Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2brh9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-27T13:44:57.621Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

34 - Innovation and Problem-Solving in Orangutans

from Part VI - Innovation and Problem-Solving

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 July 2021

Allison B. Kaufman
Affiliation:
University of Connecticut
Josep Call
Affiliation:
University of St Andrews, Scotland
James C. Kaufman
Affiliation:
University of Connecticut
Get access

Summary

This chapter reviews available evidence on cognitive contributions to orangutan innovation and problem-solving. Evidence derives from orangutans in three living conditions (wild, captive, rehabilitation), and as such includes spontaneous as well as experimentally elicited innovations. Reviewed are the range and quality of orangutans’ innovations and problem-solving, including instigating factors, cognitive complexity, and facilitating–inhibiting factors.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2021

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bastian, M. L., van Noordwijk, M. A., & van Schaik, C. P. (2012). Innovative behaviors in wild Bornean orangutans revealed by targeted population comparison. Behaviour, 149(2012) 275297.Google Scholar
Bebko, A. O. (2012). Route-Based Travel Networks in Wild Orangutans. MA Thesis, Psychology, York University, Toronto, CanadaGoogle Scholar
Boesch, C. & Boesch-Achermann, H. (2000). The Chimpanzees of the Taï Forest: Behavioural Ecology and Evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bertinetto, A. (2012). Performing the unexpected: Improvisation and artistic creativity. Revista Internacional de Filosofıa, 57, 117135.Google Scholar
Byrne, R. W. (1999). Imitation without intentionality: Using string parsing to copy the organization of behavior. Animal Cognition, 2, 6372.Google Scholar
Byrne, R. W. (2003). Imitation as behaviour parsing. Philosophical Transactions: Biological Sciences, 358, 529536.Google Scholar
Byrne, R. W. (2004). The Manual Skills and Cognition that Lie Behind Hominid Tool Use. In Russon, A. E. & Begun, D. R. (Eds.), The Evolution of Thought: Evolutionary Origins of Great Ape Intelligence (pp. 3144). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press,.Google Scholar
Byrne, R. W. & Byrne, J. M. E. (1993). Complex leaf-gathering skills of mountain gorillas (Gorilla g. beringei): Variability and standardization. American Journal of Primatology, 31, 241261.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byrne, R. W. & Russon, A. E. (1998). Learning by imitation: A hierarchical approach. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 667721.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byrne, R. W., Corps, N., & Byrne, J. M. E. (2001a). Estimating the complexity of animal behavior: How mountain gorillas eat thistles. Behaviour, 138, 525557.Google Scholar
Byrne, R. W., Corp, N., & Byrne, J. M. E. (2001b). Manual dexterity in the gorilla: Bimanual and digit role differentiation in a natural task. Animal Cognition, 4, 347361.Google Scholar
Call, J. (2015). Conservatism versus Innovation: The Great Ape Story. In Kaufman, A. B. & Kaufman, J. C. (Eds.), Animal Creativity and Innovation (pp. 397414). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Cropley, D. H. (2015). Commentary on Chapter 15: Tools for the Trees: Orangutan Arboreal Tool Use and Creativity. In Kaufman, A. B. & Kaufman, J. C. (Eds.), Animal Creativity and Innovation (pp. 456458). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Damerius, L. A., Graber, S. M., Willems, E. P., & van Schaik, C. P. (2017a). Curiosity boosts orang-utan problem-solving ability. Animal Behaviour, 134, 5770. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2017.10.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Damerius, L. A., Forss, S. I. F., Kosonen, Z. K., Willems, E. P., Burkart, J. M., Call, J., Galdikas, B. M. F, Liebal, K., Haun, D. B. M, & van Schaik, D. P. (2017b). Orientation to humans predicts cognitive performance in orangutans. Scientific Reports, 7, 40052. doi: 10.1038/srep40052Google Scholar
Deaner, R. O., van Schaik, C. P., & Johnson, V. (2006). Do some taxa have better domain-general cognition than others? A meta-analysis of nonhuman primate studies. Evolutionary Psychology, 4, 149196.Google Scholar
Delgado, R. A. & van Schaik, C. P. (2000). The behavioral ecology and conservation of the orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus): A tale of two islands. Evolutionary Anthropology, 9 (5), 201-218.Google Scholar
Forss, S. I. F., Schuppli, C., Haiden, D., Zweifel, N., & van Schaik, C. P. (2015). Contrasting responses to novelty by wild and captive orangutans. American Journal of Primatology, 77, 11091121. (doi: 10.1002/ajp.22445)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fox, E. A., Sitompul, A. F., & van Schaik, C. P. (1999). Intelligent Tool Use in Wild Sumatran Orangutans. In Parker, S. T., Mitchell, R. W., & Miles, H. L. (Eds.), The Mentalities of Gorillas and Orangutans (pp. 99116). Cambridge UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gibson, K. R. (1993). Tool Use, Language and Social Behaviour in Relationship to Information Processing Capacities. In Gibson, K. R. & Ingold, T. (Eds.), Tools, Language and Cognition in Human Evolution (pp. 251269). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press,.Google Scholar
Gottlieb, J., Oudeyer, P.-Y., Lopes, M., & Barnes, A. (2013). Information-seeking, curiosity, and attention: Computational and neural mechanisms. Trends in Cognitive Science, 17(11). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.09.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Griffin, A. S. (2016). Innovation as an emergent property: A new alignment of comparative and experimental research on animal innovation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 371, 20150544. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0544CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gruber, T., Singleton, I., & van Schaik, C. (2012). Sumatran orangutans differ in their cultural knowledge but not their cognitive abilities. Current Biology, 22, 22312235. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.09.041CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herzfeld, C. (2016). Wattana: An Orangutan in Paris. Translated from French by Martin, O. Y. & Martin, R. D.. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huffman, M. (1997). Current evidence for self-medication in primates: A multidisciplinary approach. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology, 40, 171200.Google Scholar
Huffman, M & Hirata, S (2003). Biological and Ecological Foundations of Primate Behavioral Traditions. In Fragaszy, D. M. & Perry, S. (Eds.) The Biology of Traditions: Models and Evidence (pp. 267296). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kummer, H. & Goodall, J. (1985). Conditions of innovative behaviour in primates. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society London B, 308, 203214. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1985.0020Google Scholar
Lehner, S. R., Burkart, J. M., & van Schaik, C. P. (2010). An evaluation of the geographic method for recognizing innovations in nature, using zoo orangutans. Primates, 51, 101118.Google Scholar
Lehner, S. R., Burkart, J. M., & van Schaik, C. P. (2011). Can captive orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus abelii) be coaxed into cumulative build-up of techniques? Journal of Comparative Psychology, 125, 446455. doi: 10.1037/a0024413Google Scholar
Manrique, H. M., Volter, C. J., & Call, J. (2013). Repeated innovation in great apes. Animal Behaviour, 85, 195202.Google Scholar
Marshall, A. J., Ancrenaz, M., Brearley, F. Q., Fredriksson, G. M., Ghaffar, N., Heydon, M., Husson, S. J., Leighton, M., McConkey, K. R., Morrogh-Bernard, H. C., Proctor, J., van Schaik, C. P., Yeager, C. P., & Wich, S. A. (2009). The Effects of Forest Phenology and Floristics on Populations of Bornean and Sumatran Orangutans. In Wich, S. A., Utami, S. A., Mitra, S. T., van Schaik, P. (Eds.), Orangutans: Geographic Variation in Behavioral Ecology and Conservation (pp. 97117). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Matsuzawa, T. (1996). Chimpanzee Intelligence in Nature and Captivity: Isomorphism of Symbol Use and Tool Use. In McGrew, W. C., Marchant, L. F., & Nishida, T. (Eds.), Great Ape Societies (pp. 196209). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
McGrew, W. (1992). The Intelligent Use of Tools: Twenty Propositions. In Gibson, K. R. & Ingold, T. (Eds.), Tools, Language and Intelligence: Evolutionary Implications (pp. 151170). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Morrogh-Bernard, H. (2008). Fur-rubbing as a form of self-medication in Pongo pygmaeus. International Journal of Primatology. doi: 10.1007/s10764–008-9266-5Google Scholar
Nater, A., Mattle-Greminger, M. P., Nurcahyo, A., Nowak, M. G., et al. (2017). Morphometric, behavioral, and genomic evidence for a new orangutan species. Current Biology. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.09CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parker, S. T. & McKinney, M. L. (1999). Origins of Intelligence: The Evolution of Cognitive Development in Monkeys, Apes, and Humans. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Povinelli, D. J. & Cant, J. G. H. (1995). Arboreal clambering and the evolution of self conception. Quarterly Review of Biology, 70, 393421.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramsey, G., Bastian, M. L., & van Schaik, C. P. (2007). Animal innovation defined and operationalized. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 393437.Google Scholar
Reader, S. M. (2016). Animal and human innovation: Novel problems and novel solutions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 371, 20150182. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0182Google Scholar
Reader, S. M. & Laland, K. N. (2001). Primate innovation: Sex, age and social rank differences. International Journal of Primatology, 22(5), 787805.Google Scholar
Reader, S. M. & Laland, K. N. (2002) Social intelligence, innovation and enhanced brain size in primates. Procedures of the National Academy of Science USA, 99, 44364441. doi: 10. 1073/pnas.062041299Google Scholar
Reader, S. M. & Laland, K. N. (Eds.) (2003) Animal Innovation. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Russon, A. E. (1998). The nature and evolution of intelligence in orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus). Primates, 39(4), 485503.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russon, A. E. (2004). Great Ape Cognitive Systems. In Russon, A. E. & Begun, D. R. (Eds.), The Evolution of Thought: Evolutionary Origins of Great Ape Intelligence (pp. 76100). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Russon, A. E., van Schaik, C. P., Kuncoro, P., Ferisa, A., Handayani, D. P., van Noordwijk, M. A. (2009). Innovation and Intelligence in Orangutans. In Witch, S. A., Utami, S. A., Mitra, S. T., & van Schaik, P. (Eds.), Orangutans: Geographic Variation in Behavioral Ecology and Conservation (pp. 279298). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Russon, A. E., Kuncoro, P., Ferisa, A., & Handayani, D. P. (2010). How orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) innovate for water. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 124(1), 1428.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Russon, A. E., Kuncoro, P., & Ferisa, A. (2015). Tools for the Trees: Orangutan Arboreal Tool Use and Creativity. In Kaufman, A. B. & Kaufman, J. C. (Eds.), Animal Creativity and Innovation (pp. 419455). San Diego, CA: Academic Press/Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schuppli, C., Forss, S., Meulman, E., Utami Atmoko, S., van Noordwijk, M., & van Schaik, C. (2017). The effects of sociability on exploratory tendency and innovation repertoires in wild Sumatran and Bornean orangutans. Scientific Reports, 7, 15464. doi: 10.1038/s41598–017-15640-xGoogle Scholar
Shettleworth, S. (2010). Cognition, Evolution, and Behavior (2nd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Shumaker, R. W., Walkup, K. R., & Beck, B. (2011). Animal Tool Behavior: The Use and Manufacture of Tools by Animals (Revised and updated edition). Baltimore, MD: John’s Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Singleton, I., Knott, C. D., Morrogh-Bernard, H.C., Wich, S. A., & van Schaik, C. P. (2009). Ranging Behavior of Orangutan Females and Social Organization. In Wich, S. A et al. (Eds.), Orangutans: Geographic Variation in Behavioral Ecology and Conservation (pp. 205213). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sol, D. (2015). The Evolution of Innovativeness: Exaptation or Specialized Adaptation? In Kaufman, A. B. & Kaufman, J. C. (Eds.), Animal Creativity and Innovation (pp. 163182). San Diego, CA: Academic Press/Elsevier.Google Scholar
Stokes, E. & Byrne, R. W. (2001). Cognitive abilities for behavioural flexibility in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): The effect of snare injury on complex manual food processing. Animal Cognition, 4, 1128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sugardjito, J. & van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M. (1986). Age-sex class differences in the positional behavior of the Sumatran orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus abelii) in the Gunung Leuser National Park, Indonesia. Folia Primatologica, 47, 1425.Google Scholar
Thorpe, S. K. S, Crompton, R. H., & Alexander, R. M. (2007). Orangutans use compliant branches to lower the energetic cost of locomotion. Biology Letters, 3, 253256. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2007.0049Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Kruger, A. C., & Ratner, H. H. (1993). Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 495511.Google Scholar
van Schaik, C. P., Deaner, R. O., & Merrill, M. Y. (1999). The conditions for tool use in primates: Implications for the evolution of material culture. Journal of Human Evolution, 36, 719741.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Schaik, C. P. & Knott, C. D. (2001). Geographic variation in tool use on Neesia fruit in orangutans. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 114, 331342.Google Scholar
van Schaik, C. P., Ancrenaz, M., Borgen, G., Galdikas, B., Knott, C. D., Singleton, I., Suzuki, A., Utami, S. S., & Merrill, M. (2003). Orangutan cultures and the evolution of material culture. Science, 299, 102105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Schaik, C. P., van Noordwijk, M. A., & Wich, S. A. (2006). Innovation in wild Bornean orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus wurmbii). Behaviour, 143, 839–76. doi: 10.1163/156853906778017944Google Scholar
van Schaik, C. P., Burkart, J., Damerius, L., Forss, S. I. F., Koops, K., van Noordwijk, M. A., & Schuppli, C. (2016). The reluctant innovator: Orangutans and phylogeny of creativity. Phil. Trans. Royal Society B, 371, 20150183. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0183Google Scholar
Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C., Nishida, T., Reynolds, V., Sugiyama, Y., Tutin, C. E. G., Wrangham, R. W. & Boesch, C. (1999). Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature, 399, 682685.Google Scholar
Wich, S. A., Utami-Atmoko, S. S., Mitra-Setia, T., & van Schaik, C. P. (Eds.) (2009). Orangutans: Geographic Variation in Behavioral Ecology and Conservation. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Yamakoshi, G. (2004). Evolution of Complex Feeding Techniques in Primates: Is This the Origin of Great-Ape Intelligence? In Russon, A. E. & Begun, D. R. (Eds.), The Evolution of Thought: Evolutionary Origins of Great Ape Intelligence (pp. 140171). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×