Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7czq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-30T20:19:02.577Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

20 - Tandem Running Recruitment by Temnothorax Ants as a Model System for Social Learning

from Part IV - Social Learning and Teaching

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 July 2021

Allison B. Kaufman
Affiliation:
University of Connecticut
Josep Call
Affiliation:
University of St Andrews, Scotland
James C. Kaufman
Affiliation:
University of Connecticut
Get access

Summary

Collective intelligence – superior performance by groups compared to that of the individuals that compose them – is often achieved via social information use. However, collective intelligence has rarely been studied in terms of social learning. This is partially because social learning strategies (i.e. "when" and "who" to copy) are often hard to observe in a natural group setting. Our main goal in this article is to show that tandem-running recruitment by Temnothorax ants offers a promising model to study the interaction of social learning and collective intelligence. We first review the role of tandem runs in the ecology and collective behavior of these ants, who use them to share information about the locations of valuable resources. A key advantage of Temnothorax ants as a model system is that each instance of information sharing – each tandem run – can be easily observed. Moreover, the specific information transferred can be readily inferred by tracking the history and subsequent behavior of leader and follower. We then propose new investigations into how social learning via tandem runs affects their collective performance. Finally, we discuss how the synthesis of the two fields of social learning and collective intelligence can shed light on the role of feedback from learning in improving collective performance over time.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2021

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aldlerz, G. (1896). Myrmecologiska studier III. Tomognathus sublaevis Mayr: Bihang Till K. Svenska Vet.-Akad. Handlingar, 21, 176.Google Scholar
Basari, N., Bruendl, A. C., Hemingway, C. E., Roberts, N. W., Sendova-Franks, A. B., & Franks, N. R. (2014). Landmarks and ant search strategies after interrupted tandem runs. Journal of Experimental Biology, 217(6), 944954.Google ScholarPubMed
Basari, Norasmah, Laird-Hopkins, B. C., Sendova-Franks, A. B., & Franks, N. R. (2014). Trail laying during tandem-running recruitment in the ant Temnothorax albipennis. Naturwissenschaften, 101(7), 549556. Retrieved from http://link.springer.com/10.1007/s00114-014-1191-1Google Scholar
Biro, D., Sasaki, T., & Portugal, S. J. (2016). Bringing a time–depth perspective to collective animal behaviour. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 31(7), 550562.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowens, S. R., Glatt, D. P., & Pratt, S. C. (2013). Visual navigation during colony emigration by the ant Temnothorax rugatulus. PLoS ONE, 8(5), e64367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boyd, R. & Richerson, P. J. (1988). Culture and the Evolutionary Process. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Buhl, J., Hicks, K., Miller, E. R., Persey, S., Alinvi, O., & Sumpter, D. J. T. (2009). Shape and efficiency of wood ant foraging networks. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 63(3), 451460.Google Scholar
Camazine, S., Deneubourd, J. L., Franks, N. R., Sneyd, J., Theraulaz, G., & Bonabeau, E. (2003). Self-Organization in Biological Systems. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Czaczkes, T. J., Grüter, C., & Ratnieks, F. L. W. (2015). Trail pheromones: An integrative view of their role in social insect colony organization. Annual Review of Entomology, 60(1), 581599.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Czaczkes, T. J. & Heinze, J. (2015). Ants adjust their pheromone deposition to a changing environment and their probability of making errors. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 282(1810), 20150679.Google Scholar
Dean, L. G., Kendal, R. L., Schapiro, S. J., Thierry, B., & Laland, K. N. (2012). Identification of the social and cognitive processes underlying human cumulative culture. Science, 335(6072), 11141118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dean, Lewis G., Vale, G. L., Laland, K. N., Flynn, E., & Kendal, R. L. (2014). Human cumulative culture: A comparative perspective. Biological Reviews, 89(2), 284301.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Detrain, C., Deneubourg, J.-L., & Pasteels, J. M. (1999). Decision-Making in Foraging by Social Insects. In Detrain, C, Deneubourg, J. L., & Pasteels, J. M. (Eds.), Information Processing in Social Insects (pp. 331354). Boston: Die Deutsche Bibliotek.Google Scholar
Detrain, C. & Deneubourg, J.-L. (2008). Collective decision-making and foraging patterns in ants and honeybees. Advances in Insect Physiology, 35, 123173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dorigo, M. & Stützle, T. (2004). Ant Colony Optimization. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Franklin, E. L. (2014). The journey of tandem running: The twists, turns and what we have learned. Insectes Sociaux, 61(1), 18.Google Scholar
Franklin, Elizabeth L., & Franks, N. R. (2012). Individual and social learning in tandem-running recruitment by ants. Animal Behaviour, 84(2), 361368.Google Scholar
Franklin, Elizabeth L., Robinson, E. J. H., Marshall, J. A. R., Sendova-Franks, A. B., & Franks, N. R. (2012). Do ants need to be old and experienced to teach? Journal of Experimental Biology, 215(Pt 8), 12871292.Google Scholar
Franks, N. & Richardson, T. (2006). Teaching in tandem-running ants. Nature, 439(12), 153.Google Scholar
Giraldeau, L. A., Valone, T. J., & Templeton, J. J. (2002). Potential disadvantages of using socially acquired information. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 357(1427), 15591566.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gordon, D. (2010). Ant Encounters: Interaction Networks and Colony Behavior. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Grüter, C., Leadbeater, E., & Ratnieks, F. L. W. (2010). Social learning: The importance of copying others. Current Biology, 20(16), R683R685.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grüter, C. & Leadbeater, E. (2014). Insights from insects about adaptive social information use. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 29(3), 177184.Google Scholar
Guilford, T. & Biro, D. (2014). Route following and the pigeon’s familiar area map. Journal of Experimental Biology, 217(2), 169179.Google Scholar
Henrich, J. (2004). Demography and cultural evolution: How adaptive cultural processes can produce maladaptive losses – the Tasmanian case. American Antiquity, 69(02), 197214.Google Scholar
Heyes, C. M. (1994). Social learning in animals: Categories and mechanisms. Biological Reviews, 69(2), 207231.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoppitt, W. & Laland, K. N. (2013). Social Learning. An Introduction to Mechanisms, Methods, and Models. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Jayles, B., Kim, H.-R., Escobedo, R., Cezera, S., Blanchet, A., Kameda, T., … Theraulaz, G. (2017). How social information can improve estimation accuracy in human groups. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(47), 1262012625.Google Scholar
Kameda, Tatsuya & Nakanishi, D. (2002). Cost–benefit analysis of social/cultural learning in a nonstationary uncertain environment. Evolution and Human Behavior, 23(5), 373393.Google Scholar
Kameda, T. & Nakanishi, D. (2003). Does social/cultural learning increase human adaptability? Rogers’s question revisited. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24(4), 242260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendal, R. L., Boogert, N. J., Rendell, L., Laland, K. N., Webster, M., & Jones, P. L. (2018). Social learning strategies: Bridge-building between fields. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(7), 651665.Google Scholar
Kerr, N. L. & Tindale, R. S. (2004). Group performance and decision making. Annual Review Psychology, (55), 623655.Google Scholar
Krause, J. & Ruxton, G. (2002). Living in Groups. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krause, J., Ruxton, G., & Krause, S. (2010). Swarm intelligence in animals and humans. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 25(1), 2834.Google Scholar
Laland, K. N. (2004). Social learning strategies. Animal Learning & Behavior, 32(1), 414.Google Scholar
Larson, J. R. Jr. (2013). In Search of Synergy in Small Group Performance. Philadelphia: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Laughlin, P. R. (2011). Group Problem Solving. Oxford: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Leadbeater, E. & Chittka, L. (2007). Social learning in insects: From miniature brains to consensus building. Current Biology, 17(16), R703R713.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leadbeater, E. & Chittka, L. (2009). Social Information Use in Foraging Insects. In Jarau, S. & Hrncir, M. (Eds.), Food Exploitation by Social Insects (pp. 135146). Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leadbeater, Ellouise & Dawson, E. H. (2017). A social insect perspective on the evolution of social learning mechanisms. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(30), 78387845.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lefebvre, L. & Aplin, L. M. (2017). Social Learning and Innovation. In ten Cate, C. & Healy, S. D. (Eds.), Avian Cognition (pp. 93118). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Mallon, E. B., Pratt, S. C., & Franks, N. R. (2001). Individual and collective decision-making during nest site selection by the ant Leptothorax albipennis. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 50(4), 352359.Google Scholar
Mason, J. R. & Reidinger, R. F. (1982). Observational learning of food aversions in red-winged blackbirds (Agelaius phoeniceus). The Auk, 99(3), 548554.Google Scholar
Meade, J., Biro, D., & Guilford, T. (2005). Homing pigeons develop local route stereotypy. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 272(1558), 1723.Google Scholar
Möglich, Michael. (1978). Social organization of nest emigration in Leptothorax. Insectes Sociaux, 25(3), 205225.Google Scholar
Möglich, M., Maschwitz, U., & Hölldobler, B. (1974). Tandem calling: A new kind of signal in ant communication. Science, 186(4168), 10461047.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Partridge, L. W., Partridge, K. A., & Franks, N. R. (1997). Field survey of a monogynous leptothoracine ant (Hymenoptera, Formicidae) evidence of seasonal polydomy? Insectes Sociaux, 44(2), 7583.Google Scholar
Planqué, R., Dechaume-Moncharmont, F. X., Franks, N. R., Kovacs, T., & Marshall, J. A. R. (2007). Why do house-hunting ants recruit in both directions? Naturwissenschaften, 94, 911918.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pratt, S. C. (2005). Behavioral mechanisms of collective nest-site choice by the ant Temnothorax curvispinosus. Insectes Sociaux, 52(4), 383392.Google Scholar
Pratt, Stephen C., Mallon, E., Sumpter, D., & Franks, N. (2002). Quorum sensing, recruitment, and collective decision-making during colony emigration by the ant Leptothorax albipennis. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 52(2), 117127.Google Scholar
Rendell, Luke, Fogarty, L., & Laland, K. N. (2010). Rogers’ paradox recast and resolved: Population structure and the evolution of social learning strategies. Evolution, 64(2), 534548.Google Scholar
Rendell, L., Boyd, R., Cownden, D., Enquist, M., Eriksson, K., Feldman, M. W., … Laland, K. N. (2010). Why copy others? Insights from the social learning strategies tournament. Science, 328(5975), 208213.Google Scholar
Rendell, L., Boyd, R., Enquist, M., Feldman, M. W., Fogarty, L., & Laland, K. N. (2011). How copying affects the amount, evenness and persistence of cultural knowledge: Insights from the social learning strategies tournament. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 366(1567), 11181128.Google Scholar
Rogers, A. R. (1988). Does biology constrain culture? American Anthropologist, 90(4), 819831.Google Scholar
Sasaki, T. & Biro, D. (2017). Cumulative culture can emerge from collective intelligence in animal groups. Nature Communications, 8, 15049.Google Scholar
Sasaki, T., Danczak, L., Thompson, B., Morshed, T., & Pratt, S. C. (2020). Route learning during tandem running in the rock ant Temnothorax albipennis. The Journal of Experimental Biology, 223(9), jeb221408.Google Scholar
Seeley, Thomas D. (1995). The Wisdom of Hives: The Social Physiology of Honey Bee Colonies. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Seeley, Thomas D. (2010). Honeybee Democracy. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Shaffer, Z., Sasaki, T., & Pratt, S. C. (2013). Linear recruitment leads to allocation and flexibility in collective foraging by ants. Animal Behaviour, 86(5), 967975.Google Scholar
Stroeymeyt, N., Giurfa, M., & Franks, N. R. (2010). Improving decision speed, accuracy and group cohesion through early information gathering in house-hunting ants. PLoS ONE, 5(9), e13059.Google Scholar
Stuart, R. J. (1986). An early record of tandem running in Leptothoracine ants: Gottfrid Adlerz, 1896. Psyche: A Journal of Entomology, 93(1–2), 103106.Google Scholar
Sumpter, D. J. T. (2010). Collective Animal Behavior. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Surowiecki, J. (2005). The Wisdom of Crowds. Why the Many Are Smarter than the Few. New York: Random House.Google Scholar
Tennie, C., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Ratcheting up the ratchet: On the evolution of cumulative culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 364(1528), 24052415.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Kruger, A. C., & Ratner, H. H. (1993). Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16(03), 495511.Google Scholar
Visscher, P. K. & Camazine, S. (1999). Collective decisions and cognition in bees. Nature, 397(6718), 400.Google Scholar
von Frisch, K. (1967). The Dance Language and Orientation of Bees. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Yamamoto, S., Humle, T., & Tanaka, M. (2013). Basis for cumulative cultural evolution in chimpanzees: Social learning of a more efficient tool-use technique. PLoS ONE, 8(1), e55768.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×