Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-fbnjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-20T05:27:30.918Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Works Cited

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 August 2023

Ross A. Thompson
Affiliation:
University of California, Davis
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
The Brain Development Revolution
Science, the Media, and Public Policy
, pp. 289 - 312
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2023

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Works Cited

Adato, A. (2002). The education of Meathead. Los Angeles Times Magazine, January 20, pp. 14–33. www.latimes.com/archives/la-xpm-2002-jan-20-tm-24150-story.html, retrieved January 14, 2023.Google Scholar
Aguiar, A., & Baillargeon, R. (1998). Eight-and-a-half-month-old infants’ reasoning about containment events. Child Development, 69, 636653.Google Scholar
Akin, H. (2017). Overview of the science of science communication. In Jamieson, K. H., Kahan, D. M., & Scheufele, D. A. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of the science of science communication (pp. 2534). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Alexander, W. T., & Brown, J. W. (2011). Medial prefrontal cortex as an action-outcome predictor. Nature Neuroscience, 14, 13381344.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allen, G. (2011). Early intervention: Smart investment, massive savings. Second independent report to Her Majesty’s Government. https://core.ac.uk/reader/4153920, retrieved June 29, 2022.Google Scholar
Alvarez, M., Golden-Testa, K., Navarro, A., & Nolledo, R. (2018). A golden opportunity: Lessons from California on advancing coverage for all children. Los Angeles, CA: Children’s Partnership. https://health4allkids.org/wp-content/uploads/2018/01/Golden-Opportunity-Lessons-From-Californias-Journey-to-Advancing-Coverage-for-All-Children-Report.pdf, retrieved June 3, 2022.Google Scholar
American Psychological Association (1989). Amicus curiae brief filed in U.S. Supreme Court in Hodgson v. Minnesota, 497 U.S. 417 (1990). www.apa.org/about/offices/ogc/amicus/index-chron, retrieved November 15, 2021.Google Scholar
American Psychological Association (2004). Amicus curiae brief filed in U.S. Supreme Court in Roper v. Simmons, 543 U.S. 551 (2005). www.apa.org/about/offices/ogc/amicus/roper.pdf, retrieved November 11, 2021.Google Scholar
Aono, D., Yaffe, G., & Kober, H. (2019). Neuroscientific evidence in the courtroom: A review. Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications, 4, 40.Google ScholarPubMed
Aristotle (350 bce). The History of Animals, Book VIII, Part 1, translated by Thompson, D. W. (Oxford: Clarendon, 1910).Google Scholar
Atkins v. Virginia, 536 U.S. 302 (2002).Google Scholar
Bachleda, A. R., & Thompson, R. A. (2018). How babies think. Zero to Three Journal, 38(3), 410.Google Scholar
Bales, S. N. (2014). Haruv Award Lecture: Stickiness is an empirical pursuit: The case for reframing child mental health. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 84, 1218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bangerter, A., & Heath, C. (2004). The Mozart effect: Tracking the evolution of a scientific legend. British Journal of Social Psychology, 43, 605623.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baptista, P., & Andrade, J. P. (2018). Adult hippocampal neurogenesis: Regulation and possible functional and clinical correlates. Frontiers in Neuroanatomy, 12, article 44.Google Scholar
Barnett, W. S., & Masse, L. N. (2007). Comparative benefit-cost analysis of the Abecedarian program and its policy implications. Economics of Education Review, 26, 113125.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F. (2009). The future of psychology: Connecting mind to brain. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4, 326339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bartik, T. J. (2011). Investing in kids: Early childhood programs and local economic development. Kalamazoo, MI: Upjohn Institute.Google Scholar
Bassok, D., Latham, S., & Rorem, A. (2016). Is kindergarten the new first grade? AERA Open, 1, 131.Google Scholar
Bateson, G. (1972). Steps to an ecology of mind. Novato, CA: Chandler Publishing.Google Scholar
Bauer, P. J. (2013). Memory development. In Rubenstein, J. L. R. & Rakic, P. (Eds.), Neural circuit development and function in the brain (pp. 297314). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Baxter, L. (n.d.). An evaluation of Pew’s pre-kindergarten initiative. Washington, DC: Pew Charitable Trusts. www.pewtrusts.org/en/about/how-we-work/lessons-learned/expanding-prek-education, retrieved July 9, 2022.Google Scholar
Becht, A. I., Wierenga, L. M., Mills, K. L., Meuwese, R., van Duijvendoorde, A., Blakemore, S.-J., Güroglu, B., & Crone, E. A. (2021). Beyond the average brain: Individual differences in social brain development are associated with friendship quality. Social and Affective Neuroscience, 16, 292301.Google Scholar
Beck, J. (1997). For kids, learning doesn’t start in school. Chicago Tribune, May 4.Google Scholar
Begley, S. (1996). Your child’s brain. Newsweek, February 19, pp. 54–62.Google Scholar
Begley, S. (1998). The parent trap. Newsweek, September 7, pp. 50–59.Google Scholar
Bernzweig, J., Branom, C., & Wellenkamp, J. (2021). New directions in kindergarten readiness. Zero to Three Journal, 41(Suppl.). www.zerotothree.org/resources/3857-perspectives-new-directions-in-kindergarten-readiness, retrieved June 13, 2022.Google Scholar
Best, J. R., & Miller, P. H. (2010). A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Development, 81, 16411660.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bjork, J. M., & Pardini, D. A. (2015). Who are those “risk-taking adolescents”? Individual differences in developmental neuroimaging research. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 11, 5664.Google Scholar
Blakemore, S.-J. (2012). Development of the social brain in adolescence. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine, 105, 111116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blakemore, S.-J., & Frith, U. (2005a). The learning brain: Lessons for education. Oxford: Blackwell.Google ScholarPubMed
Blakemore, S.-J., & Frith, U. (2005b). The learning brain: Lessons for education: A precis. Developmental Science, 8, 459471.Google Scholar
Blakemore, S.-J., & Mills, K. L. (2014). Is adolescence a sensitive period for sociocultural processing? Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 187207.Google Scholar
Bodenhorn, K. A., & Kelch, D. R. (2001). Implementation of California’s Children and Families First Act of 1998. The Future of Children, 11, 150157.Google Scholar
Bohlander, A., Lerner, C., & Thompson, R. (Eds.). (2017). The growing brain: From birth to 5 years old. A curriculum for early childhood providers. Washington, DC: Zero to Three.Google Scholar
Boldt, D. (1999). Drilling holes in myth about first three years. Baltimore Sun, August 30, p. 20.Google Scholar
Bonnie, R. J., & Scott, E. S. (2013). The teenage brain: Adolescent brain research and the law. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 158161.Google Scholar
Bower, C. (1997). Study hear examines babies’ brain growth. St. Louis Post-Dispatch, August 5, p. 1A.Google Scholar
Brink, K. A., Lane, J. D., & Wellman, H. M. (2015). Developmental pathways for social understanding: Linking social cognition to social contexts. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, article 719.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brito, N. H., & Noble, K. G. (2014). Socioeconomic status and structural brain development. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 8, article 217.Google Scholar
Brooks-Gunn, J., & Duncan, G. J. (1997). The effects of poverty on children. The Future of Children, 7, 5571.Google Scholar
Bruer, J. T. (1997). Education and the brain: A bridge too far. Educational Researcher, 26, 416.Google Scholar
Bruer, J. T. (1999). The myth of the first three years. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. (1986). Value presuppositions of developmental theory. In Cirillo, L. & Wapner, S. (Eds.), Value presuppositions in theories of human development (pp. 1928). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Buchanan, C. M., & Hughes, J. L. (2009). Construction of social reality during early adolescence: Can expecting storm and stress increase real or perceived storm and stress? Journal of Research on Adolescence, 19, 261285.Google Scholar
Bushouse, B. K. (2009). Universal preschool: Policy change, stability, and the Pew Charitable Trusts. Albany: State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
Buss, D. (2015). Evolutionary psychology: The new science of the mind (6th Ed.). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Butler, L. P., & Markman, E. M. (2012). Preschoolers use intentional and pedagogical cues to guide inductive inferences and exploration. Child Development, 83, 14161428.Google Scholar
Butler, L. P., & Walton, G. M. (2013). The opportunity to collaborate increases preschoolers’ motivation for challenging tasks. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116, 953961.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byrne, M. L., Whittle, S., Vijayakumar, N., Dennison, M., Simmons, J. G., & Allen, N. B. (2017). A systematic review of adrenarche as a sensitive period in neurobiological development and mental health. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 25, 1228.Google Scholar
Byrnes, J. P. (2001). Minds, brains, and learning: Understanding the psychological and educational relevance of neuroscientific research. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Byrnes, J. P. (2007). Some ways in which neuroscientific research can be relevant to education. In Coch, D., Fischer, K. W., & Dawson, G. (Eds.), Human behavior, learning, and the developing brain: Typical development (pp. 3049). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Cannon, J. S., Kilburn, M. R., Karoly, L. A., Mattox, T., Muchow, A. N., & Burnaventura, M. (2017). Investing early: Taking stock of outcomes and economic returns from early childhood programs. Santa Monica, CA: RAND Corporation. www.rand.org/pubs/research_reports/RR1993.html, retrieved January 5, 2023.Google Scholar
Caplan, N. (1976). Social research and national policy: What gets used, by whom, for what purposes, and with what effects? International Social Science Journal, 28, 187194.Google Scholar
Caplan, N. (1979). The two-communities theory and knowledge utilization. American Behavioral Scientist, 22, 459470.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caplan, N., & Nelson, S. D. (1973). On being useful: The nature and consequences of psychological research on social problems. American Psychologist, 28, 199211.Google Scholar
Carey, S. (1978). The child as word learner. In Bresnan, J., Miller, G., & Halle, M. (Eds.), Linguistic theory and psychological reality (pp. 264293). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Carnegie Task Force on Meeting the Needs of Young Children (1994). Starting points: Meeting the needs of our youngest children. Waldorf, MD: Carnegie Corporation of New York.Google Scholar
Carneiro, P., & Heckman, J. (2003). Human capital policy. National Bureau of Economic Research Working Paper 9495, February. www.nber.org/papers/w9495, retrieved June 16, 2022.Google Scholar
Casey, B. J., Jones, R. M., & Somerville, L. H. (2011). Braking and accelerating of the adolescent brain. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 21, 2133.Google Scholar
Casey, B. J., Trainor, R., Giedd, J., Vauss, Y. Vaituzis, C. K., Hamburger, S., Kozuch, P., & Rapoport, J. L. (1997). The role of the anterior cingulate in automatic and controlled processes: A developmental neuroanatomical study. Developmental Psychobiology, 30, 6169.Google Scholar
Casey, B. J., Trainor, R. J., Orendi, J. L., Schubert, A. B., Nystrom, L. E., et al. (1997). A developmental functional MRI study of prefrontal activation during performance of a go-no-go task. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 835847.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Center on the Developing Child (2007). A science-based framework for early childhood policy: Using evidence to improve outcomes in learning, behavior, and health for vulnerable children. https://harvardcenter.wpenginepowered.com/wp-content/uploads/2016/02/Policy_Framework.pdf, retrieved January 8, 2023.Google Scholar
Center on the Developing Child (2014). A decade of science informing policy: The story of the National Scientific Council on the Developing Child. https://developingchild.harvard.edu/resources/decade-science-informing-policy-story-national-scientific-council-developing-child/, retrieved July 29, 2022.Google Scholar
Chabot, M. J., Kwan, K., Guenzburger, G. V., Lavezzo, M., & Pressfield, L. (2021). Adolescent births in California, 2000–2018. Sacramento: California Dept. of Public Health. www.cdph.ca.gov/Programs/CFH/DMCAH/surveillance/CDPH%20Document%20Library/Adolescents/Adolescent-Births-in-CA-2018.pdf, retrieved June 3, 2022.Google Scholar
Charen, M. (1999). Child’s first three years not as critical. Omaha World-Herald, September 22, p. 24.Google Scholar
Chen, B.-B., Li, X., & Chen, N. (2017). Positive youth development in China. In Dimitrova, R. (Ed.), Well-being of youth and emerging adults across cultures (pp. 3548). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1968). Language and mind. New York: Harcourt, Brace.Google Scholar
Choudhury, S., McKinney, K. A., & Merten, M. (2012). Rebelling against the brain: Public engagement with the “neurological adolescent.” Social Science & Medicine, 74, 565573.Google Scholar
Choudhury, S., & Slaby, J. (2012). Introduction: Critical neuroscience – between lifeworld and laboratory. In Choudhury, S. & Slaby, J. (Eds.), Critical neuroscience: A handbook of the social and cultural contexts of neuroscience (pp. 126). London: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Clinton, H. R. (1996). It takes a village, and other lessons children teach us. New York: Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
Cohen, L. B., & Oakes, L. M. (1993). How infants perceive a simple causal event. Developmental Psychology, 29, 421433.Google Scholar
Coleman, J. C., (2011). The nature of adolescence (4th Ed.). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Collins, W. A. (1990). Parent-child relationships in the transition to adolescence: Continuity and change in interaction, affect, and cognition. In Montemayor, R., Adams, G. R., & Gullota, T. P. (Eds.), Advances in adolescent development, Vol. 2: From childhood to adolescence: A transitional period? (pp. 85106). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Committee on the Science of Adolescence, Board on Children, Youth, and Families (2011). The science of adolescent risk-taking: Workshop report. Washington, DC: National Academies Press.Google Scholar
Conger, K. J., Martin, M. J., Reeb, B. T., Little, W. M., Craine, J. L., Shebloski, B., & Conger, R. D. (2012). Economic hardship and its consequences across generations. In Maholmes, V. & King, R. B. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of poverty and child development (pp. 3753). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cowan, W. M. (1979). The development of the brain. Scientific American, 241, 112133.Google Scholar
Cusick, S. E., & Georgieff, M. K. (2016). The role of nutrition in brain development: The golden opportunity of the “first 1000 days.” Journal of Pediatrics, 175, 1621.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dahl, M. (2016). 5 big ways kindergarten has changed since the 1990s. The Cut, June 21. www.thecut.com/2016/06/5-big-ways-kindergarten-has-changed-since-the-1990s.html, retrieved May 10, 2021.Google Scholar
Dahl, R. E., Allen, N. B., Wilbrecht, L., & Suleiman, A. B. (2018). Importance of investing in adolescence from a developmental science perspective. Nature, 554, 441450.Google Scholar
Dai, J., & Scherb, K. S. (2019). Puberty and functional brain development in humans: Convergence of findings? Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 39, 100690.Google Scholar
Dalziel, K. M., Halliday, D., & Segal, L. (2015). Assessment of the cost-benefit literature on early childhood education for vulnerable children: What the findings mean for policy. SAGE Open, 5, 114.Google Scholar
Davey, L. (Ed.) (2009). Special issue: Framing youth development for public support. New Directions for Youth Development, no. 124.Google Scholar
Davis, E. P., & Thompson, R. A. (2014). Prenatal foundations: Fetal programming of health and development. Zero to Three Journal, 34, 611.Google Scholar
de Boysson-Bardies, B., Halle, P., Sagart, L., & Durand, C. (1989). A crosslinguistic investigation of vowel formants in babbling. Journal of Child Language, 16, 117.Google Scholar
Dekaban, A. S., & Sadowsky, D. (1978). Changes in brain weights during the span of human life: Relation of brain weights to body heights and body weights. Annals of Neurology, 4, 345356.Google Scholar
Dickens, C. (1848). Dombey and son. London: Bradbury & Evans.Google Scholar
Dmitrieva, J., Steinberg, L., & Belsky, J. (2007). Child-care history, classroom composition, and children’s functioning in kindergarten. Psychological Science, 18, 10321039.Google Scholar
Do, K. T., McCormick, E. M., & Telzer, E. H. (2020). Neural sensitivity to conflicting attitudes supports greater conformity toward positive over negative influence in early adolescence. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 45, 100837.Google Scholar
Drake, S. R. (2008). It’s all about the children: An ethnographic study of the First 5 Children and Families Commission of California, unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California, Santa Barbara. Available at www.proquest.com/dissertations-theses/all-about-children-ethnographic-study-first-5/docview/304663954/se-2?accountid=14505, retrieved June 1, 2022.Google Scholar
Duerksen, S. (1997). Researchers unsure about link between music and IQ. San Diego Union-Tribune, June 11, p. E-1.Google Scholar
Duncan, G. J., & Hoynes, H. (2021). Reducing child poverty can promote children’s development and productivity in adulthood. SRCD Child Evidence Brief, no. 11, October. www.srcd.org/sites/default/files/resources/CEB-Child%20Tax%20Credits_final.pdf, retrieved September 21, 2022.Google Scholar
Duncan, G. J., & Magnuson, K. (2013). Investing in preschool programs. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 27, 109132.Google Scholar
Duncan, G. J., Ziol-Guest, K. M., & Kalil, A. (2010). Early-childhood poverty and adult attainment, behavior, and health. Child Development, 81, 306325.Google Scholar
Dunkel Schetter, C., & Tanner, L. (2012). Anxiety, depression and stress in pregnancy: Implications for mothers, children, research, and practice. Current Opinion Psychiatry, 25, 141148.Google Scholar
Dutil, C., Walsh, J. J., Featherstone, R. B., Gunnell, K. E., Tremblay, M. S., Gruber, R., Weiss, S. K., Cote, K. A., Sampson, M., & Chaput, J.-P. (2018). Influence of sleep on developing brain functions and structures in children and adolescents: A systematic review. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 42, 184201.Google Scholar
Eliot, K. (2009). Pink brain, blue brain: How small differences grow into troublesome gaps – And what we can do about it. New York: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.Google Scholar
Eliot, L., Ahmed, A., Khan, H., & Patel, J. (2021). Dump the “dimorphism”: Comprehensive synthesis of human brain studies reveals few male-female differences beyond size. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 125, 667697.Google Scholar
Elman, J., Bates, E., Johnson, M., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Parisi, D., & Plunkett, K. (1996). Rethinking innateness: A connectionist perspective on development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Emerson, S. (2019). California’s First 5 programs evolve as smoking declines and tobacco taxes go away. Daily Democrat (Woodland, CA), January 3. www.dailydemocrat.com/2019/01/03/californias-first-5-programs-evolve-as-smoking-declines-and-tobacco-taxes-go-away/, retrieved June 1, 2022.Google Scholar
Enright, R. D., Levy, V. M. Jr., Harris, D., & Lapsley, D. K. (1987). Do economic conditions influence how theorists view adolescents? Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 16, 541559.Google Scholar
Epstein, B. (1997). Stimulation in child’s early years crucial to development. Tampa Bay Times, July 16, p. 3D.Google Scholar
Erard, M., Kendall-Taylor, N., Davey, L., & Simon, A. (2010). The power of levelness: Making child mental health visible and concrete through a simplifying model: A FrameWorks research report. Washington, DC: FrameWorks Institute. www.frameworksinstitute.org/publication/the-power-of-levelness-making-child-mental-health-visible-and-concrete-through-a-simplifying-model/, retrieved July 25, 2022.Google Scholar
Erickson, K. I., Voss, M. W., Prakash, R. S., et al. (2011). Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory. PNAS, 108, 30173022.Google Scholar
Eriksson, P. S., Perfilieva, E., Bjork-Eriksson, T., Alborn, A.-M., Nordborg, C., Peterson, D. A., & Gage, F. H. (1998). Neurogenesis in the adult human hippocampus. Nature Medicine, 4, 13131317.Google Scholar
Euston, D. R., Gruber, A. J., & McNaughton, B. L. (2012). The role of medial prefrontal cortex in memory and decision making. Neuron, 76, 10571070.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, G. W., Chen, E., Miller, G. E., & Seeman, T. (2012). How poverty gets under the skin: A life course perspective. In King, R. & Mahalmes, V. (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Poverty and Child Development (pp. 1336). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Evans, G. W., & Kim, P. (2013). Childhood poverty, chronic stress, self-regulation, and coping. Child Development Perspectives, 7, 4348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fasick, F. A. (1994), On the “invention of adolescence.” Journal of Early Adolescence, 14, 623.Google Scholar
Fields, G. (2009). White House czar calls for end to “war on drugs.” Wall Street Journal, May 14. www.wsj.com/articles/SB124225891527617397, retrieved August 17, 2022.Google Scholar
First 5 Center for Children’s Policy (2020). Reading our state: How kindergarten readiness inventories can benefit California. https://first5center.org/assets/files/kri-paper-v6-WEB.pdf, retrieved June 13, 2022.Google Scholar
First 5 San Joaquin (2013). Building blocks for school success: Findings from a 5-year longitudinal study. Executive summary. https://harderco.com/wp-content/uploads/2016/08/First5_SJ_Building-Blocks-For-School-Success_Feb_2013.pdf, retrieved June 3, 2022.Google Scholar
Flavell, J. H. (1963). The developmental psychology of Jean Piaget. Princeton, NJ: D. Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1983). The modularity of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Forman-Katz, N., & Matsa, K. E. (2022). News platform fact sheet. Washington, DC: Pew Research Center. www.pewresearch.org/journalism/fact-sheet/news-platform-fact-sheet/, retrieved January 15, 2023.Google Scholar
FrameWorks Institute (2005). Talking early child development and exploring the consequences of frame choices: A FrameWorks message memo. Washington, DC: FrameWorks Institute. www.frameworksinstitute.org/publication/talking-early-child-development-and-exploring-the-consequences-of-frame-choices/, retrieved July 31, 2022.Google Scholar
FrameWorks Institute (2009). Talking to business leaders about early childhood development: A FrameWorks message brief. Washington, DC: FrameWorks Institute. www.frameworksinstitute.org/wp-content/uploads/2020/03/ecd_business_leaders_brief.pdf, retrieved July 25, 2022.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1899). The interpretation of dreams (Die Traumdeutung). Translated by James Strachey. London: Hogarth, 1953.Google Scholar
Friedman, M. (2000). Results accountability for Prop 10 commissions: A planning guide for improving the well-being of young children and their families. Unpublished paper prepared for the Center for Healthier Children, Families and Communities, University of California, Los Angeles. www.impactstrategist.com/wp-content/uploads/2015/12/Prop10GuideV4.pdf, retrieved January 14, 2023.Google Scholar
Friedman-Kraus, A. H., Barnett, W. S., Garver, K. A., Hodges, K. S., Weisenfeld, G. G., Gardiner, B. A., & Jost, T. M. (2022). The state of preschool 2021. New Brunswick, NJ: National Institute for Early Education Research. https://nieer.org/wp-content/uploads/2022/04/YB2021_Full_Report.pdf, retrieved July 3, 2022.Google Scholar
Funk, C. (2020). Key findings about Americans’ confidence in society and their views on scientists’ role in society. Washington, DC: Pew Research Center. www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2020/02/12/key-findings-about-americans-confidence-in-science-and-their-views-on-scientists-role-in-society/, retrieved May 25, 2020.Google Scholar
Gaskins, S., Beeghly, M., Bard, K. A., Gernhardt, A., Liu, C. H., Teti, D. M., Thompson, R. A., Weisner, T. S., & Yovsi, R. D. (2017). Meaning and methods in the study and assessment of attachment. In Keller, H. & Bard, K. A. (Eds.), The cultural nature of attachment (pp. 195229). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gaylor, E., Spiker, D., Williamson, C., & Ferguson, K. (2011). Saint Paul Early Childhood Scholarship evaluation: Final Evaluation Report – 2008–2011. Menlo Park, CA: SRI International. https://hubert.hhh.umn.edu/ECEpdf/Scholarships2008-2011FinalFullReport.pdf, retrieved July 5, 2022.Google Scholar
Geldhof, G. J., Bowers, E. P., Mueller, M. K., Napolitano, C. M., Callina, K. S., Walsh, K. J., Lerner, J. V., & Lerner, R. M. (2015). The five Cs model of positive youth development. In Bowers, E. P., Geldhof, G. J., Johnson, S. K., Hilliard, L. J., Hershberg, R. M., Lerner, J. V., & Lerner, R. M. (Eds.), Promoting positive youth development (pp. 161186). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Gelman, S. A. (2005). The essential child: Origins of essentialism in everyday thought. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gelman, S. A. (2009). Learning from others: Children’s construction of concepts. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 115140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gelman, S. A., Ware, E. A., Kleinberg, F., Manczak, E. M., & Stilwell, S. M. (2014). Individual differences in children’s and parents’ generic language. Child Development, 85, 924940.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gelman, S. A. & Wellman, H. M. (1991). Insides and essences: Early understandings of the non-obvious. Cognition, 38, 213244.Google Scholar
Gershberg, Z., & Illing, S. (2022). The paradox of democracy: Free speech, open media, and perilous persuasion. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Ghetti, S., & Fandakova, Y. (2020). Neural development of memory and metamemory in childhood and adolescence: Toward an integrative model of the development of episodic recollection. Annual Review of Developmental Psychology, 2, 365388.Google Scholar
Gibson, E. T., & Walker, A. S. (1984). Development of knowledge of visual-tactual affordances of substance. Child Development, 55, 453460.Google Scholar
Giffin, F., & Mitchell, D. E. (1978). The rate of recovery of vision after early monocular deprivation in kittens. Journal of Physiology, 274, 511537.Google Scholar
Gladwell, M. (1998). Do parents matter? The New Yorker, August 17, pp. 52–64.Google Scholar
Gladwell, M. (2000). Baby steps: Do our first three years of life determine how we’ll turn out? New Yorker, January 10, pp. 80–87.Google Scholar
Gladwell, M. (2008). Outliers: The story of success. New York: Little, Brown.Google Scholar
Gluckman, P., & Hanson, M. (2005). The fetal matrix. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Goddings, A.-L., Beltz, A., Peper, J. S., Crone, E. A., & Braams, B. R. (2019). Understanding the role of puberty in structural and functional development of the adolescent brain. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 29, 3253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goffman, E. (1974). Frame analysis. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Gopnik, A. (2009). The philosophical baby. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.Google Scholar
Gopnik, A. (2016). The gardener and the carpenter. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.Google Scholar
Gopnik, A., Meltzoff, A. N., & Kuhl, P. K. (1999). The scientist in the crib: Minds, brains, and how children learn. New York: Morrow.Google Scholar
Gough, M. (Ed.) (2003). Politicizing science. Stanford, CA: Hoover Institution Press.Google Scholar
Gould, E., Beylin, A., Tanapat, P., Reeves, A., & Shors, T. J. (1999). Learning enhances adult neurogenesis in the hippocampal formation. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 260265.Google Scholar
Graham v. Florida, 560 U.S. 48 (2010).Google Scholar
Greenough, W. T. (1997). We can’t focus just on ages 0 to 3. American Psychological Association Monitor, November, p. 19.Google Scholar
Grigorenko, E. (2017). Brain development: The effect of interventions on children and adolescents. In Bundy, D., de Silva, N., Horton, S., Jamison, D. T., & Patton, G. (Eds.), Disease control priorities, Vol. 8: Child and adolescent health and development (pp. 119131). Washington, DC: World Bank.Google Scholar
Gunnar, M. R., & Donzella, B. (2002). Social regulation of the cortisol levels in early human development. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 27, 199220.Google Scholar
Gweon, H., & Saxe, R. (2013). Developmental cognitive neuroscience of theory of mind. In Rubenstein, J. L. R. & Rakic, P. (Eds.), Comprehensive developmental neuroscience: Neural circuit development and function in the healthy and diseased brain (pp. 367377). Amsterdam: Elsevier and Academic Press.Google Scholar
Hackman, D. A., & Farah, M. J. (2009). Socioeconomic status and the developing brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 6573.Google Scholar
Hair, N. L., Hanson, J. L., Wolfe, B. L., & Pollak, S. D. (2015). Association of child poverty, brain development, and academic achievement. JAMA Pediatrics, 169, 822829.Google Scholar
Hall, N. (2014). The Kardashian index: A measure of discrepant social media profile for scientists. Genome Biology, 15, 424.Google Scholar
Hanson, J. L., Hair, N., Shen, D. G., Shi, F., Gilmore, J. H., Wolfe, B. L., & Pollak, S. D. (2013). Family poverty affects the rate of human infant brain growth. PLoS ONE, 8, e80954.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, J. R. (1998). The nurture assumption: Why children turn out the way they do. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Haskins, R., & Margolis, G. (2014). Show me the evidence: Obama’s fight for rigor and results in social policy. Washington, DC: Brookings Institution Press.Google Scholar
Heckman, J. J. (2000). Policies to foster human capital. Research in Economics, 54, 356.Google Scholar
Heckman, J. J. (2004). Lessons from the technology of skill formation. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1038, 179200.Google Scholar
Heckman, J. J. (2006). Skill formation and the economics of investing in disadvantaged children. Science, 312, 19001902.Google Scholar
Heckman, J. J. (2007). The economics, technology, and neuroscience of human capability formation. PNAS, 104, 1325013255.Google Scholar
Heckman, J. J. (2008). Schools, skills, and synapses. Economic Inquiry, 36, 289324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hernandez, A. E., Claussenius-Kalman, H. L., Ronderos, J., Castilla-Earls, A. P., Sun, L., Weiss, S. D., & Young, D. R. (2019). Neuroemergentism: A framework for studying cognition and brain. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 49, 214223.Google Scholar
Hestres, L. E., Rochman, A., Busso, D., & Volmert, A. (2021). How are children’s issues portrayed in the news? A media content analysis. Washington, DC: FrameWorks Institute. www.frameworksinstitute.org/publication/how-are-childrens-issues-portrayed-in-the-news/, retrieved July 25, 2022.Google Scholar
Heyes, C., & Catmur, C. (2022). What happened to mirror neurons? Perspectives in Psychological Science, 17, 153168.Google Scholar
Hines, M. (2020). Neuroscience and sex/gender: Looking back and forward. Journal of Neuroscience, 40, 3743.Google Scholar
Hirsh-Pasek, K., Adamson, L. B., Bakeman, R., Owen, M. T., Golinkoff, R. M., Pace, A., Yust, P. K. S., & Suma, K. (2015). The contribution of early communication quality to low-income children’s language success. Psychological Science, 26, 10711083.Google Scholar
Hodgson v. Minnesota, 497 U.S. 417 (1990).Google Scholar
Hughes, C., & Leekam, S. (2004). What are the links between theory of mind and social relations? Review, reflections and new directions for studies of typical and atypical development. Social Development, 13, 590619.Google Scholar
Huttenlocher, P. R., & Dabholkar, A. S. (1997). Regional differences in synaptogenesis in human cerebral cortex. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 387, 167178.Google Scholar
Hwang, C. P., Lamb, M. E., & Siegel, I. E. (Eds.) (1996). Images of childhood. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Illes, J., Moser, M. A., McCormick, J. B., Racine, E., Blakeslee, S., Caplan, A., Hayden, E. C., Ingram, J., Lohwater, T., McKnight, P., Nicholson, C., Phillips, A., Sauvé, K. D., Snell, E., & Weiss, S. (2010). Neurotalk: Improving the communication of neuroscience research. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 6169.Google Scholar
Iyengar, S. (1991). Is anyone responsible? How television frames political issues. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Iyengar, S., & Kinder, D. R. (1987). News That matters: Television and American opinion. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Jamieson, K. H. (2017). The need for a science of science communication: Communicating science’s values and norms. In Jamieson, K. H., Kahan, D. M., & Scheufele, D. A. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of the science of science communication (pp. 1524). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Jessor, R., Turbin, M. S., Costa, F. M., Dong, Q., Zhang, H., & Wang, C. (2003). Adolescent problem behavior in China and the United States: A cross-national study of psychosocial protective factors. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 13, 329360.Google Scholar
Jobson, D. D., Hase, Y., Clarkson, A. N., & Kalaria, R. H. (2021). The role of the medial prefrontal cortex in cognition, ageing and dementia. Brain Communications, 3, article fcab125.Google Scholar
Johnson, M. H. (2013). Theories in developmental neuroscience. In Rubenstein, J. L. R. & Rakic, P. (Eds.), Neural circuit development and function in the brain (pp. 191205). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Johnson, M. H., & de Haan, M. (2015). Developmental cognitive neuroscience: An introduction (4th Ed.). New York: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Johnson, S. B., Blum, R. W., & Giedd, J. N. (2009). Adolescent maturity and the brain: The promise and pitfalls of neuroscience research in adolescent health policy. Journal of Adolescent Health, 45, 216221.Google Scholar
Jones v. Mississippi, 593 U.S. __ (2021).Google Scholar
Kagan, J. (1998). Three seductive ideas. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Karoly, L. A., Greenwood, P. W., Everingham, S. S., Houbé, J., Kilburn, M. R., Rydell, C. P., Sanders, M., & Chiesa, J. (1998). Investing in our children: What we know and don’t know about the costs and benefits of early childhood interventions. Santa Monica, CA: RAND.Google Scholar
Kendall-Taylor, N. (2009). Conflicting models of mind in mind: Mapping the gaps between the expert and public understandings of child mental health as part of strategic frame analysis: A FrameWorks research report. Washington, DC: FrameWorks Institute. www.frameworksinstitute.org/wp-content/uploads/2020/03/childmentalhealthculturalmodels.pdf, retrieved January 14, 2023.Google Scholar
Kendall-Taylor, N., & Stevens, A. (2017). Can frames make change? Using communications science to translate the science of child mental health. In Maurer, M. H. (Ed.), Child and adolescent mental health (pp. 5971). London: Intech Open. www.intechopen.com/books/5470, retrieved July 25, 2022.Google Scholar
Kennedy, B., Tyson, A., & Funk, C. (2022). Americans’ trust in scientists, other groups declines. Washington, DC: Pew Research Center. www.pewresearch.org/science/2022/02/15/americans-trust-in-scientists-other-groups-declines/, retrieved November 16, 2022.Google Scholar
Kim, I.-K., & Spelke, E. S. (1992). Infants’ sensitivity to effects of gravity on visible object motion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 33853393.Google Scholar
Kim, I.-K., & Spelke, E. S. (1999). Perception and understanding of effects of gravity and inertia on object motion. Developmental Science, 2, 339362.Google Scholar
Klein, J. (1997). Clintons on the brain. New Yorker, March 17, pp. 59–63. www.newyorker.com/magazine/1997/03/17/clintons-on-the-brain, retrieved January 14, 2023.Google Scholar
Knitzer, J., & Adely, F. (2001). Executive summary: Learning from Starting Points: Findings from the Starting Points Assessment Project. New York: National Center for Children in Poverty, Columbia University. https://academiccommons.columbia.edu/doi/10.7916/D8QR55V9, retrieved May 23, 2022.Google Scholar
Kolb, B. (1989). Brain development, plasticity, and behavior. American Psychologist, 44, 12031212.Google Scholar
Kopp, F., & Lindenberger, U. (2011). Effects of joint attention on long-term memory in 9-month-old infants: An event-related potentials study. Developmental Science, 14, 660672.Google Scholar
Kotulak, R. (1996), Inside the brain: Revolutionary discoveries of how the mind works. Kansas City, MO: Andrews and McMeel.Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. K., Tsao, F.-M., & Liu, H.-M. (2003). Foreign-language experience in infancy: Effects of short-term exposure and social interaction on phonetic learning. PNAS, 100, 90969101.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. (2002). Moral politics: How liberals and conservatives think (2nd Ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G., & Grady, J. (1998). Why early ed benefits all of us. In Bales, S. N. (Ed.), Effective language for discussing early childhood education and policy (pp. 719). Washington, DC: Benton Foundation.Google Scholar
Lange-Kubick, C. (2010). A new life for Amber. Lincoln Journal-Star, January 14, p. A1. https://journalstar.com/news/local/a-new-life-for-amber/article_78afdaea-06e9-11df-a7c6-001cc4c03286.html, retrieved September 7, 2022.Google Scholar
Lansford, J. E., Zietz, S., Bornstein, M. H., Deater-Deckard, K., et al. (2020). Opportunities and peer support for aggression and delinquency during adolescence in nine countries. New Directions in Child and Adolescent Development, 172, 7388.Google Scholar
Lany, J., & Saffran, J. R. (2013). Statistical learning mechanisms in infancy. In Rubenstein, J. L. R. & Rakic, P. (Eds.), Comprehensive developmental neuroscience: Neural circuit development and function in the healthy and diseased brain (pp. 231248). Amsterdam: Elsevier and Academic Press.Google Scholar
Larsen, B., & Luna, B. (2018). Adolescence as a neurobiological critical period for the development of higher-order cognition. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 94, 179195.Google Scholar
Leong, Y. C., Chen, J., Willer, R., & Zaki, J. (2020). Conservative and liberal attitudes drive polarized neural responses to political content. PNAS, 117, 2773127739.Google Scholar
Lesher, A. I. (1997). Addiction is a brain disease, and it matters. Science, 278, 4547.Google Scholar
Levine, M. H. & Smith, S. V. (2001). Starting points: State and community partnerships for young children. The Future of Children, 11, 142149.Google Scholar
Levine, R., & LeVine, S. (2016). Do parents matter? New York: Public Affairs.Google Scholar
Lewin, K. (1943). Psychology and the process of group living. Journal of Social Psychology, 17, 113131.Google Scholar
Lewin, T. (2009). No Einstein in your crib? Get a refund. New York Times, October 23, section A, p. 1. www.nytimes.com/2009/10/24/education/24baby.html, retrieved January 6, 2023.Google Scholar
L’Hôte, E., & Volmert, A. (2021). Why aren’t kids a policy priority? The cultural mindsets and attitudes that keep kids off the public agenda: A FrameWorks strategic brief. Washington, DC: FrameWorks Institute. www.frameworksinstitute.org/publication/why-arent-kids-a-policy-priority-the-cultural-mindsets-and-attitudes-that-keep-kids-off-the-public-agenda/, retrieved July 31, 2022.Google Scholar
Lieberman, Z., Woodward, A. L., & Kinzler, K. D. (2017). The origins of social categorization. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21, 556568.Google Scholar
Lindblom, C. E., & Cohen, D. K. (1979). Usable knowledge: Social science and social problem-solving. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Lindquist, K. A., Wager, T. D., Koger, H., Bliss-Moreau, E., & Barrett, L. F. (2012). The brain basis of emotion: A meta-analytic review. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35, 121202.Google Scholar
Locke, J. (1693). Some thoughts concerning education. London: Churchill.Google Scholar
Luby, J., Belden, A., Botteron, K., Marrus, N., Harris, M. P., Babb, C., Nishino, T., & Barch, D. (2013). The effects of poverty on childhood brain development: The mediating effect of caregiving and stressful life events. JAMA Pediatrics, 167, 11351142.Google Scholar
Luciana, M. (2013). Adolescent brain development in normality and psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 25, 13251345.Google Scholar
Lumey, L. H., Stein, A. D., Kahn, H. S., van der Pal-de Bruin, K. M., Blauw, G. J., Zybert, P. A., & Susser, E. S.(2007). Cohort profile: The Dutch Hunger Winter Families Study. International Journal of Epidemiology, 36, 11971204.Google Scholar
Lutz, C. (1988). Unnatural emotions: Everyday sentiments on a Micronesian atoll and their challenge to Western theory. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Mann, J. (1994). Children in crisis. Washington Post, April 15, p. E3.Google Scholar
Manuel, T. (2009). Refining the core story of early childhood development: The effects of science and health frames: A FrameWorks research report. Washington, DC: FrameWorks Institute. www.frameworksinstitute.org/publication/refining-the-core-story-of-early-childhood-development-the-effects-of-science-and-health-frames/, retrieved August 19, 2022.Google Scholar
Manuel, T., & Gilliam, F. D. Jr. (2009). Advancing support for child mental health policies: Early results from Strategic Frame Analysis experimental research: A FrameWorks research report. Washington, DC: FrameWorks Institute. www.frameworksinstitute.org/publication/advancing-support-for-child-mental-health-policies-early-results-from-strategic-frame-analysis-experimental-research/, retrieved July 25, 2022.Google Scholar
Marcus, G. (2004). The birth of the mind: How a tiny number of genes creates the complexities of human thought. New York: Basic.Google Scholar
Maroney, T. A. (2013). Adolescent brain science after Graham v. Florida. Notre Dame Law Review, 86, 765793.Google Scholar
McCabe, D. P., & Castel, A. D. (2008). Seeing is believing: The effect of brain images on judgments of scientific reasoning. Cognition, 107, 343352.Google Scholar
McCoy, D. C., Yoshikawa, H., Ziol-Guest, K. M., Duncan, G. J., Schindler, H. S., Magnuson, K., Yang, R., Koepp, A., & Shonkoff, J. P. (2017). Impacts of early childhood education on medium- and long-term educational outcomes. Educational Researcher, 46, 474487.Google Scholar
McLuhan, M (1964). Understanding media: The extensions of man. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Meins, E. (2013). Sensitive attunement to infants’ internal states: Operationalizing the construct of mind-mindedness. Attachment & Human Development, 15, 524544.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. (1995). Understanding the intentions of others: Re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Developmental Psychology, 31, 838850.Google Scholar
Michael, R. B., Newman, E. J., Vuorre, M., Cumming, G., & Gerry, M. (2013). On the (non)persuasive power of a brain image. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20, 720725.Google Scholar
Miller v. Alabama, 567 U.S. 460 (2012).Google Scholar
Miller, T. L., Volmert, A., Rochman, A., & Aassar, M. (2021). Talking about poverty: Narratives, counter-narratives, and telling effective stories. Washington, D.C.: FrameWorks Institute. www.frameworksinstitute.org/publication/talking-about-poverty-narratives-counter-narratives-and-telling-effective-stories/x, retrieved September 27, 2022.Google Scholar
Mills, D. L., Coffey-Corina, S. A., & Neville, H. J. (1993). Language acquisition and cerebral specialization in 20-month-old infants. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 317334.Google Scholar
Mills, K. L., Lalonde, F., Clasen, L. S.,, Giedd, J. N., & Blakemore, S.-J. (2014). Developmental changes in the structure of the social brain in late childhood and adolescence. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9, 123131.Google Scholar
Mitchell, A., Jurkowitz, M., Oliphant, J. B., & Shearer, E. (2020). Americans who mainly get their news on social media are less engaged, less knowledgeable. Washington, DC: Pew Research Center. www.pewresearch.org/journalism/2020/07/30/americans-who-mainly-get-their-news-on-social-media-are-less-engaged-less-knowledgeable/, retrieved September 28, 2022.Google Scholar
Moffitt, T. (2006). Life-course persistent versus adolescence-limited antisocial behavior. In Cicchetti, D. & Cohen, D. (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology, Vol. 3: Risk, disorder, and adaptation (2nd ed.) (pp. 570598). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Monk, C., Lugo-Candelas, C., & Trumpff, C. (2019). Prenatal developmental origins of future psychopathology: Mechanisms and pathways. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 15, 16.116.28.Google Scholar
Monterosso, J., Royzman, E. B., & Schwartz, B. (2005). Explaining away responsibility: Effect of scientific explanation on perceived culpability. Ethics & Behavior, 15, 139158.Google Scholar
Monterosso, J., & Schwartz, B. (2012). Did your brain make you do it? New York Times, July 29, section SR, p. 12. www.nytimes.com/2012/07/29/opinion/sunday/neuroscience-and-moral-responsibility.html, retrieved September 19, 2012.Google Scholar
Montgomery v. Louisiana, 577 U.S. __ (2016).Google Scholar
Moriguchi, Y. (2014). The early development of executive function and its relation to social interaction: A brief review. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, article 388.Google Scholar
Morimoto, K., & Nakajima, K. (2019). Role of the immune system in the development of the central nervous system. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 13, article 916.Google Scholar
Moshman, D. (2011). Adolescent rationality and development (3rd Ed.). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Mulders, P., lera, A., Tendolkar, I., van Eijndhoven, P., & Beckmann, C. (2018). Personality profiles are associated with functional brain networks related to cognition and emotion. Scientific Reports, 8, 13874.Google Scholar
Müller, U., & Kerns, K. (2015). The development of executive function. In Liben, L. S. & Müller, U. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science, Vol. 2: Cognitive processes (7th Ed.) (pp. 153). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Munakata, Y., Snyder, H. R., & Chatham, C. H. (2012). Developing cognitive control: Three key transitions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 7177.Google Scholar
Nachmias, M., Gunnar, M., Mangelsdorf, S., Parrit, R. H., & Buss, K. (1996). Behavioral inhibition and stress reactivity: The moderating role of attachment security. Child Development, 67, 508522.Google Scholar
Nash, J. M. (1997). Fertile minds. Time, February 3, pp. 48–56.Google Scholar
National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine, Committee on Building an Agenda to Reduce the Number of Children in Poverty by Half in 10 Years (2019). A roadmap to reducing child poverty. Washington, DC: National Academies Press. https://nap.nationalacademies.org/catalog/25246/a-roadmap-to-reducing-child-poverty, retrieved April 27, 2023.Google Scholar
National Commission on Excellence in Education (1983). A nation at risk: The imperative for educational reform. Washington, DC: US Government Printing Office. https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED226006.pdf, retrieved July 15, 2022.Google Scholar
National Research Council (2012). From neurons to neighborhoods: An update: Workshop summary. Washington, DC: National Academies Press.Google Scholar
National Research Council and Institute of Medicine (2000). From neurons to neighborhoods: The science of early childhood development. Committee on Integrating the Science of Early Childhood Development. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.Google Scholar
National Research Council and Institute of Medicine (2015). Transforming the workforce for children birth through age 8: A unifying foundation. Committee on the Science of Children Birth to Age 8: Deepening and Broadening the Foundation for Success. Washington, DC: National Academies Press.Google Scholar
National Scientific Council on the Developing Child (2004a). Young children develop in an environment of relationships. Working Paper no. 1. https://developingchild.harvard.edu/resources/wp1/, retrieved August 2, 2022.Google Scholar
National Scientific Council on the Developing Child (2004b). Children’s emotional development is built into the architecture of their brains. Working Paper no. 2, https://developingchild.harvard.edu/resources/childrens-emotional-development-is-built-into-the-architecture-of-their-brains/, retrieved August 2, 2022.Google Scholar
National Scientific Council on the Developing Child (2005). Excessive stress disrupts the architecture of the developing brain. Working Paper no. 3. https://developingchild.harvard.edu/resources/wp3/, retrieved August 2, 2022.Google Scholar
National Scientific Council on the Developing Child (2007). The science of early childhood development: Closing the gap between what we know and what we do. https://developingchild.harvard.edu/resources/the-science-of-early-childhood-development-closing-the-gap-between-what-we-know-and-what-we-do/, retrieved August 3, 2022.Google Scholar
National Scientific Council on the Developing Child (2014). A decade of science informing policy: The story of the National Scientific Council on the Developing Child. https://developingchild.harvard.edu/science/national-scientific-council-on-the-developing-child/, retrieved August 3, 2022.Google Scholar
Nelson, C. A., de Haan, M., & Thomas, K. M. (2006). Neuroscience and cognitive development. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Nelson, C. A., & Webb, S. J. (2003). A cognitive neuroscience perspective on early memory development. In de Haan, M. & Johnson, M. H. (Eds.), The cognitive neuroscience of development (pp. 99126). Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network (1997). The effects of infant child care on infant-mother attachment security: Results of the NICHD Study of Early Child Care. Child Development, 68, 860879.Google Scholar
O’Donnell, K. J., Glover, V., Barker, E. D., & O’Connor, T. G. (2014). The persisting effect of maternal mood in pregnancy on child psychopathology. Development & Psychopathology, 26, 393403.Google Scholar
Olds, D. L., Sadler, L., & Kitzman, H. (2007). Programs for parents of infants and toddlers: Recent evidence from randomized trials. Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 355391.Google Scholar
Parker, E., Diffey, L., & Atchison, B. (2018). How states fund pre-K: A primer for policymakers. Denver, CO: Education Commission of the States. www.ecs.org/wp-content/uploads/How-States-Fund-Pre-K_A-Primer-for-Policymakers.pdf, retrieved May 10, 2021.Google Scholar
Parker, K. (1997). Babies’ brains and government schemes. Orlando Sentinel, April 27.Google Scholar
Patoine, B. (2005). Mental retardation: Struggle, stigma, science. Cerebrum (publication of the Dana Foundation). https://dana.org/article/mental-retardation-struggle-stigma-science/, retrieved February 16, 2002.Google Scholar
Paul, A. M. (2021). The extended mind: The power of thinking outside the brain. New York: Mariner Books.Google Scholar
Phillips, D. A., Lipsey, M. W., Dodge, K. A., Haskins, R., Bassok, D., Burchinal, M. R., Duncan, G. J., Dynarski, M., Magnuson, K. A., & Weiland, C. (2017). Puzzling it out: The current state of scientific knowledge on pre-kindergarten effects: A consensus statement. www.brookings.edu/wp-content/uploads/2017/04/consensus-statement_final.pdf, retrieved June 20, 2022.Google Scholar
Pietschnig, J., Voracek, M., & Formann, A. K. (2010). Mozart effect – Schmozart effect: A meta-analysis. Intelligence, 38, 314323.Google Scholar
Poldrack, R. A. (2006). Can cognitive processes be inferred from neuroimaging data? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 5963.Google Scholar
Poldrack, R. A. (2011). Inferring mental states from neuroimaging data: From reverse inference to large-scale decoding. Neuron, 72, 692697.Google Scholar
Porter, T., Molina, D. C., Cimpian, A., Roberts, S., Fredericks, A., Blackwell, L. S., & Trzesniewski, K. (2022). Growth-mindset intervention delivered by teachers boosts achievement in early adolescence. Psychological Science, 33, 10861096.Google Scholar
Posner, J., Cha, J., Roy, A. K., Peterson, B. S., Bansal, R., Gustafsson, H. C., Raffanello, E., Gingrich, J., & Monk, C. (2016). Alterations in amygdala-prefrontal circuits in infants exposed to prenatal maternal depression. Translational Psychiatry, 6, e395.Google Scholar
Postle, B. R. (2006). Working memory as an emergent property of the mind and brain. Neuroscience, 139, 2338.Google Scholar
Poulin-Dubois, D., Lepage, A., & Ferland, D. (1996). Infants’ concept of animacy. Cognitive Development, 11, 1936.Google Scholar
Pyle, A. (1999). Setting the pace for windfall. Los Angeles Times, November 25, section A, pp. 46–48.Google Scholar
Qu, Y., Jorgensen, N. A., & Telzer, E. H. (2020). A call for greater attention to culture in the study of brain and development. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 16, 275293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Qu, Y., Pomerantz, E. M., McCormick, E., & Telzer, E. H. (2018). Youth’s conceptions of adolescence predict longitudinal changes in prefrontal cortex activation and risk taking during adolescence. Child Development, 89, 773783.Google Scholar
Qu, Y., Pomerantz, E. M., Wang, Q., & Ng, F.-Y. (2019). Early adolescents’ stereotypes about teens in Hong Kong and Chongqing: Reciprocal pathways with problem behavior. Developmental Psychology, 56, 10921106.Google Scholar
Racine, E., Bar-Ilan, O., & Illes, J. (2005). fMRI in the public eye. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 159164.Google Scholar
Rea, D., & Burton, T. (2020). New evidence on the Heckman curve. Journal of Economic Surveys, 43, 241262.Google Scholar
Reiner, P. B. (2011). The rise of neuroessentialism. In Illes, J. & Sahakian, B. J. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of neuroethics (pp. 161175). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Repacholi, B. M., & Gopnik, A. (1997). Early reasoning about desires: Evidence from 14- and 18-month-olds. Developmental Psychology, 33, 1221.Google Scholar
Research and Policy Committee, Committee for Economic Development (2002). Preschool for all: Investing in a productive and just society. New York: Committee for Economic Development. www.ced.org/pdf/Preschool-for-All.pdf, retrieved July 12, 2022.Google Scholar
Reynolds, A. J., Temple, J. A., White, B. A., Ou, S.-R., & Robertson, D. L. (2011). Age-26 cost-benefit analysis of the Child-Parent Center Early Education Program. Child Development, 82, 379404.Google Scholar
Rhodes, M., & Baron, A. (2019). The development of social categorization. Annual Review of Developmental Psychology, 1, 359386.Google Scholar
Rivera, C. (2002). Veteran educator to head county’s preschool effort. Los Angeles Times, November 15.Google Scholar
Rivera‐Gaxiola, M., Silva‐Pereyra, J., & Kuhl, P. K. (2005). Brain potentials to native and non‐native speech contrasts in 7‐ and 11‐month‐old American infants. Developmental Science, 8, 162172.Google Scholar
Rolnick, A., & Grunewald, R. (2003). Early childhood development: Economic development with a high public return. Minneapolis, MN: Federal Reserve Bank of Minneapolis. www.minneapolisfed.org/publications_papers/studies/earlychild/abc-part2.pdf, retrieved July 5, 2022.Google Scholar
Romer, D. (2010). Adolescent risk taking, impulsivity, and brain development: Implications for prevention. Developmental Psychobiology, 52, 263276.Google Scholar
Romer, D., Duckworth, A. L., Sznitman, S., & Park, S. (2010). Can adolescents learn self-control? Delay of gratification in the development of control over risk taking. Prevention Science, 11, 319330.Google Scholar
Romer, D., Reyna, V. F., & Satterthwaite, T. D. (2017). Beyond stereotypes of adolescent risk taking: Placing the adolescent brain in developmental context. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 27, 1934.Google Scholar
Roper v. Simmons, 543 U.S. 551 (2005).Google Scholar
Rose, H., & Rose, S. (2016). Can neuroscience change our minds? Cambridge: Polity Press.Google Scholar
Rousseau, J. J. (1762). Emile, or Treatise on education (Emile, ou De l’education), edited and translated by Bloom., Alan New York: Basic Books, 1979.Google Scholar
Royal Society (2011). Neuroscience: Implications for education and lifelong learning. London: Royal Society. https://royalsociety.org/~/media/Royal_Society_Content/policy/publications/2011/4294975733.pdf, retrieved July 6, 2022.Google Scholar
Rymer, R. (1994). Genie: A scientific tragedy (2nd Ed.). New York: Harper Perennial.Google Scholar
Saffran, J. R., & Kirkham, N. Z. (2018). Infant statistical learning. Annual Review of Psychology, 69, 181203.Google Scholar
Santrock, J. W. (2021). Life-span development (18th Ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Schachter, D. L., & Addis, D. R. (2007). The optimistic brain. Nature Neuroscience, 10, 13451347.Google Scholar
Schäfer, M. S. (2017). How changing media structures are affecting science news coverage. In Jamieson, K. H., Kahan, D. M., & Scheufele, D. A. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of the science of science communication (pp. 5160). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Schmidt, S. N. L., Hass, J., Kirsch, P., & Mier, D. (2021). The human mirror neuron system – A common neural basis for social cognition? Psychophysiology, 58, e13781.Google Scholar
Schoenfeld, T., & Gould, E. (2012). Stress, stress hormones, and adult neurogenesis. Experimental Neurology, 233, 1221.Google Scholar
Schulz, L. C. (2010). The Dutch Hunger Winter and the developmental origins of health and disease. PNAS, 107, 1675716758.Google Scholar
Schwartz, J. M., & Beyette, B. (1996). Brain lock. New York: HarperCollins.Google Scholar
Schweinhart, L. J., Montie, J., Xiang, Z., Barnett, W. S., Belfield, C. R., & Nores, M. (2005). Lifetime effects: The High/Scope Perry Preschool Study through age 40. Ypsilanti, MI: High/Scope Press.Google Scholar
Seifer, R., & Parade, S. H. (2020). Teratology. In Benson, J. B. & Haith, M. M. (Eds.), Encyclopedia of infant and early childhood development (2nd Ed.), Vol. 3 (pp. 344355). New York: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Shen, F. X. (2013). Legislating neuroscience: The case of juvenile justice. Loyola of Los Angeles Law Review, 46, 9851018.Google Scholar
Shonkoff, J. P. (2022). Re-envisioning early childhood policy and practice in a world of striking inequality and uncertainty. Center on the Developing Child, Harvard University. https://developingchild.harvard.edu/re-envisioning-ecd/ retrieved January 8, 2023.Google Scholar
Shonkoff, J. P., & Bales, S. N. (2011). Science does not speak for itself: Translating child development research for the public and its policymakers. Child Development, 82, 1732.Google Scholar
Shore, R. (1997). Rethinking the brain: New insights into early development. New York: Families and Work Institute.Google Scholar
Shulman, E. P., Smith, A. R., Silva, K., Icenogle, G., Duell, N., Chein, J., & Steinberg, L. (2016). The dual systems model: Review, reappraisal, and reaffirmation. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 103117.Google Scholar
Sims, M. (2017). Neoliberalism and early childhood. Cogent Education, 4, 110.Google Scholar
Sisk, C. L. (2017). Development: Pubertal hormones meet the adolescent brain. Current Biology, 27, R706R709.Google Scholar
Skinner, A. L., Olson, K. R., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2020). Acquiring group bias: Observing other people’s nonverbal signals can create social group biases. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology: Interpersonal Relations and Group Processes, 119, 824838.Google Scholar
Skolnick, D. (2005). The sweetest baby milestones. Parents, 80, 134139.Google Scholar
Smith, L. (1998). Directing a crusade. Los Angeles Times, February 25, pp. E1–E6. www.latimes.com/archives/la-xpm-1998-feb-25-ls-22611-story.html, retrieved January 14, 2023.Google Scholar
Smith, R., Lane, R. D., Alkozei, A., Bao, J., Smith, C., Sanova, A., Nettles, M., & Killgore, W. D. S. (2018). The role of medial prefrontal cortex in the working memory maintenance of one’s own emotional responses. Scientific Reports, 8, 3460.Google Scholar
Smuts, A. B. (2006). Science in the service of children, 1893–1935. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Spelke, E. S. (1990). Principles of object perception. Cognitive Science, 14, 2956.Google Scholar
Spelke, E. S., & Kinzler, K. D. (2007). Core knowledge. Developmental Science, 10, 8996.Google Scholar
Stahl, A. E., & Feigenson, L. (2015). Observing the unexpected enhances infants’ learning and exploration. Science, 348, 9194.Google Scholar
Stanford v. Kentucky, 492 U.S. 361 (1989).Google Scholar
Steinberg, L. (2010). A dual systems model of adolescent risk-taking. Developmental Psychobiology, 52, 216224.Google Scholar
Steinberg, L. (2011). Should the science of adolescent brain development inform public policy? Henry and Bryna David Lecture, National Academy of Sciences, Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Steinberg, L. (2020). Adolescence (12th Ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Steinberg, L., Albert, D., Cauffman, E., & Banich, M. T. (2008). Age differences in sensation seeking and impulsivity as indexed by behavior and self-report: Evidence for a dual systems model. Developmental Psychology, 44, 17641778.Google Scholar
Steinberg, L., Cauffman, E., Woolard, J., Graham, S., & Banich, M. (2009). Are adolescents less mature than adults? Minors’ access to abortion, the juvenile death penalty, and the alleged APA “flip-flop.” American Psychologist, 64, 583594.Google Scholar
Steinberg, L., Icenogle, G., Shulman, E. P., Chein, J., et al. (2018). Around the world, adolescence is a time of heightened sensation seeking and immature self-regulation. Developmental Science, 21, e12532.Google Scholar
Steiner, P. (2020). Brain fuel utilization in the developing brain. Annals of Nutrition and Metabolism, 75, 818.Google Scholar
Sterck, E. (2010). Theory of mind: Specialized capacity or emergent property? European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7, 116.Google Scholar
Stevens, K. B., & Weidinger, M. (2021). Improving early childhood development by allowing advanced child tax credits. Washington, DC: American Enterprise Institute. www.aei.org/articles/improving-early-childhood-development-by-allowing-advanced-child-tax-credits/, retrieved September 10, 2022.Google Scholar
Stiles, J. (2008). The fundamentals of brain development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Stone, L. J., Smith, H. T., & Murphy, L. (1974). The competent infant: Research and commentary. New York: Basic.Google Scholar
Suskind, D., & Denworth, L. (2022). U.S. kids are falling behind global competition, but brain science shows how to catch up. Scientific American, 326, 4853. www.scientificamerican.com/article/u-s-kids-are-falling-behind-global-competition-but-brain-science-shows-how-to-catch-up/, retrieved July 6, 2022.Google Scholar
Tabor, M. B. W. (1997). Actor-director focusing on children. New York Times, August 6, sec. B, p. 8. www.nytimes.com/1997/08/06/us/actor-director-focusing-on-children.html, retrieved January 14, 2023.Google Scholar
Tavris, C. (1999) The myth of the first three years. New York Times Book Review, October 17.Google Scholar
Taylor, M. G., Rhodes, M., & Gelman, S. A. (2009). Boys will be boys; cows will be cows: Children’s essentialist reasoning about gender categories and animal species. Child Development, 80, 461481.Google Scholar
Telzer, E. H., Fuligni, A. J., Lieberman, M. D., & Galvan, A. (2013). Meaningful family relationships: Neurocognitive buffers of adolescent risk taking. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25, 374387.Google Scholar
Temple, E., Deutsch, G. K., Poldrack, R. A., Miller, S. L., Tallal, P., Merzenich, M. M., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2003). Neural deficits in children with dyslexia ameliorated by behavioral remediation: Evidence from functional MRI. PNAS, 100, 28602865.Google Scholar
Thompson, R. A. (1993). Developmental research and legal policy: Toward a two-way street. In Cicchetti, D. & Toth, S. (Eds.), Child abuse, child development, and social policy (pp. 75115). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Thompson, R. A. (1998). Making brain development headline news. Family Futures, 2, 3236.Google Scholar
Thompson, R. A. (2001a). Development in the first years of life. The Future of Children, 11, 2033.Google Scholar
Thompson, R. A. (2001b). Science vs. sound bites: Science and advocacy in public information campaigns for children. The Children’s Beat, 8, 1415.Google Scholar
Thompson, R. A. (2012). Bridging developmental neuroscience and the law: Child-caregiver relationships. Hastings Law Journal, 63, 14431468.Google Scholar
Thompson, R. A. (2014a). Stress and child development. The Future of Children, 24, 4159.Google Scholar
Thompson, R. A. (2014b). Why are relationships important to children’s well-being? In Ben-Arieh, A., Casas, F., Frones, I., Cases, F., & Korbin, J. E. (Eds.), Handbook of child well-being: Theories, methods, and policies in global perspective, Vol. 4 (pp. 19171954). Dordrecht: Springer.Google Scholar
Thompson, R. A., & Baumrind, D. (2019). Ethics of parenting. In Bornstein, M. H. (Ed.), Handbook of parenting, Vol. 5: The practice of parenting (3rd Ed.) (pp. 333). New York: Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
Thompson, R. A., & Haskins, R. (2014). Early stress gets under the skin: Promising initiatives to help children facing chronic adversity. The Future of Children Policy Brief, Spring, 1–7.Google Scholar
Thompson, R. A., & Nelson, C. A. (2001). Developmental science and the media: Early brain development. American Psychologist, 56, 515.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2016). Cultural learning redux. Child Development, 87, 643653.Google Scholar
Troller-Renfree, S. V., Costanzo, M. A., Duncan, G. J., Magnuson, K., Gennetian, L. A., Yoshikawa, H., Halpern-Meekiin, S., Fox, N. A., & Noble, K. G. (2022). The impact of a poverty reduction intervention on infant brain activity. PNAS, 119, e2115649119.Google Scholar
Tronick, E. Z., & Gianino, A. (1986). Interactive mismatch and repair: Challenges to the coping infant. Zero to Three Journal, 6, 16.Google Scholar
United States Census (2022). CPS historical time series on school enrollment. Washington, DC: US Census Bureau. www.census.gov/data/tables/time-series/demo/school-enrollment/cps-historical-time-series.html, retrieved January 14, 2023.Google Scholar
Urahn, S. K., & Watson, S. (2007). The Pew Charitable Trusts: Advancing quality Pre-K for all; Five years later. www.pewtrusts.org/~/media/legacy/uploadedfiles/wwwpewtrustsorg/reports/pre-k_education/prekarticle0207pdf.pdf, retrieved July 10, 2022.Google Scholar
Vijayakumar, N., Op de Macks, Z., Shirtcliff, E. A., & Pfeider, J. H. (2018). Puberty and the human brain: Insights into adolescent development. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 92, 417436.Google Scholar
Wagmiller, R. L., & Adelman, R. M. (2009). Childhood and intergenerational poverty: The long-term consequences of growing up poor. New York: National Center for Children in Poverty. www.nccp.org/wp-content/uploads/2020/05/text_909.pdf, retrieved April 7, 2021.Google Scholar
Wang, S., Harvey, L., Martin, R., van der Beek, E. M., Knol, J., Cryan, J. F., & Renes, I. B. (2018). Targeting the gut microbiota to influence brain development and function in early life. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 95, 191201.Google Scholar
Wang, Z., Williamson, R. A., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2015). Imitation as a mechanism in cognitive development: A cross-cultural investigation of 4-year-old children’s rule learning. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, article 562.Google Scholar
Watson, J. B. (1928a). Psychological care of infant and child. New York: W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
Watson, J. B. (1928b). The ways of behaviorism. New York: Harper & Brothers.Google Scholar
Watson, S. D. (2011). The right policy at the right time: The Pew Prekindergarten Campaign. In Zigler, E., Gilliam, W. S., & Barnett, W. S. (Eds.), The Pre-K debates: Current controversies and issues (pp. 920). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
Weingart, P. (2012). The lure of the mass media and its repercussions on science. In Rödder, S., Franzen, M., & Weingart, P. (Eds.), The sciences’ media connection: Public communication and its repercussions (pp. 1731). Dordrecht: Springer.Google Scholar
Weisberg, D. S., Keil, F. C., Goodstein, J., Rawson, E., & Gray, J. R. (2008). The seductive allure of neuroscience explanations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 470477.Google Scholar
Weiss, C. H. (1978). Improving the linkage between social research and public policy. In Lynn, L. E. (Ed.), Knowledge and policy: The uncertain connection (pp. 2381). Washington, DC: National Academies Press.Google Scholar
Weiss, C. H. (1980). Knowledge creep and decision accretion. Knowledge: Creation, Diffusion, Utilization, 1, 381404.Google Scholar
Weiss, C. H. (1987). The diffusion of social science research to policymakers: An overview. In Melton, G. B. (Ed.), Reforming the law: Impact of child development research (pp. 6385). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Weiss, C. H., & Singer, E. (1988). Reporting of social science in the national media. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
Wellman, H. M. (2015). Making minds: How theory of mind develops. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Wellman, H. M., & Gelman, S. A. (1992). Cognitive development: Foundational theories of core domains. Annual Review of Psychology, 43, 337375.Google Scholar
Werker, J. F., & Polka, L. (1993). Developmental changes in speech perception: New challenges and new directions. Journal of Phonetics, 21, 83101.Google Scholar
Werner, H. (1940). Comparative psychology of mental development. New York: International Universities Press.Google Scholar
Wolfe, D. A., & Mash, E. J. (Eds.) (2006). Behavioral and emotional disorders in adolescents: Nature, assessment, and treatment. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Wood, J. N., & Spelke, E. S. (2005). Chronometric studies of numerical cognition in five-month-old infants. Cognition, 97, 2339.Google Scholar
Wynn, K. (1992). Addition and subtraction by human infants. Nature, 358, 749750.Google Scholar
Xu, F., & Garcia, V. (2008). Intuitive statistics by 8-month-old infants. PNAS, 105, 50125015.Google Scholar
Yan, W. (2020). Coronavirus tests science’s need for speed limits. New York Times, April 21, section D, p. 7. www.nytimes.com/2020/04/14/science/coronavirus-disinformation.html, retrieved October 16, 2020.Google Scholar
Yeo, S. K., & Brossard, D. (2017). The (changing) nature of scientist-media interactions: A cross-national analysis. In Jamieson, K. H., Kahan, D. M., & Scheufele, D. A. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of the science of science communication (pp. 261272). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Zeballos-Roig, J. (2022). Signs of a midterm backlash are mounting for the GOP after Roe was overturned. Here are 3 proposals put forward by Republicans to support families. Business Insider. www.businessinsider.com/gop-family-proposals-child-benefits-midterm-backlash-roe-overturned-2022-9, retrieved September 7, 2022.Google Scholar
Zero to Three (1997). Key findings from a nationwide survey among parents of zero-to-three-year-olds. Washington, DC: Zero to Three. www.upi.com/Archives/1997/04/17/Parents-unsure-on-emotional-upbringing/2037861249600/, retrieved April 29, 2022.Google Scholar
Zero to Three (2017). Public perceptions of baby brain development: A national survey of voters. Zero to Three. www.zerotothree.org/resources/2124-public-perceptions-of-baby-brain-development-a-national-survey-of-voters, retrieved March 1, 2022.Google Scholar
Zigler, E. (2011). A warning against exaggerating the benefits of preschool education programs. In Zigler, E., Gilliam, W. S., & Barnett, W. S. (Eds.), The pre-K debates: Current controversies and issues (pp. 197200). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • Works Cited
  • Ross A. Thompson, University of California, Davis
  • Book: The Brain Development Revolution
  • Online publication: 24 August 2023
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009304276.010
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • Works Cited
  • Ross A. Thompson, University of California, Davis
  • Book: The Brain Development Revolution
  • Online publication: 24 August 2023
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009304276.010
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • Works Cited
  • Ross A. Thompson, University of California, Davis
  • Book: The Brain Development Revolution
  • Online publication: 24 August 2023
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009304276.010
Available formats
×