Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-8bhkd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-17T16:10:37.002Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

7 - Diglossia, Variation, and Structural Complexity

from Part II - Arabic Variation and Sociolinguistics

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 September 2021

Karin Ryding
Affiliation:
Georgetown University, Washington DC
David Wilmsen
Affiliation:
American University of Beirut
Get access

Summary

Samira Farwaneh interrogates the largely unquestioned and untested assumption in linguistics that languages are all equally complex. Again, the indelible mark of diglossia in Arabic on theorizing about language colours the analysis, specifically with the notion that the formal Arabic of writing and declamation is necessarily more complex than the natively spoken varieties of the language. Starting from the assumption, shared by native speakers of Arabic and many linguists studying Arabic alike, that spoken varieties are simplifications of a more structurally complex and presumably chronologically older Arabic, represented by the Arabic of classical writing and its modern written descendant, she demonstrates that Arab dialects are in some ways more structurally complex than the Arabic of writing, specifically respecting the tense, mood, and aspect systems of spoken Arabi, the manifestations of indefinite noun constructs and object marking, and specifically in the so-called ‘dialectal tanwin’, co-referential and ethical dative marking, and in negation.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2021

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abu Haidar, F. (1991). Christian Arabic of Baghdad. Wiesbaden: Harrassowitz.Google Scholar
Albirini, A. (2016). Modern Arabic Sociolinguistics: Diglossia, Variation, Codeswitching, Attitudes and Identity. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Al-Zahre, N. and Boneh, N. (2010). Coreferential dative constructions in Syrian Arabic and modern Hebrew. Brill’s Journal of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics, 2(1), 248–82.Google Scholar
Bassiouney, R. (2009). Arabic Sociolinguistics. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Benmamoun, E. (2000). The Feature Structure of Functional Categories: A Comparative Study of Arabic Dialects. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Benmamoun, E. and Choueiri, L. (2015). The syntax of Arabic from a generative perspective. In Owens, J., ed., The Oxford Handbook of Arabic Linguistics. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 115–64.Google Scholar
Blanc, H. (1964). Communal Dialects in Baghdad. Harvard Middle Eastern Monographs X. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Bossong, G. (1991). Differential object marking in Romance and beyond. In Warner, D. and Kibbee, D. A., eds., New Analyses in Romance Linguistics. Amsterdam: Benjamins, 143–70.Google Scholar
Brame, M. (1973). On stress in two Arabic dialects. In Anderson, S. and Kiparsky, P., eds., A Festschrift for Morris Halle. New York: Holt, 1425.Google Scholar
Broselow, E. (1982). On predicting the interaction of stress and epenthesis. Glossa: An International Journal of Linguistics, 16(2), 115–32.Google Scholar
Broselow, E. (1992). Parametric variation in Arabic dialect phonology. In Broselow, E., Eid, M., and McCarthy, J., eds., Perspectives on Arabic Linguistics IV. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 746.Google Scholar
Brustad, K. E. (2000). The Syntax of Spoken Arabic: A Comparative Study of Moroccan, Egyptian, Syrian, and Kuwaiti Dialects. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the Theory of Syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Culicover, P. (2013). Grammar and Complexity. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dahl, O. (2004). The Growth and Maintenance of Linguistic Complexity. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
De Groot, C. (2008). Morphological complexity as a parameter of linguistic typology Hungarian as a contact language. In Miestamo, M., Sinnemäki, K., and Karlsson, F., eds., Language Complexity: Typology, Contact, Change. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 191216.Google Scholar
Eid, M. (1983). Copula function of pronouns. Lingua, 59, 197207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eksell, K. (2006). The origin and development of the cursive b-imperfect in Syrian Arabic. In Eksell, K. and Vinther, T., eds., Change in Verbal Systems: Issues on Explanation. Frankfurt am Main : Peter Lang, 7398.Google Scholar
Farwaneh, S. (1995). Directionality Effects in Arabic Dialect Syllable Structure. PhD dissertation, University of Utah.Google Scholar
Farwaneh, S. (1996). The interaction of stress and epenthesis in Arabic: An optimality-theoretic approach. In Grenoble, L., Whaley, L., and Przezdziecki, M., eds., Proceedings of the East State Conference on Linguistics. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press, 4857.Google Scholar
Fenk-Oczlon, G. and Fenk, A. (2008). Complexity trade-offs between the subsystems of language. In Miestamo, M., Sinnemäki, K., and Karlsson, F., eds., Language Complexity: Typology, Contact, Change. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 4366.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferguson, C. (1959a). Diglossia. Word, 15, 325–40.Google Scholar
Ferguson, C. (1959b). Myths about Arabic. In Harrell, R. S., ed., Georgetown University Languages and Linguistics Monograph Series, XI. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press, 7582.Google Scholar
Ferguson, C. (1959c). The Arabic koine. Language, 35(4), 616–30.Google Scholar
Ghazali, S., Hamdi, R., and Barkat, M. (2002). Speech rhythm variation in Arabic dialects. In Proceedings of Speech Prosody 2002, Aix-en-Provence, 331–4.Google Scholar
Haeri, N. (2003). Sacred Language, Ordinary People. London: Palgrave Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Håland, E. M. (2011). Miš yinfaʿ– a change in progress? A study of extended usage of the negation marker miš in Cairene Arabic. MA thesis, University of Oslo.Google Scholar
Haspelmath, M. (1999). Why is grammaticalization irreversible? Linguistics, 37(1), 4368.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holes, C. (2004). Modern Arabic: Structures, Functions, and Varieties. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Holes, C. (2018a). Arabic Historical Dialectology: Linguistic and Sociolinguistic Approaches. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holes, C. (2018b). The Arabic dialects of the Gulf: Aspects of their historical and sociolinguistics development. In Holes, C., ed., Arabic Historical Dialectology: Linguistic and Sociolinguistic Approaches. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 112–47.Google Scholar
Ingham, B. (1982). North East Arabian Dialects. London: Kegan Paul International.Google Scholar
Jarad, N. I. (2013). The evolution of the b-future marker in Syrian Arabic. Lingua Posnaniensis, LV(1), 6985.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kager, R. (2000). Surface opacity of metrical structure in optimality theory. In Hermans, B. and van Oostendorp, M., eds., The Derivational Residue in Phonological Optimality Theory. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 207–46.Google Scholar
Kenstowicz, M. and Kisseberth, C. (1979). Generative Phonology: Description and Theory. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Key, G. (2009). Differential Object Marking as an Areal Trait in Persian and Turkish. MA thesis, University of Arizona.Google Scholar
Kiparsky, Paul. (1973). Abstractness, Opacity, and Global Rules. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Linguistics Club.Google Scholar
Kiparsky, P. (2003). Syllables and moras in Arabic In Féry, C. and van de Vijver, R., eds., The Syllable in Optimality Theory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 147–82.Google Scholar
Koutsoudas, A. (1967). Object particles in Lebanese. Journal of the American Oriental Society, 87(4), 512–17.Google Scholar
Kusters, W. (2003). Linguistic Complexity: the Influence of Social Change on Verbal Inflection. PhD dissertation, University of Leiden.Google Scholar
Kusters, W. (2008). Complexity in linguistic theory, language learning and language change. In Miestamo, M., Sinnemäki, K., and Karlsson, F., eds., Language Complexity: Typology, Contact, Change, Studies in Language Companion Series. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 322.Google Scholar
Levin, A. (1987). The particle la as an object marker in some Arabic dialects of the Galilee. Zeitschrift für Arabische Linguistik, 17, 3140.Google Scholar
McWhorter, J. H. (2001). The world’s simplest grammars are creole grammars. Linguistic Typology, 5, 125–66.Google Scholar
McWhorter, J. H. (2007). Language Interrupted: Signs of Non-Native Acquisition in Standard Language Grammars. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miestamo, M. (2006). On the feasibility of complexity metrics. In Kerge, K. and Sepper, M.-M., eds., FinEst Linguistics: Proceedings of the Annual Finnish and Estonian Conference of Linguistics. Tallinn, 6– 7 May, 2004. Tallinn: TLÜ, 1126.Google Scholar
Miestamo, M. (2007). Symmetric and asymmetric encoding of functional domains, with remarks on typological markedness. In Miestamo, M. and Wälchli, B., eds., New Challenges in Typology: Broadening the Horizons and Redefining the Foundations. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, 293314.Google Scholar
Miestamo, M. (2008). Grammatical complexity in a cross-linguistic perspective. In Miestamo, M., Sinnemäki, M. K., and Karlsson, F., eds., Language Complexity: Typology, Contact, Change. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 2341.Google Scholar
Miller, K. (2014). The morpheme /-in(n)-/ in Central Asian Arabic: A comparative study. In Farwaneh, S. and Ouali, H., eds., Perspectives on Arabic Linguistics XXIV–XXV, Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins, 91118.Google Scholar
Owens, J. (2005). Pre-diaspora Arabic: Dialects, statistics and historical reconstruction. Diachronica, 22(2), 271308.Google Scholar
Owens, J. (2006). A Linguistic History of Arabic. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Owens, J. (2018). Dialects (speech communities), the apparent past, and grammaticalization: Towards an understanding of the history of Arabic. In Holes, C., ed., Arabic Historical Dialectology: Linguistic and Sociolinguistic Approaches. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 206–56.Google Scholar
Palmer, F. R. (2001). Mood and Modality, 2nd ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Persson, M. (2008). The role of the ‘b’-prefix in Gulf Arabic dialects as a marker of future, intent and/or irrealis. Journal of Arabic and Islamic Studies, 8, 2652.Google Scholar
Sampson, G. R. (2014). Complexity in language and in law. Poznan Studies in Contemporary Linguistics, 50, 169–77. www.grsampson.net/ACil.htm; last accessed 19 November 2020.Google Scholar
Sampson, G. (2017). The Linguistics Delusion. Sheffield, UK: Equinox.Google Scholar
Sampson, G. R., Gil, D., and Trudgill, P. (eds.) (2009). Language Complexity as an Evolving Variable. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Shaaban, K. A. (1977). The Phonology of Omani Arabic. PhD dissertation, University of Texas, Austin.Google Scholar
Versteegh, K. (2004). Pidginization and creolization revisited: The case of Arabic. In Haak, M., de Jong, R., and Versteegh, K., eds., Approaches to Arabic Dialects: A Collection of Articles Presented to Manfred Woidich on the Occasion of His Sixtieth Birthday. Leiden: Brill, 343–58.Google Scholar
Walters, K. (2006). Language attitudes. In Versteegh, K., Eid, M., Elgibali, A., Woidich, M., and Zaborski, A., eds., Encyclopedia of Arabic Language and Linguistics, vol. II, Leiden: Brill, 650–64.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×