144/1144.pdf.Google Scholar
Mattock, K., & Burnham, D. (2006). Chinese and English infants’ tone perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization. Infancy, 10(3), 241265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mok, P. P. K. (2011). The acquisition of speech rhythm by three-year-old bilingual and monolingual children: Cantonese and English. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14(4), 458472.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mok, P. P. K. (2013). Speech rhythm of monolingual and bilingual children at age 2;6: Cantonese and English. Bilingualism, 16(3), 693703.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mok, P. P. K. & Lee, A. (2018). The acquisition of lexical tones by Cantonese-English bilingual children. Journal of Child Language, 45(6), 13571376.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Molnar, M., Gervain, J., & Carreiras, M. (2013). Within-rhythm class native language discrimination abilities of Basque-Spanish monolingual and bilingual infants at 3.5 months of age. Infancy, 19(3), 326337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nacar Garcia, L., Guerrero-Mosquera, C., Colomer, M., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2018). Evoked and oscillatory EEG activity differentiates language discrimination in young monolingual and bilingual infants. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 2770.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nazzi, T., Bertoncini, J., & Mehler, J. (1998). Language discrimination by newborns: Toward an understanding of the role of rhythm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(3), 756766.Google ScholarPubMed
Nazzi, T., Iakimova, G., Bertoncini, J., Frédonie, S., & Alcantara, C. (2006). Early segmentation of fluent speech by infants acquiring French: Emerging evidence for crosslinguistic differences. Journal of Memory and Language, 54(3), 283299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nazzi, T., Jusczyk, P. W., & Johnson, E. K. (2000). Language discrimination by English-learning 5-month-olds: Effects of rhythm and familiarity. Journal of Memory and Language, 43(1), 119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J. (2001). Do bilingual two-year-olds have separate phonological systems? International Journal of Bilingualism, 5(1), 1938.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Payne, E., Post, B., Astruc, L., Prieto, P., & Vanrell, M. (2012). Measuring child rhythm. Language and Speech, 55(2), 203229.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Polyanskaya, L. & Ordin, M. (2015). Acquisition of speech rhythm in first language. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 138(3), EL199–EL204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Post, B. & Payne, E. (2018). Speech rhythm in development: What is the child acquiring? In Prieto, P. & Esteve-Gibert, N., eds., The Development of Prosody in First Language Acquisition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 125144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prieto, P., & Esteve-Gibert, N., eds. (2018). The Development of Prosody in First Language Acquisition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prieto, P., Estrella, A., Thorson, J., & Vanrell, M. (2012). Is prosodic development correlated with grammatical and lexical development? Evidence from emerging intonation in Catalan and Spanish. Journal of Child Language, 39(1), 137.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Queen, R. M. (2001). Bilingual intonation patterns: Evidence of language change from Turkish-German bilingual children. Language in Society, 30(1), 5580.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Queen, R. M. (2006). Phrase-final intonation in narratives told by Turkish-German bilinguals. International Journal of Bilingualism, 10(2), 153178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ramachers, S., Brouwer, S., & Fikkert, P. (2018). No perceptual reorganization for Limburgian tones? A cross-linguistic investigation with 6-to 12-month-old infants. Journal of Child Language, 45(2), 290318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ramus, F. (2002). Language discrimination by newborns. Annual Review of Language Acquisition, 2(1), 85115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ramus, F., Hauser, M. D., Miller, C., Morris, D., & Mehler, J. (2000). Language discrimination by human newborns and by cotton-top tamarin monkeys. Science, 288(5464), 349351.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rattanasone, N. X., Tang, P., Yuen, I., Gao, L., & Demuth, K. (2018). Five-year-olds’ acoustic realization of Mandarin tone sandhi and lexical tones in context are not yet fully adult-like. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 817.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rose, Y, & Champdoizeau, C. (2007). There is no innate trochaic bias: Acoustic evidence in favour of the neutral start hypothesis. In Gavarró, A. & Freitas, M. F., Language Acquisition and Development: Proceedings of GALA 2007. Newcastle-upon-Tyne: Cambridge Scholars, pp. 359369.Google Scholar
Schmidt, E. & Post, B. (2015). The development of prosodic features and their contribution to rhythm production in simultaneous bilinguals. Language and Speech, 58(1), 2447.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwanenflugel, P. J., Westmoreland, M. R., & Benjamin, R. G. (2015). Reading fluency skill and the prosodic marking of linguistic focus. Reading and Writing, 28(1), 930.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sebina, B., Setter, J., & Daller, M. (2020). The Setswana speech rhythm of 6–7 year-old Setswana-English bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingualism, 25(3), 592605.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singh, L. & Fu, C. S. L. (2016). A new view of language development: The acquisition of lexical tone. Child Development, 87(3), 834854.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singh, L., Fu, C. S. L., Seet, X. H., et al. (2018). Developmental change in tone perception in Mandarin monolingual, English monolingual, and Mandarin-English bilingual infants: Divergences between monolingual and bilingual learners. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 173, 5977.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skoruppa, K., Pons, F., Bosch, L., et al. (2013). The development of word stress processing in French and Spanish infants. Language Learning and Development, 9(1), 88104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skoruppa, K., Pons, F., Christophe, A., et al. (2009). Language-specific stress perception by 9-month-old French and Spanish infants. Developmental Science, 12(6), 914919.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snow, D. & Balog, H. L. (2002). Do children produce the melody before the words? A review of developmental intonation research. Lingua, 112(12), 10251058.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
So, L. K. H. & Dodd, B. J. (1995). The acquisition of phonology by Cantonese-speaking children. Journal of Child Language, 22(3), 473495.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teixidó, M., François, C., Bosch, L., & Männel, C. (2018). The role of prosody in early speech segmentation and word-referent mapping. In Prieto, P. & Esteve-Gibert, N., eds., The Development of Prosody in First Language Acquisition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 79100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thorson, J. (2018). The role of prosody in early word learning: Behavioral evidence. In Prieto, P. & Esteve-Gibert, N., eds., The Development of Prosody in First Language Acquisition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 5977.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vihman, M. M., DePaolis, R. A., & Davis, B. L. (1998). Is there a “trochaic bias” in early word learning? Evidence from infant production in English and French. Child Development, 69, 935949.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whalley, K. & Hansen, J. (2006). The role of prosodic sensitivity in children’s reading development. Journal of Research in Reading, 29(3), 288303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whitworth, N. (2002). Speech rhythm production in three German-English bilingual families. Leeds Working Papers in Linguistics and Phonetics, 9, 175205.Google Scholar
Wong, P. (2012). Acoustic characteristics of three-year-olds’ correct and incorrect monosyllabic Mandarin lexical tone productions. Journal of Phonetics, 40(1), 141151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wong, P., Schwartz, R. G., & Jenkins, J. J. (2005). Perception and production of lexical tones by 3-year-old, Mandarin-speaking children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48(5), 10651079.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yao, Y., Chan, A., Fung, R., et al. (2020). Cantonese tone production in pre-school Urdu-Cantonese bilingual minority children. International Journal of Bilingualism, 24(4), 767782.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yeung, H. H., Chen, K. H., & Werker, J. F. (2013). When does native language input affect phonetic perception? The precocious case of lexical tone. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(2), 123139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zacharaki, K. & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2021). The ontogeny of early language discrimination: Beyond rhythm. Cognition, 213, 104628.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zembrzuski, D., Marecka, M., Otwinowska, A., et al. (2020). Bilingual children do not transfer stress patterns: Evidence from suprasegmental and segmental analysis of L1 and L2 speech of Polish-English child bilinguals. International Journal of Bilingualism, 24(2), 93–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×