Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-7cvxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T01:47:23.641Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

10 - The Role of Reflection in Promoting Adolescent Self-Regulation

from Part III - Neural Mechanisms

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 October 2015

Gabriele Oettingen
Affiliation:
New York University
Peter M. Gollwitzer
Affiliation:
New York University
Get access

Summary

Author Note

Philip D. Zelazo, Institute of Child Development, University of Minnesota, Minnesota; Sabine Doebel, Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of Colorado, Boulder.

Correspondence concerning this chapter should be addressed to Philip David Zelazo, Institute of Child Development, University of Minnesota, 51 East River Road, Minneapolis, MN 55455–0345. E-mail: [email protected], or Sabine Doebel, Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of Colorado, Boulder, D244 Muenzinger, 345 UCB, Boulder, Colorado, 80309-0345. E-mail: [email protected]

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2015

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Amsterdam, B. (1972). Mirror self-image reactions before age two. Developmental Psychobiology, 5, 297305. doi:10.1002/dev.420050403CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Seghete, K. L. M., Claus, E. D., Burgess, G. C., Ruzic, L., & Banich, M. T. (2011). Cognitive control in adolescence: Neural underpinnings and relation to self-report behaviors. PloS One, 6, 114. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0021598CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Badre, D., & D'Esposito, M. (2007). Functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence for a hierarchical organization of the prefrontal cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 20822099. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.12.2082CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., & Anderson, S. W. (1994). Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition, 50, 715. doi:10.1016/0010–0277(94)90018–3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bengtsson, S. L., Nagy, Z., Skare, S., Forsman, L., Forssberg, H., & Ullén, F. (2005). Extensive piano practicing has regionally specific effects on white matter development. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 11481150. doi:10.1038/nn1516CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Botvinick, M. M. (2008). Hierarchical models of behavior and prefrontal function. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 201208. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.02.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Broderick, P. C. (2013). Learning to BREATHE: A mindfulness curriculum for adolescents to cultivate emotion regulation, attention, and performance. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger.Google Scholar
Bryck, R. L., & Fisher, P. A. (2012). Training the brain: Practical applications of neural plasticity from the intersection of cognitive neuroscience, developmental psychology, and prevention science. American Psychologist, 67, 87100. doi: 10.1037/a0024657CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bunge, S. A., & Zelazo, P. D. (2006). A brain-based account of the development of rule use in childhood. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 118121. doi: 10.1111/j.0963–7214.2006.00419.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buodo, G., Sarlo, M., & Palomba, D. (2002). Attentional resources measured by reaction times highlight differences within pleasant and unpleasant, high arousing stimuli. Motivation and Emotion, 26, 123138. doi: 10.1023/A:1019886501965CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlson, S. M., Davis, A. C., & Leach, J. G. (2005). Less is more: Executive function and symbolic representation in preschool children. Psychological Science, 16, 609616. doi: 10.1111/j.1467–9280.2005.01583.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlson, S. M., & Moses, L. J. (2001). Individual differences in inhibitory control and children's theory of mind. Child Development, 72, 10321053. doi: 10.1111/1467–8624.00333CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlson, S. M., Zelazo, P. D., & Faja, S. (2013). Executive function. In Zelazo, P. D. (Ed.), Oxford handbook of developmental psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 706743). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Christoff, K., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2000). The frontopolar cortex and human cognition: Evidence for a rostrocaudal hierarchical organization within the human prefrontal cortex. Psychobiology, 28, 168186. doi: 10.3758/BF03331976CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cunningham, W. A., & Zelazo, P. D. (2007). Attitudes and evaluations: A social cognitive neuroscience perspective. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 97104. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.12.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cunningham, W. A., Zelazo, P. D., Packer, D. J., & Van Bavel, J. J. (2007). The Iterative Reprocessing Model: A multi-level framework for attitudes and evaluation. Social Cognition, 25, 736760. doi: 10.1521/soco.2007.25.5.736CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Damon, W., & Hart, D. (1982). The development of self-understanding from infancy through adolescence. Child Development, 53, 841864. doi:10.1177/0272431686063007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, M. C., Amso, D Cruess-Anderson, L, & Diamond, A. (2006). Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4–13 years: Evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition and task switching. Neuropsychologia, 44, 20372078.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diamond, A., & Lee, K. (2011). Interventions shown to aid executive function development in children 4 to 12 years old. Science, 333, 959964. doi: 10.1126/science.1204529CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duckworth, A. L., Grant, H., Loew, B., Oettingen, G., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2011). Self-regulation strategies improve self-discipline in adolescents: Benefits of mental contrasting and implementation intentions. Educational Psychology, 31, 1726.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Emerson, M. J., & Miyake, A. (2003). The role of inner speech in task switching: A dual-task investigation. Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 148168. doi: 10.1080/01443410.2010.506003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Espinet, S. D., Anderson, J. E., & Zelazo, P. D. (2012). N2 amplitude as a neural marker of executive function in young children: An ERP study of children who switch versus perseverate on the dimensional change card sort. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 2, 4958. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2011.12.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Espinet, S. D., Anderson, J. E., & Zelazo, P. D. (2013). Reflection training improves executive function in preschool-age children: Behavioral and neural effects. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 4, 315. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2012.11.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frye, D., Zelazo, P. D., & Palfai, T. (1995). Theory of mind and rule-based reasoning. Cognitive Development, 10, 483527. doi:10.1016/0885–2014(95)90024–1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fujita, K., Trope, Y., Liberman, N., & Levin-Sagi, M. (2006). Construal levels and self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 351367. doi: 10.1037/0022–3514.90.3.351CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gardner, M., & Steinberg, L. (2005). Peer influence on risk taking, risk preference, and risky decision making in adolescence and adulthood: An experimental study. Developmental Psychology, 41, 625635. doi: 10.1037/0012–1649.41.4.625CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gogtay, N., Giedd, J. N., Lusk, L., Hayashi, K. M., Greenstein, D., et al. (2004). Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 101, 81748179. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0402680101CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. American Psychologist, 54, 493503. doi: 10.1037/0003–066X.54.7.493CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M., & Oettingen, G. (2011). Planning promotes goal striving. In Vohs, K. D. & Baumeister, R. F. (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications (2nd ed., pp. 162185). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M., Wieber, F., Meyers, A. L., & McCrea, S. M. (2010). How to maximize implementation intention effects. In Agnew, C. R., Carlston, D. E., Graziano, W. G., & Kelly, J. R. (Eds.), Then a miracle occurs: Focusing on behavior in social psychological theory and research (pp. 137161). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Greenough, W. T., Black, J. E., & Wallace, C. S. (1987). Experience and brain development. Child Development, 58, 539559. doi: 10.2307/1130197CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Happaney, K., Zelazo, P. D., & Stuss, D. T. (2004). Development of orbitofrontal function: Current themes and future directions. Brain and Cognition, 55, 110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behavior: A neuropsychological theory. New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Huttenlocher, P. R. (2002). Neural plasticity: The effects of environment on the development of the cerebral cortex. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacques, S., & Zelazo, P. D. (2001). The Flexible Item Selection Task (FIST): A measure of executive function in preschoolers. Developmental Neuropsychology, 20, 573591. doi: 10.1207/S15326942DN2003_2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacques, S., & Zelazo, P. D. (2005). Language and the development of cognitive flexibility: Implications for theory of mind. In Astington, J. W. & Baird, J. A. (Eds.), Why language matters for theory of mind (pp. 144162). New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
James, W. (1984 [1892]). Psychology: The briefer course. New York: Harper.Google Scholar
Johnson, A. E., Forston, J. L., & Zelazo, P. D. (2013). Mindfulness training in preschool children. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
Kappes, A., Singmann, H., & Oettingen, G. (2012). Mental contrasting instigates goal-pursuit by linking obstacles of reality with instrumental behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 811818. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2012.02.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kappes, A., Wendt, M., Reinelt, T., & Oettingen, G. (2013). Mental contrasting changes the meaning of reality. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49, 797810. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2013.03.010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karbach, J., & Kray, J. (2007). Developmental changes in switching between mental task sets: The influence of verbal labeling in childhood. Journal of Cognition and Development, 8, 205236. doi: 10.1080/15248370701202430CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kerr, A., & Zelazo, P. D. (2004). Development of “hot” executive function: The Children's Gambling Task. Brain and Cognition, 55, 148157. doi: 10.1016/S0278–2626(03)00275–6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kharitonova, M., Chien, S., Colunga, E., & Munakata, Y. (2009). More than a matter of getting “unstuck”: Flexible thinkers use more abstract representations than perseverators. Developmental Science, 12, 662669. doi: 10.1111/j.1467–7687.2008.00799.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klingberg, T., Fernell, E., Olesen, P. J., Johnson, M., Gustafsson, P., et al. (2005). Computerized training of working memory in children with ADHD: A randomized, controlled trial. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 44, 177186. doi: 10.1097/00004583–200502000–00010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koechlin, E., Ody, C., & Kouneiher, F. (2003). The architecture of cognitive control in the human prefrontal cortex. Science, 302, 11811185. doi: 10.1126/science.1088545CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kray, J., Eber, J., & Karbach, J. (2008). Verbal self-instructions in task switching: A compensatory tool for action-control deficits in childhood and old age? Developmental Science, 11, 223236. doi: 10.1111/j.1467–7687.2008.00673.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luciana, M., Conklin, H. M., Hooper, C. J., & Yarger, R. S. (2005). The development of nonverbal working memory and executive control processes in adolescents. Child Development, 76, 697712. doi: 10.1111/j.1467–8624.2005.00872.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luria, A. R. (1959). The directive function of speech in development and dissolution. Word, 15, 341352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luria, A. R. (1961). The role of speech in the regulation of normal and abnormal behavior. Oxford: Liveright.Google Scholar
Manes, F., Sahakian, B., Clark, L., Rogers, R., Antoun, N., et al. (2002). Decision-making processes following damage to prefrontal cortex. Brain, 125, 624639. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf049CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marcovitch, S., & Zelazo, P. D. (1999). The A-not-B error: Results from a logistic meta-analysis. Child Development, 70, 12971313. doi: 10.1111/1467–8624.00095CrossRefGoogle Scholar
May, A. (2011). Experience-dependent structural plasticity in the adult human brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15, 475482. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.08.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moore, S., & Gullone, E. (1996). Predicting adolescent risk behavior using a personalized cost-benefit analysis. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 25, 343359. doi: 10.1007/BF01537389CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Munakata, Y., Snyder, H. R., & Chatham, C. H. (2012). Developing cognitive control: Three key transitions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 7177. doi: 10.1177/0963721412436807CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, E. E., Leibenluft, E., McClure, E., & Pine, D. S. (2005). The social re-orientation of adolescence: A neuroscience perspective on the process and its relation to psychopathology. Psychological Medicine, 35, 163174. doi: 10.1017/S0033291704003915CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oettingen, G. (1999). Free fantasies about the future and the emergence of developmental goals. In Brandtstädter, J. & Lerner, R. M. (Eds.), Action and self-development: Theory and research through the life span (pp. 315342). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Oettingen, G. (2012). Future thought and behaviour change. European Review of Social Psychology, 23, 163. doi: 10.1080/10463283.2011.643698CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oettingen, G., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2010). Strategies of setting and implementing goals: Mental contrasting and implementation intentions. In Maddux, J. E. & Tangney, J. P. (Eds.), Social psychological foundations of clinical psychology (pp. 114135). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Olesen, P. J., Westerberg, H., & Klingberg, T. (2003). Increased prefrontal and parietal activity after training of working memory. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 7579. doi: 10.1038/nn1165CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ortner, C. N. M., Kilner, S. J., & Zelazo, P. D. (2007). Mindfulness meditation and reduced emotional interference on a cognitive task. Motivation and Emotion, 31, 271283. doi: 10.1007/s11031–007–9076–7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perner, J., & Lang, B. (1999). Development of theory of mind and executive control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 337344. doi: 10.1016/S1364–6613(99)01362–5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perner, J., Lang, B., & Kloo, D. (2002). Theory of mind and self-control: More than a common problem of inhibition. Child Development, 73, 752767. doi: 10.1111/1467–8624.00436CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Povinelli, D. J., Landau, K. R., & Perilloux, H. K. (1996). Self-recognition in young children using delayed versus live feedback: Evidence of a developmental asynchrony. Child Development, 67, 15401554. doi: 10.2307/1131717CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prencipe, A., Kesek, A., Cohen, J., Lamm, C., Lewis, M. D., & Zelazo, P. D. (2011). Development of hot and cool executive function during the transition to adolescence. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 621637. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.09.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rochat, P., Broesch, T., & Jayne, K. (2012). Social awareness and early self-recognition. Consciousness and Cognition, 21, 14911497. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2012.04.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rueda, M. R., Rothbart, M. K., McCandliss, B. D., Saccomanno, L., & Posner, M. I. (2005). Training, maturation, and genetic influences on the development of executive attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102, 1493114936. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0506897102CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2010). Construal-level theory of psychological distance. Psychological Review, 117, 440463. doi: 10.1037/a0018963CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vygotsky, L. (1962). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiesel, T. N., & Hubel, D. H. (1963). Single-cell responses in striate cortex of kittens deprived of vision in one eye. Journal of Neurophysiology, 26, 10031017.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zelazo, P. D. (2004). The development of conscious control in childhood. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 1217. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2003.11.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zelazo, P. D. (2006). The Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS): A method of assessing executive function in children. Nature Protocols, 1, 297301. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2006.46CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zelazo, P. D., Anderson, J. E., Richler, J., Wallner-Allen, K., Beaumont, J. L., & Weintraub, S. (2013). NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (NIHTB-CB): Measuring executive function and attention. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 1633.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zelazo, P. D., & Carlson, S. M. (2012). Hot and cool executive function in childhood and adolescence: Development and plasticity. Child Development Perspectives, 6, 354360. doi: 10.1111/j.1750–8606.2012.00246.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zelazo, P. D., Frye, D., & Rapus, T. (1996). An age-related dissociation between knowing rules and using them. Cognitive Development, 11, 3763. doi: 10.1016/S0885–2014(96)90027–1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zelazo, P. D., & Lyons, K. E. (2012). The potential benefits of mindfulness training in early childhood: A developmental social cognitive neuroscience perspective. Child Development Perspectives, 6, 154160. doi: 10.1111/j.1750–8606.2012.00241.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zelazo, P. D., & Müller, U. (2002). Executive function in typical and atypical development. In Goswami, U (Ed.), Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development (pp. 445469). Malden: Blackwell Publishing.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zelazo, P. D., Müller, U., Frye, D., & Marcovitch, S. (2003). The development of executive function in early childhood. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 68, vii137. doi: 10.1111/j.1540–5834.2003.06803001.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zelazo, P. D., & Sommerville, J. A. (2001). Levels of consciousness of the self in time. In Moore, C. & Lemmon, K. (Eds.), The self in time: Developmental perspectives (pp. 229252). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×