Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jn8rn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-19T10:15:35.986Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

References

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 October 2019

Brian Parkinson
Affiliation:
University of Oxford
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Heart to Heart
How Your Emotions Affect Other People
, pp. 346 - 394
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abelson, R. P., & Sermat, V. (1962). Multidimensional scaling of facial expressions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 546–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adams, R. B. Jr., Albohn, D. N., & Kveraga, K. (2017). A social vision account of facial expression perception. In Fernandez-Dols, J.-M. & Russell, J. A. (Eds.), The science of facial expression (pp. 315–32). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Adams, R. B. Jr., & Franklin, R. G. (2009). Influence of emotional expression on the processing of gaze direction. Motivation and Emotion, 33, 106112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adams, R. B. Jr., Hess, U., & Kleck, R. E. (2015). The intersection of gender-related facial appearance and facial displays of emotion. Emotion Review, 7, 513.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adams, R. B. Jr., & Kleck, R. E. (2003). Perceived gaze direction and the processing of facial displays of emotion. Psychological Science, 14, 644–7.Google Scholar
Adams, R. B. Jr., & Kleck, R. E. (2005). Effects of direct and averted gaze on the perception of facially communicated emotion. Emotion, 5, 311.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aldao, A. (2013). The future of emotion regulation research: Capturing context. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8, 155–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 217–37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alexander, M. G., Brewer, M. B., & Herrmann, R. K. (1999). Images and affect: A functional analysis of out-group stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 7893.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ambadar, Z., Schooler, J., & Cohn, J. (2005). Deciphering the enigmatic face: The importance of facial dynamics in interpreting subtle facial expressions. Psychological Science, 16, 403–10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association (2013). DSM-V: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th edition). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.Google Scholar
Andersen, S. M., & Chen, S. (2002). The relational self: An interpersonal social-cognitive theory. Psychological Review, 109, 619–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, C., Keltner, D., & John, O. P. (2003). Emotional convergence between people over time. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 1054–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andreasson, P., & Dimberg, U. (2008). Emotional empathy and facial feedback. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 32, 215–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andrew, R. J. (1963). Evolution of facial expression. Science, 142, 1034–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anshel, A., & Kipper, D. A. (1988). The influence of group singing on trust and cooperation. Journal of Music Therapy, 25, 145–55.Google Scholar
Aristotle, (1991). On rhetoric: A theory of civic discourse (tr. G. A. Kennedy). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Armstrong, S. L., Gleitman, H., & Gleitman, L. R. (1983). What some concepts might not be. Cognition, 13, 263308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arnold, M. B. (1960). Emotion and personality: (Vol. 1) Psychological aspects. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Ashforth, B. E., & Humphrey, R. H. (1993). Emotional labor in service roles: The influence of identity. Academy of Management Review, 18, 88115.Google Scholar
Augustine, A. A., & Hemenover, S. H. (2009). On the relative effectiveness of affect regulation strategies: A meta-analysis. Cognition and Emotion, 23, 1181–220.Google Scholar
Austin, J. L. (1962). How to do things with words. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Averill, J. R. (1975). A semantic atlas of emotional concepts. JSAS Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology, 5, 330 (Ms. No. 421).Google Scholar
Averill, J. R. (1980). A constructivist view of emotion. In Plutchik, R. & Kellerman, H. (Eds.), Theories of emotion (pp. 305–40). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Averill, J. R. (1982). Anger and aggression: An essay on emotion. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Averill, J. R. (1985). The social construction of emotion: With special reference to love. In Gergen, K. J. & Davis, K. (Eds.), The social construction of the person (pp. 89109). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Averill, J. R., & Boothroyd, P. (1977). On falling in love in conformance with the romantic ideal. Motivation and Emotion, 1, 235–47.Google Scholar
Averill, J. R., Catlin, G., & Chon, K. K. (1990). Rules of hope. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Averill, J. R., & Nunley, E. P. (1988). Grief as an emotion and as a disease: A social-constructionist perspective. Journal of Social Issues, 44, 7995.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Averill, J. R., & Nunley, E. P. (1992). Voyages of the heart: Living an emotionally creative life. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Aviezer, H., & Hassin, R. R. (2017). Inherently ambiguous: An argument for contextualized emotion perception. In Fernandez-Dols, J.-M. & Russell, J. A. (Eds.), The science of facial expression (pp. 333–49). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Aviezer, H., Hassin, R. R., Ryan, J., Grady, C., Susskind, J., Anderson, A., Moscovitch, M., & Bentin, S. (2008). Angry, disgusted, or afraid? Studies on the malleability of emotion perception. Psychological Science, 19, 724–32.Google Scholar
Aviezer, H., Trope, Y., & Todorov, A. (2012). Holistic person processing: Faces with bodies tell the whole story. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103, 2037.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ax, A. F. (1953). The physiological differentiation between fear and anger in humans. Psychosomatic Medicine, 15, 433–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baile, W. F., Buckman, R., Lenzi, R., Glober, G., Beale, E. A., & Kudelka, A. P. (2000). SPIKES – A six-step protocol for delivering bad news: Application to the patient with cancer. The Oncologist, 5, 302–11.Google Scholar
Baker, S. D. (2007). Followership: The theoretical foundation of a contemporary construct. Journal of Leadership and Organizational Studies, 14, 5060.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bakker, A. B., Schaufeli, W. B., Demerouti, E., Janssen, P. M. P., Van der Hulst, R., & Brouwer, J. (2000). Using equity theory to examine the difference between burnout and depression. Anxiety, Stress and Coping, 13, 247–68.Google Scholar
Barger, P., & Grandey, A. (2006). ‘Service with a smile’ and encounter satisfaction: Emotional contagion and appraisal mechanisms. Academy of Management Journal, 49, 1229–38.Google Scholar
Barr, C. L., & Kleck, R. E. (1995). Self-other perception of the intensity of facial expressions of emotion: Do we know what we show? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 608–18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrett, L. F. (2006). Are emotions natural kinds? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 2858.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F. (2017). How emotions are made: The secret life of the brain. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Barsade, S. G. (2002). The ripple effect: Emotional contagion and its influence on group behavior. Administrative Science Quarterly, 47, 644–75.Google Scholar
Barsade, S. G., & Gibson, D. E. (1998). Group emotion: A view from top and bottom. In Neale, M. A. & Mannix, E. A. (Eds.), Research on managing groups and teams (Vol. 1, pp. 81102). Stamford, CT: JAI Press.Google Scholar
Barsade, S. G., & Gibson, D. E. (2012). Group affect: Its influence on individual and group outcomes. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 119–23.Google Scholar
Barsalou, L. W. (1985). Ideals, central tendency, and frequency of instantiation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11, 629–54.Google Scholar
Barsalou, L.W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 577660.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barsalou, L. W. (2005). Abstraction as dynamic interpretation in perceptual symbol systems. In Gershkoff-Stowe, L. & Rakison, D. (Eds.), Building object categories (pp. 389431). Carnegie Symposium Series. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bartel, C., & Saavedra, R. (2000). The collective construction of work group moods. Administrative Science Quarterly, 45, 197231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bass, B. M., & Riggio, R. E. (2006). Transformational leadership (2nd edition). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baudrillard, J. (1988). America. New York: Verso.Google Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., Stillwell, A. M., & Heatherton, T. F. (1994). Guilt: An interpersonal approach. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 243–67.Google Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., Stillwell, A. M., & Heatherton, T. F. (1995). Personal narratives about guilt: Role in action control and interpersonal relationships. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 17, 173–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bavelas, J. B., Black, A., Chovil, N., Lemery, C. R., & Mullett, J. (1988). Form and function in motor mimicry: Topographic evidence that the primary function is communicative. Human Communication Research, 14, 275–99.Google Scholar
Bavelas, J. B., Black, A., Lemery, C. R., & Mullett, J. (1986). ‘I show how you feel’: Motor mimicry as a communicative act. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 322–9.Google Scholar
Bavelas, J., Coates, L., & Johnson, T. (2004). Listener responses as a collaborative process: The role of gaze. Journal of Communication, 52, 566–80.Google Scholar
Bayliss, A. P., Frischen, A., Fenske, M. J., & Tipper, S. P. (2007). Affective evaluations of objects are influenced by observed gaze direction and emotion expression. Cognition, 104, 644–53.Google Scholar
Beal, D., & Trougakos, J. (2013). Episodic intrapersonal emotion regulation: Or, dealing with life as it happens. In Grandey, A. A., Diefendorff, J. M. & Rupp, D. (Eds.), Emotional labor in the 21st century: Diverse perspectives on emotion regulation at work (pp. 3155). New York: Psychology Press/Routledge.Google Scholar
Beal, D. J., Trougakos, J. P., Weiss, H. M., & Green, S. G. (2006). Episodic processes in emotional labor: Perceptions of affective delivery and regulation strategies. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 1053–65.Google Scholar
Bedford, E. (1957). Emotions. Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society, 57, 281304.Google Scholar
Beebe, B., Gerstman, L., Carson, B., Dolins, M., Zigman, A., Rosenzweig, H., Faughey, K., & Korman, M. (1982). Rhythmic communication in the mother-infant dyad. In Davis, M. (Ed.), Interaction rhythms: Periodicity in communicative behaviour (pp. 77100). New York: Human Sciences Press.Google Scholar
Benbassat, J., & Baumal, R. (2004). What is empathy, and how can it be promoted during clinical clerkships? Academic Medicine, 79, 832–9.Google Scholar
Benitez-Quiroz, C. F., Srinivasan, R., & Martinez, A. M. (2018). Facial color is an efficient mechanism to visually transmit emotion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 115, 3581–6.Google Scholar
Bernardi, N. F., Bellemare-Pepin, A., & Peretz, I. (2017). Enhancement of pleasure during spontaneous dance. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 11, article 572.Google ScholarPubMed
Berndsen, M., Hornsey, M. J., & Wohl, M. J. A. (2015). The impact of a victim-focused apology on forgiveness in an intergroup context. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 18, 726–39.Google Scholar
Bernieri, F. J., Davis, J. M., Rosenthal, R., & Knee, C. R. (1994). Interactional synchrony and rapport: Measuring synchrony in displays devoid of sound and facial affect. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 303–11.Google Scholar
Bernieri, F., Reznick, J. S., & Rosenthal, R. (1988). Synchrony, pseudo-synchrony, and dissynchrony: Measuring the entrainment process in mother-infant interactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 243–53.Google Scholar
Berry, D. S., & McArthur, L. A. (1982). Perceiving character in faces: The impact of age-related craniofacial changes on social perception. Psychological Bulletin, 100, 318.Google Scholar
Biglan, A. A., Hops, H., Sherman, L., Friedman, L. S., Arthur, J., & Osteen, V. (1985). Problem-solving interactions of depressed women and their husbands. Behavior Therapy, 16, 431–51.Google Scholar
Billig, M. (1987). Arguing and thinking: A rhetorical approach to social psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Billig, M., & Tajfel, H. (1973). Social categorization and similarity in intergroup behaviour. European Journal of Social Psychology, 3, 2752.Google Scholar
Birdwhistell, R. (1970). Kinesics and context. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.Google Scholar
Blackmore, J., & Sachs, J. (1998). You never show you can’t cope: Women in school leadership roles managing their emotions. Gender and Education, 10, 265–79.Google Scholar
Blairy, S., Herrera, P., & Hess, U. (1999). Mimicry and the judgment of emotional facial expressions. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 23, 541.Google Scholar
Blascovich, J., Ginsburg, G. P., & Howe, R. C. (1975). Blackjack and the risky shift, II: Monetary stakes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 11, 224–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Block, J. (1957). Studies in the phenomenology of emotions. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 54, 358–63.Google Scholar
Boerebach, B. C., Scheepers, R. A., van der Leeuw, R. M., Heineman, M. J., Arah, O. A., & Lombarts, K. M. (2014). The impact of clinicians’ personality and their interpersonal behaviors on the quality of patient care: A systematic review. International Journal for Quality in Health Care, 26, 426–81.Google Scholar
Bonanno, G. A., & Burton, C. L. (2013). Regulatory flexibility: An individual differences perspective on coping and emotion regulation. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8, 591612.Google Scholar
Bono, J. E., & Ilies, R. (2006). Charisma, positive emotions and mood contagion. Leadership Quarterly, 17, 317–34.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., Kessen, W., & Weiskopf, S. (1976). Color vision and hue categorization in young human infants. Journal of the Experimental Psychology: Human Performance, 1, 115–29.Google Scholar
Bould, E., Morris, N., & Wink, B. (2008). Recognising subtle emotional expressions: The role of facial movements. Cognition and Emotion, 22, 1569–87.Google Scholar
Bourgeois, P., & Hess, U. (2008). The impact of social context on mimicry. Biological Psychology, 77, 343–52.Google Scholar
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss (Vol. 2) Separation: Anxiety and anger. London: Hogarth Press.Google Scholar
Brand, R. J., Baldwin, D. A., & Ashburn, L. A. (2002). Evidence for ‘motionese’: Modifications in mothers’ infant-directed action. Developmental Science, 5, 7283.Google Scholar
Branscombe, N. R., & Doosje, B. (Eds.). (2004). Collective guilt: International perspectives. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Branscombe, N. R., & Miron, A. M. (2004). Interpreting the ingroup’s negative actions toward another group: Emotional reactions to appraised harm. In Tiedens, L. Z. & Leach, C. W. (Eds.), The social life of emotions (pp. 314–35). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Brauer, M., & Judd, C. M. (1996). Group polarization and repeated attitude expressions: A new take on an old topic. European Review of Social Psychology, 7, 174207.Google Scholar
Braunstein, L. M., Gross, J. J., & Ochsner, K. N. (2017). Explicit and implicit emotion regulation: A multi-level framework. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12, 1545–57.Google Scholar
Brewer, M. B. (1991). The social self: On being the same and different at the same time. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17, 475–82.Google Scholar
Brewer, M. B., & Alexander, M. G. (2002). Intergroup emotions and images. In Mackie, D. M. & Smith, E. R. (Eds.), From prejudice to intergroup emotions: Differentiated reactions to social groups (pp. 209–25). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Brotheridge, C., & Lee, R. T. (2003). Development and validation of the Emotional Labour Scale. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 76, 365–79.Google Scholar
Brotheridge, C., & Lee, R. T. (2008). The emotions of managing: An introduction to the special issue. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 23, 108–17.Google Scholar
Brown, P., & Levinson, S. C. (1987). Politeness: Some universals in language usage. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bruder, M., Dosmukhambetova, D., Nerb, J., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2012). Emotional signals in nonverbal interaction: Dyadic facilitation and convergence in expressions, appraisals, and feelings. Cognition and Emotion, 26, 480502.Google Scholar
Bush, K. A., Inman, C. S., Hamann, S., Kilts, C. D., & James, G. A. (2017). Distributed neural processing predictors of multi-dimensional properties of affect. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 11, article 49.Google Scholar
Bush, L. K., Barr, C. L., McHugo, G. J., & Lanzetta, J. T. (1989). The effects of facial control and facial mimicry on subjective reactions to comedy routines. Motivation and Emotion, 13, 3152.Google Scholar
Buss, D. M., & Schmidt, D. P. (1993). Sexual strategies theory: An evolutionary perspective on human mating. Psychological Review, 100, 204–32.Google Scholar
Butalid, L., Bensing, J. M., & Verhaak, P. F. (2014). Talking about psychosocial problems: An observational study on changes in doctor-patient communication in general practice between 1977 and 2008. Patient Education and Counseling, 94, 314–21.Google Scholar
Butler, E. A., Egloff, B., Wilhelm, F. H., Smith, N. C., Erickson, E. A., & Gross, J. J. (2003). The social consequences of expressive suppression. Emotion, 3, 4867.Google Scholar
Butler, E. A., & Randall, A. K. (2013). Emotional coregulation in close relationships. Emotion Review, 5, 202210.Google Scholar
Byrne, D. (1961). Interpersonal attraction and attitude similarity. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 62, 713–15.Google Scholar
Cacioppo, J. T., Berntson, G. G., & Klein, D. J. (1992). What is an emotion?: The role of somatovisceral afference, with special emphasis on somatovisceral ‘illusions’. Review of Personality and Social Psychology, 14, 6398.Google Scholar
Campos, J. J., Frankel, C. B., & Camras, L. (2004). On the nature of emotion regulation. Child Development, 75, 377–94.Google Scholar
Campos, J. J., Mumme, D., Kermoian, R., & Campos, R. (1994). A functionalist perspective on the nature of emotion. In Fox, N. (Ed.), The development of emotion regulation: Biological and behavioral considerations. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59, (2–3, Serial No. 240), 284303.Google Scholar
Campos, J. J., & Stenberg, C. (1981). Perception, appraisal, and emotion: The onset of social referencing. In Lamb, M. E. & Sherrod, L. R. (Eds.), Infant social cognition: Empirical and theoretical considerations (pp. 273314). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Camras, L. A. (1992). Expressive development and basic emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 269–83.Google Scholar
Camras, L. A., Campos, J. J., Oster, H., Miyake, K., & Bradshaw, D. (1992). Japanese and American infants’ responses to arm restraint. Developmental Psychology, 28, 578–83.Google Scholar
Cannon, W. B. (1927). The James-Lange theory of emotions: A critical examination and an alternative theory. American Journal of Psychology, 39, 106–24.Google Scholar
Carlson, D., Ferguson, M., Hunter, E., & Whitten, D. (2012). Abusive supervision and work-family conflict: The path through emotional labor and burnout. The Leadership Quarterly, 23, 849–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63, 1143.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. M., & Russell, J. A. (1996). Do facial expressions signal specific emotions? Judging emotion from the face in context. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 205218.Google Scholar
Chaiken, S. (1979). Communicator physical attractiveness and persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1387–97.Google Scholar
Chang, L. J., Gianaros, P. J., Manuck, S. B., Krishnan, A., & Wager, T. D. (2015). A sensitive and specific neural signature for picture-induced negative affect. PLOS Biology, 13, e1002180.Google Scholar
Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 893910.Google Scholar
Chen, C. N., Crivelli, C., Garrod, O. G. B., Schyns, P. G., Fernández-Dols, J.-M., & Jack, R. E. (2018). Distinct facial expressions represent pain and pleasure across cultures. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 115, e10013e10021.Google Scholar
Cheshin, A., Amit, A., & van Kleef, G. A. (2018). The interpersonal effects of emotion intensity in customer service: Perceived appropriateness and authenticity of attendants’ emotional displays shape customer trust and satisfaction. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 144, 97111.Google Scholar
Chi, N.-W., Grandey, A., Diamond, J., & Krimmel, K. (2011). Want a tip? Service performance as a function of emotion regulation and extraversion. Journal of Applied Psychology, 96, 1337–46.Google Scholar
Chi, N.-W., Yang, J. X., & Lin, C. Y. (2018). Service workers’ chain reactions to daily customer mistreatment: Behavioral linkages, mechanisms, and boundary conditions. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 23, 5870.Google Scholar
Chovil, N. (1997). Facing others: A social communicative perspective on facial displays. In Russell, J. A. & Fernández-Dols, J.-M. (Eds.), The psychology of facial expression (pp. 321–33). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Clark, H. H. (1985). Language use and language users. In Lindzey, G. & Aronson, E. (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (3rd edition, pp. 179231). New York: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
Clayton, M., Sager, R., & Will, U. (2004). In time with the music: The concept of entrainment and its significance for ethnomusicology. ESEM CounterPoint, 1, 184.Google Scholar
Clément, F., & Dukes, D. (2017). Social appraisal and social referencing: Two components of affective social learning. Emotion Review, 9, 253–61.Google Scholar
Clift, S., & Hancox, G. (2001). The perceived benefits of singing: Findings from preliminary surveys of a university college choral society. Journal of the Royal Society for the Promotion of Health, 121, 248–56.Google Scholar
Clore, G. L., & Ortony, A. (1991). What more is there to emotion concepts than prototypes? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 4850.Google Scholar
Clore, G. L., Ortony, A., & Foss, M. A. (1987). The psychological foundation of the affective lexicon. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 751–66.Google Scholar
Cohen, J. B., & Andrade, E. B. (2004). Affective intuition and task-contingent affect regulation. Journal of Consumer Research, 31, 358–67.Google Scholar
Collins, R. (2004). Interaction ritual chains. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Collins, R. (2014). Interaction ritual chains and collective effervescence. In von Scheve, C. & Salmela, M. (Eds.), Collective emotions (pp. 299311). Oxford: University of Oxford Press.Google Scholar
Conger, J. A., & Kanungo, R. N. (1987). Toward a behavioral theory of charismatic leadership in organizational settings. Academy of Management Review, 12, 637–47.Google Scholar
Connelly, S., & Ruark, G. (2010). Leadership style and activating potential moderators of the relationships among leader emotional displays and outcomes. Leadership Quarterly, 21, 745–64.Google Scholar
Cossette, M., & Hess, U. (2015). Service with style and smile: How and why employees are performing emotional labor. European Review of Applied Psychology, 65, 7182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Costa, P., Magalhaes, E., & Costa, M. J. (2013). A latent growth model suggests that empathy of medical students does not decline over time. Advances in Health Sciences Education: Theory and Practice, 18, 509–22.Google Scholar
Côté, S. (2005). A social interaction model of the effects of emotion regulation on work strain. Academy of Management Review, 30, 509–30.Google Scholar
Coulter, J. (1979). The social construction of mind: Studies in ethnomethodology and linguistic philosophy. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Cowen, A. S., & Keltner, D. (2017). Self-report captures 27 distinct categories of emotion bridged by continuous gradients. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 114, E7900E7909.Google Scholar
Cowen, A. S., & Keltner, D. (2018). Clarifying the conceptualization, dimensionality, and structure of emotion: Response to Barrett and colleagues. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22, 274–6.Google Scholar
Coyne, J. C. (1976). Toward an interactional description of depression. Psychiatry, 39, 2840.Google Scholar
Crivelli, C. (2016). Facial expressions and emotions in small-scale societies. Ph.D. dissertation, Universita Autonóma de Madrid, Spain.Google Scholar
Crivelli, C., Carrera, P., & Fernández-Dols, J.-M. (2015). Are smiles a sign of happiness? Spontaneous expressions of judo winners. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33, 52–8.Google Scholar
Crivelli, C., & Fridlund, A. J. (2018). Facial displays are tools for social influence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22, 388399.Google Scholar
Crivelli, C., & Fridlund, A. J. (2019). Inside-out: From basic emotions theory to the behavioral ecology view. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 43, 161–94.Google Scholar
Crivelli, C., Jarillo, S., Russell, J. A., & Fernández-Dols, J.-M. (2016a). Reading emotions from faces in two indigenous societies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145, 830–43.Google Scholar
Crivelli, C., Russell, J. A., Jarillo, S., & Fernández-Dols, J.-M. (2016b). The fear gasping face as a threat display in a Melanesian society. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 113, 12403–7.Google Scholar
Crivelli, C., Russell, J. A., Jarillo, S., & Fernández-Dols, J.-M. (2017). Recognizing spontaneous facial expressions of emotion in a small-scale society in Papua New Guinea. Emotion, 17, 337–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2009). Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Science, 13, 148–53.Google Scholar
Csíkszentmihályi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Damen, F., van Knippenberg, B., & van Knippenberg, D. (2008). Affective match in leadership: Leader emotional displays, follower positive affect, and follower performance. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 38, 868902.Google Scholar
Daniels, M. J. (1960). Affect and its control in the medical intern. American Journal of Sociology, 66, 259–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Darwin, C. (1872/1998). The expression of the emotions in man and animals (3rd edition). London: HarperCollins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dasborough, M. T., Ashkanasy, N. M., Tee, E. Y. J., & Tse, H. M. (2009). What goes around comes around: How meso-level negative emotional contagion can ultimately determine organizational attitudes toward leaders. Leadership Quarterly, 20, 571–85.Google Scholar
Davis, J. I., Senghas, A., Brandt, F., & Ochsner, K.N. (2010). The effects of BOTOX injections on emotional experience. Emotion, 10, 433–40.Google Scholar
Dawkins, R., & Krebs, J. R. (1978). Animal signals: Information or manipulation. In Krebs, J. R. & Davies, N. B. (Eds.), Behavioural ecology: An evolutionary approach (pp. 282309). Oxford, UK: Blackwell Scientific.Google Scholar
Decety, J., Yang, C.Y., & Cheng, Y. (2010). Physicians down-regulate their pain empathy response: An event-related brain potential study. NeuroImage, 50, 1676–82.Google Scholar
De Hoogh, A. H. B., Den Hartog, D. N., & Koopman, P. L. (2005). Linking the Big Five-Factors of personality to charismatic and transactional leadership; perceived dynamic work environment as a moderator. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 26, 839–65.Google Scholar
De Jong, P. (1999). Communicative and remedial effects of social blushing. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 23, 197217.Google Scholar
De Klerk, C. J. M., Hamilton, A. F. de C., & Southgate, V. (2018). Eye contact modulates facial mimicry in 4-month-old infants: An EMG and fNIRS study. Cortex, 106, 93103.Google Scholar
Del Piccolo, L., & Finset, A. (2018). Patients’ autonomic activation during clinical interaction: A review of empirical studies. Patient Education and Counseling, 101, 195208.Google Scholar
Delvaux, E., Meeussen, L., & Mesquita, B. (2016). Emotions are not always contagious: Longitudinal spreading of self-pride and group pride in homogeneous and status-differentiated groups. Cognition and Emotion, 30, 101–16.Google Scholar
Delvaux, E., Vanbeselaere, N., & Mesquita, B. (2015). Dynamic interplay between norms and experiences of anger and gratitude in groups. Small Group Research, 46, 300–23.Google Scholar
De Melo, C. M., Carnevale, P. J., Read, S. J., & Gratch, J. (2014). Reading people’s minds from emotion expressions in interdependent decision making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106, 7388.Google Scholar
De Rivera, J. (1977). A structural theory of emotions. New York: International Universities Press.Google Scholar
De Rivera, J. (2014). Emotion and the formation of social identities. In von Scheve, C. & Salmela, M. (Eds.), Collective emotions (pp. 217–31). Oxford: University of Oxford Press.Google Scholar
De Rivera, J., & Grinkis, C. (1986). Emotions as social relationships. Motivation and Emotion, 10, 351–69.Google Scholar
Derksen, F. A., olde Hartman, T. C., Bensing, J. M., & Lagro-Janssen, A. L. (2016). Managing barriers to empathy in the clinical encounter: A qualitative interview study with GPs. The British Journal of General Practice, 66, 653, e887e895.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Descartes, R. (1649/1911). The passions of the soul. In The philosophical works of Descartes (tr. E. S. Haldane & G. R. T. Ross, Vol. 1, pp. 329427). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
De Vos, B., van Zomeren, M., Gordijn, E., & Postmes, T. (2013). The communication of ‘pure’ group-based anger reduces tendencies toward intergroup conflict because it increases out-group empathy. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 39, 1043–52.Google Scholar
Dewey, J. (1894). The theory of emotion I: Emotional attitudes. Psychological Review, 1, 553–69.Google Scholar
Diefendorff, J. M., Croyle, M. H., & Gosserand, R. H. (2005). The dimensionality and antecedents of emotional labor strategies. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 66, 339–57.Google Scholar
Diefendorff, J. M., & Gosserand, R. (2003). Understanding the emotional labor process: A control theory perspective. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 24, 945–59.Google Scholar
Dijker, A. J. M. (1987). Emotional reactions to ethnic minorities. European Journal of Social Psychology, 47, 1105–17.Google Scholar
Dimberg, U., & Öhman, A. (1983). The effects of directional facial cues on electrodermal conditioning to facial stimuli. Psychophysiology, 20, 160–7.Google Scholar
Dixon, T. (2012). ‘Emotion’: The history of a keyword in crisis. Emotion Review, 4, 338–44.Google Scholar
Dogan, K., & Vecchio, R. P. (2001). Managing envy and jealousy in the workplace. Compensation & Benefits Review, 33, 5764.Google Scholar
Doosje, B., Branscombe, N., Spears, R., & Manstead, A. S. R. (1998). Guilty by association: When one’s group has a negative history. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 872–86.Google Scholar
Doré, B. P., Silvers, J. A., & Ochsner, K. N. (2016). Toward a personalized science of emotion regulation. Social & Personality Psychology Compass, 10, 171–87.Google Scholar
Draghi-Lorenz, R., Reddy, V., & Morris, P. (2005). Young infants can be perceived as shy, coy, bashful, embarrassed. Infant and Child Development, 14, 6383.Google Scholar
Driver, J., Davis, G., Ricciardelli, P., Kidd, P., Maxwell, E., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). Gaze perception triggers reflexive visuospatial orienting. Visual Cognition, 6, 509–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Drury, J., Cocking, C., Beale, J., Hanson, C., & Rapley, F. (2005). The phenomenology of empowerment in collective action. British Journal of Social Psychology, 44, 309–28.Google Scholar
Duchenne de Boulogne, G-B. (1862, tr. 1990, R. A. Cuthbertson). The mechanism of human facial expression. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Duffy, M. K., & Shaw, J. D. (2000). The Salieri syndrome: Consequences of envy in groups. Small Group Research, 31, 323.Google Scholar
Dunn, B. D., Billotti, D., Murphy, V., & Dalgleish, T. (2009). The consequences of effortful emotion regulation when processing distressing material: A comparison of suppression and acceptance. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47, 761–73.Google Scholar
Durán, J. I., Reisenzein, R., & Fernández-Dols, J.-M. (2017). Coherence between emotions and facial expressions: A research synthesis. In Fernández-Dols, J.-M. & Russell, J. A. (Eds.), The science of facial expression (pp. 107–29). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Durkheim, É. (1893, tr. 1997). The division of labour in society. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Durkheim, É. (1912, tr. 1995). The elementary forms of religious life. (tr. K. E. Fields). New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Dyrbye, L. N., Thomas, M. R., Harper, W., Massie, F. S. Jr., Power, D. V., Eacker, A., Szydlo, D. W., Novotny, P. J., Sloan, J. A., & Shanafelt, T. D. (2009). The learning environment and medical student burnout: a multicentre study. Medical Education, 43, 274–82.Google Scholar
Edwards, D. (1999). Emotion discourse. Cultural Psychology, 5, 271–91.Google Scholar
Edwards, D., & Potter, J. (1992). Discursive psychology. London: Sage.Google Scholar
Eggly, S., Penner, L., Albrecht, T. L., Cline, R. J., Foster, T., Naughton, M., Peterson, A., & Ruckdeschel, J. C. (2006). Discussing bad news in the outpatient oncology clinic: Rethinking current communication guidelines. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 24, 716–19.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (1972). Universals and cultural differences in facial expressions of emotion. In Cole, J. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (pp. 207–83). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (1979). About brows: Emotional and conversational signals. In von Cranach, M., Foppa, K., Lepenies, W. & Ploog, D. (Eds.), Human ethology: Claims and limits of a new discipline (pp. 169202). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (1989). The argument and evidence about universals in facial expressions of emotion. In Wagner, H. & Manstead, A. (Eds.), Handbook of psychophysiology: The biological psychology of emotions and social processes (pp. 143164). London: Wiley.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (1997). Should we call it expression or communication? Innovations in Social Science Research, 10, 333–44.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (2003). Emotions revealed: Understanding faces and feeling. London: Weidenfeld and Nicolson.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (2009). Darwin’s contributions to our understanding of facial expressions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B, 364, 3449–51.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (2017). Facial expression. In Fernandez-Dols, J.-M. & Russell, J. A. (Eds.), The science of facial expression (pp. 3956). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1975). Unmasking the face: A guide to recognizing emotions from facial clues. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1976). Pictures of facial affect. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1982). Felt, false, and miserable smiles. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 6, 238–58.Google Scholar
Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., & Ellsworth, P. (1972). Emotion in the human face: Guidelines for research and an integration of findings. New York: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V. and Ellsworth, P. (1982). What are the relative contributions of facial behavior and contextual information to the judgment of emotion? In Ekman, P. (Ed.), Emotion in the human face (2nd edition, pp. 111–27). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., & Simons, R. C. (1985). Is the startle reaction an emotion? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 1416–26.Google Scholar
Ekman, P., Levenson, R. W., & Friesen, W. V. (1983). Autonomic nervous system activity distinguishing among emotions. Science, 221, 1208–10.Google Scholar
Ekman, P., Sorensen, E. R., & Friesen, W. V. (1969). Pan-cultural elements in facial displays of emotions. Science, 164, 86–8.Google Scholar
Elfenbein, H. A. (2007). Emotion in organizations: A review and theoretical integration. Academy of Management Annals, 1, 371457.Google Scholar
Elfenbein, H. A. (2014). The many faces of emotional contagion: An affective process theory for affective linkage. Organizational Psychology Review, 4, 326–62.Google Scholar
Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2002). On the universality and cultural specificity of emotion recognition: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 203–35.Google Scholar
Elfenbein, H. A., Beaupré, M. G., Lévesque, M., & Hess, U. (2007). Toward a dialect theory: Cultural differences in the expression and recognition of posed facial expressions. Emotion, 7, 131–46.Google Scholar
Elfenbein, H. A., Marsh, A. A., & Ambady, N. (2002). Emotional intelligence and the recognition of emotion from facial expressions. In Barrett, L. F. & Salovey, P. (Eds.), The wisdom in feeling: Psychological processes in emotional intelligence (pp. 3758). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Ellsworth, P. C. (2013). Appraisal theory: Old and new questions. Emotion Review, 5, 125–31.Google Scholar
Ellsworth, P. C., & Tourangeau, R. (1981). On our failure to disconfirm what nobody ever said. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 363–69.Google Scholar
Erdmann, G., & Janke, W. (1978). Interaction between physiological and cognitive determinants of emotions. Biological Psychology, 6, 6174.Google Scholar
Etcoff, N. L., & Magee, J. J. (1992). Categorical perception of facial expressions. Cognition, 44, 227–40.Google Scholar
Evaldsson, A.-C., & Melander, H. (2017). Managing disruptive student conduct: Negative emotions and accountability in reproach-response sequences. Linguistics and Education, 37, 7386.Google Scholar
Fehr, B., & Russell, J. A. (1984). Concept of emotion viewed from a prototype perspective. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 464–86.Google Scholar
Ferguson, C. A. (1977). Baby talk as a simplified register. In Snow, C. & Ferguson, C. (Eds.), Talking to children: Language input and acquisition (pp. 209–35). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Fernández-Dols, J.-M. (2017). Natural facial expression: A view from psychological constructionism and pragmatics. In Fernández-Dols, J.-M. & Russell, J. A. (Eds.), The science of facial expression (pp. 457–75). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fernández-Dols, J.-M., Carrera, P., & Crivelli, C. (2011). Facial behavior while experiencing sexual excitement. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 35, 6371.Google Scholar
Fernández-Dols, J.-M., Carrera, P., Barchard, K., & Gacitua, M. (2008). False recognition of facial expressions of emotion: Causes and consequences. Emotion, 8, 530–9.Google Scholar
Fernández-Dols, J.-M., & Crivelli, C. (2013). Emotion and expression: Naturalistic studies. Emotion Review, 5, 24–9.Google Scholar
Fernández-Dols, J.-M., & Russell, J. A. (2017). Introduction. In Fernández-Dols, J.-M. & Russell, J. A. (Eds.), The science of facial expression (pp. 314). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fernández-Dols, J.-M., Sierra, B., & Ruiz-Belda, M. A. (1993). On the clarity of expressive and contextual information in the recognition of emotions: A methodological critique. European Journal of Social Psychology, 23, 195202.Google Scholar
Fernández-Dols, J.-M., & Ruiz-Belda, M. A. (1995). Are smiles a sign of happiness? Gold medal winners at the Olympic Games. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 1113–19.Google Scholar
Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Field, T. M., Woodson, R., Greenberg, R., & Cohen, D. (1982). Discrimination and imitation of facial expressions by neonates. Science, 278, 179–81.Google Scholar
Figley, C. (Ed.). (1995). Compassion fatigue: Coping with secondary traumatic stress disorder in those who treat the traumatized. New York: Brunner/Mazel.Google Scholar
Finzi, E., & Rosenthal, N. E. (2014). Treatment of depression with onabotulinumtoxinA: A randomized, double-blind, placebo controlled trial. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 52, 16.Google Scholar
Finzi, E., & Wasserman, E. (2006). Treatment of depression with botulinum toxin A: A case series. Dermatological Surgery, 32, 645–9.Google Scholar
Fischer, A. H., & Evers, C. (2011). The social costs and benefits of anger as a function of gender and relationship context. Sex Roles, 65, 2334.Google Scholar
Fischer, A. H., Rotteveel, M., Evers, C., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2004). Emotional assimilation: How we are influenced by others’ emotionsCahier de Psychologie Cognitive, 22, 223–45.Google Scholar
Fischer, A. H., & Giner-Sorolla, R. (2016). Contempt: Derogating others while keeping calm. Emotion Review, 8, 346–57.Google Scholar
Fischer, A. H., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2008). Social functions of emotion. In Lewis, M., Haviland-Jones, J. M. & Barrett, L. F., (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (3rd edition, pp. 456–68). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Fischer, A. H., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2016). Social functions of emotion and emotion regulation. In Barrett, L. F., Lewis, M. & Haviland-Jones, J. M. (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (4th edition, pp. 424–39). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Fischer, A. H., Manstead, A. S. R., & Zaalberg, R. (2003). Social influences on the emotion process. European Review of Social Psychology, 14, 171201.Google Scholar
Fischer, A. H., & Roseman, I. J. (2007). Beat them or ban them: The characteristics and social functions of anger and contempt. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 103–15.Google Scholar
Fischer, R., Xygalatas, D., Mitkidis, P., Reddish, P., Tok, P., Konvalinka, I., & Bulbulia, J. (2014). The fire-walker’s high: Affect and physiological responses in an extreme collective ritual. PLoS ONE, 9, e88355.Google Scholar
Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., & Glick, P. (2002). Emotions up and down: Intergroup emotions result from perceived status and competition. In Mackie, D. M. & Smith, E. R. (Eds.), From prejudice to intergroup emotions: Differentiated reactions to social groups (pp. 247–64). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Fleming, J. H. (1994). Multiple audience problems, tactical communication, and social interaction: A relational-regulation perspective. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology 26, 215–92.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1975). The language of thought. Boston: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Fogarty, L. A., Curbow, B. A., Wingard, J. R., McDonnell, K., & Somerfield, M. R. (1999). Can 40 seconds of compassion reduce patient anxiety? Journal of Clinical Oncology, 17, 371–9.Google Scholar
Folkow, B. (2000). Perspectives on the integrative functions of the ‘sympathoadrenomedullary system’. Autonomic Neuroscience: Basic and Clinical, 83, 101–15.Google Scholar
Fontaine, J. R. J., Scherer, K. R., & Soriano, C. (Eds.). (2013). Components of emotional meaning: A sourcebook. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fransen, K., Steffens, N. K., Haslam, S. A., Vanbeselaere, N., Van de Broek, G., & Boen, F. (2016). We will be champions: Leaders’ confidence in ‘us’ inspires team members’ team confidence and performance. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine and Science in Sports, 26, 1455–69.Google Scholar
Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. American Psychologist, 56, 218–26.Google Scholar
Freeman, J. B., & Ambady, N. (2011). A dynamic interactive theory of person construal. Psychological Review, 118, 247–79.Google Scholar
Freudenberger, H. J. (1974). Staff burnout. Journal of Social Issues, 30, 159–65.Google Scholar
Freudenberger, H. J. (1975). The staff burn-out syndrome in alternative institutions. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 12, 7382.Google Scholar
Fridlund, A. J. (1991). Sociality of solitary smiling: Potentiation by an implicit audience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 229–40.Google Scholar
Fridlund, A. J. (1994). Human facial expression: An evolutionary view. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Friedman, R., Anderson, C., Brett, J., Olekalns, M., Goates, N., & Lisco, C. C. (2004). The positive and negative effects of anger in dispute resolution: Evidence from electronically mediated disputes. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89, 369–76.Google Scholar
Friesen, W. V. (1972). Cultural differences in facial expressions in a social situation: An experimental test of the concept of display rules. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of California, San Francisco.Google Scholar
Frijda, N. H. (1953). The understanding of facial expression of emotion. Acta Psychologica, 9, 294362.Google Scholar
Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Frijda, N. H. (1993). The place of appraisal in emotion. Cognition and Emotion, 7, 357–87.Google Scholar
Frijda, N. H. (2005). Emotion experience. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 473–97.Google Scholar
Frijda, N. H., Kuipers, P., & ter Schure, E. (1989). Relations among emotion, appraisal, and emotional action readiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 212–28.Google Scholar
Frijda, N. H., & Tcherkassof, A. (1997). Facial expressions as modes of action readiness. In Russell, J. A. & Fernández-Dols, J. M. (Eds.), The psychology of facial expression (pp. 78102). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Frost, P. (2003). Toxic emotions at work: How compassionate managers handle pain and conflict. Boston: HBS Press.Google Scholar
Fugate, J. M. B. (2013). Categorical perception for emotional faces. Emotion Review, 5, 84–9.Google Scholar
Gabriel, A. S., & Diefendorff, J. (2015). Emotional labor dynamics: A momentary approach. Academy of Management Journal, 58, 1804–25.Google Scholar
Gausel, N., Leach, C. W., Mazziotta, A., & Feuchte, F. (2018). Seeking revenge or seeking reconciliation? How concern for social-image and felt shame helps explain responses in reciprocal intergroup conflict. European Journal of Social Psychology, 48, 6272.Google Scholar
Gemmiti, M., Hamed, S., Lauber-Biason, A., Wildhaber, J., Pharisa, C., & Klumb, P. L. (2017). Pediatricians’ affective communication behavior attenuates parents’ stress response during the medical interview. Patient Education and Counseling, 100, 480–6.Google Scholar
Gendron, M., Crivelli, C., & Barrett, L. F. (2018). Universality reconsidered: Diversity in meaning making about facial expressions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27, 211–19.Google Scholar
Gendron, M., Lindquist, K. A., Barsalou, L., & Barrett, L. F. (2012). Emotion words shape emotion percepts. Emotion, 12, 314–25.Google Scholar
Gendron, M., Roberson, D., van der Vyver, J. M., & Barrett, L. F. (2014). Perceptions of emotion from facial expressions are not culturally universal: Evidence from a remote culture. Emotion, 14, 251–62.Google Scholar
George, J. M. (1990). Personality, affect, and behavior in groups. Journal of Applied Psychology, 75, 107–16.Google Scholar
George, M. (2008). Interactions in expert service work. Journal of Contemporary Ethnography, 37, 108–31.Google Scholar
Gergely, G., & Watson, J. (1996). The social biofeedback theory of parental affect-mirroring: The development of emotional self-awareness and self-control in infancy. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 77, 1181–212.Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Gilbert, D. T., & Malone, P. S. (1995). The correspondence bias. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 2138.Google Scholar
Gilbert, D. T, Pelham, B. W., & Krull, D. S. (1988). On cognitive busyness: When person perceivers meet persons perceived. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 733–40.Google Scholar
Ginsburg, S. G. (1974). The problem of the burned-out executive. Personnel Journal, 48, 598600.Google Scholar
Gladstones, W. H. (1962). A multidimensional scaling study of facial expression of emotion. Australian Journal of Psychology, 14, 95100.Google Scholar
Gleichgerrcht, E., & Decety, J. (2012). The costs of empathy among health professionals. In Decety, J. (Ed.), Empathy: From bench to bedside (pp. 245–61). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gleichgerrcht, E., & Decety, J. (2013). Empathy in clinical practice: How individual dispositions, gender, and experience moderate empathic concern, burnout, and emotional distress in physicians. PLoS ONE, 8, e61526.Google Scholar
Gleichgerrcht, E., & Decety, J. (2014). The relationship between different facets of empathy, pain perception and compassion fatigue among physicians. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 19.Google Scholar
Glomb, T. M., & Tews, M. J. (2004). Emotional labor: A conceptualization and scale development. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 64, 123.Google Scholar
Godinho, R. M., Spikins, P., & O’Higgins, P. (2018). Supraorbital morphology and social dynamics in human evolution. Nature Ecology & Evolution, 2956–61.Google Scholar
Goffman, E. (1959). The presentation of self in everyday life. New York: Doubleday.Google Scholar
Goffman, E. (1961). Encounters: Two studies in the sociology of interaction. Indianapolis, IN: The Bobbs-Merrill Company.Google Scholar
Goldenberg, A., Halperin, E., van Zomeren, M., & Gross, J. J. (2016). The process model of group‐based emotion: Integrating intergroup emotion and emotion regulation perspectives. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 20, 118–41.Google Scholar
Goodwin, M. H., Cekaite, A., & Goodwin, C. (2012). Emotion as stance. In Peräkylä, A., & Sorjonen, M.-L. (Eds.), Emotion in interaction (pp. 1641). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gordon, R. M. (1974). The aboutness of emotions. American Philosophical Quarterly, 11, 1736.Google Scholar
Grandey, A. A. (2000). Emotion regulation in the workplace: A new way to conceptualize emotional labor. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 5, 95110.Google Scholar
Grandey, A. (2003). When ‘the show must go on’: Surface acting and deep acting as predictors of emotional exhaustion and peer-rated service delivery. Academy of Management Journal, 46, 8696.Google Scholar
Grandey, A., Dickter, D. N., & Sin, H. P. (2004). The customer is not always right: Customer aggression and emotion regulation of service employees. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 25, 397418.Google Scholar
Grandey, A., Fisk, G., Mattila, A., Jansen, K. J., & Sideman, L. (2005). Is service with a smile enough? Authenticity of positive displays during service encounters. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 96, 3855.Google Scholar
Grandey, A. A., & Melloy, R. C. (2017). The state of the heart: Emotional labor as emotion regulation reviewed and revised. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 22, 407–22.Google Scholar
Granström, K. (2011). Cheering as an indicator of social identity and self regulation in Swedish ice hockey supporter groups. International Review for the Sociology of Sport, 47, 133–48.Google Scholar
Greene, G. (1961). A burnt out case. London: Heinemann.Google Scholar
Grice, H. P. (1975). Logic and conversation. In Cole, P. & Morgan, J. (Eds.), Syntax and semantics 3: Speech acts (pp. 4158). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Griffith, J., Connelly, S., Thiel, C., & Johnson, G. (2015). How outstanding leaders lead with affect: An examination of charismatic, ideological, and pragmatic leaders. Leadership Quarterly, 26, 502–17.Google Scholar
Griffiths, P.E. (1997). What emotions really are. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Griffiths, P. E., & Scarantino, A. (2009). Emotions in the wild: The situated perspective on emotion. In Robbins, P. & Aydede, M. (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of situated cognition (pp. 437–53). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gross, J. J. (1998a). The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Review of General Psychology, 3, 271–99.Google Scholar
Gross, J. J. (1998b). Antecedent- and response-focused emotion regulation: Divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 224–37.Google Scholar
Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion regulation: Current status and future prospects. Psychological Inquiry, 26, 126.Google Scholar
Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 348–62.Google Scholar
Gross, J. J., & Levenson, R. W. (1993). Emotional suppression: Physiology, self-report, and expressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 970–86.Google Scholar
Gross, J. J., Richards, J. M., & John, O. P. (2006). Emotion regulation in everyday life. In Snyder, D. K., Simpson, J. A. & Hughes, J. N. (Eds.), Emotion regulation in families: Pathways to dysfunction and health (pp. 1335). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Groth, M., & Grandey, A. A. (2012). From bad to worse: Negative exchange spirals in employee-customer service interactions. Organizational Psychology Review, 2, 208–33.Google Scholar
Groth, M., Hennig-Thurau, T., & Walsh, G. (2009). Customer reactions to emotional labor: The roles of employee acting strategy and customer detection accuracy. Academy of Management Journal, 52, 958–74.Google Scholar
Gump, B. B., & Kulik, J. A. (1997). Stress, affiliation, and emotional contagion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 305–19.Google Scholar
Gunnery, S., Hall, J., and Ruben, M. (2013). The deliberate Duchenne smile: Individual differences in expressive control. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 37, 2941.Google Scholar
Gyurak, A., Gross, J. J., & Etkin, A. (2011). Explicit and implicit emotion regulation: A dual-process framework. Cognition and Emotion, 25, 400–12.Google Scholar
Hackenbracht, J., & Tamir, M. (2010). Preferences for sadness when eliciting help: Instrumental motives in sadness regulation. Motivation and Emotion, 34, 306–15.Google Scholar
Hadjikhani, N., Hoge, R., Snyder, J., & de Gelder, B. (2008). Pointing with the eyes: The role of gaze in communicating danger. Brain and Cognition, 68, 18.Google Scholar
Hagen, E. H., & Bryant, G. A. (2003). Music and dance as a coalition signaling system. Human Nature, 14, 2151.Google Scholar
Hains, S. C., Hogg, M. A., & Duck, J. M. (1997). Self-categorization and leadership: Effects of group prototypicality and leader stereotypicality. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 1087–99.Google Scholar
Halberstadt, A. G., & Green, L. R. (1993). Social attention and placation theories of blushing. Motivation and Emotion, 17, 5364.Google Scholar
Halberstadt, J., Winkielmann, P., Niedenthal, P. M., & Dalle, N. (2009). Emotional conception: How embodied emotion concepts guide perception and facial action. Psychological Science, 20, 1254–61.Google Scholar
Halperin, E. (2016). Emotions in conflict: Inhibitors and facilitators of peace making. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Halpern, J. (2001). From detached concern to empathy: Humanizing medical practice. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Handford, C., Lemon, J., Grimm, M. C., & Vollmer-Conna, U. (2013). Empathy as a function of clinical exposure: Reading emotion in the eyes. PLoS ONE, 8, e65159.Google Scholar
Hansen, C. H., & Hansen, R. D. (1988). Finding the face-in-the-crowd: An anger superiority effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 917–24.Google Scholar
Hareli, S. (2014). Making sense of the social world and influencing it by using a naïve attribution theory of emotions. Emotion Review, 6, 336–43.Google Scholar
Hareli, S., & Hess, U. (2010). What emotional reactions can tell us about the nature of others: An appraisal perspective on person perception. Cognition and Emotion, 24, 128–40.Google Scholar
Hareli, S., & Parkinson, B. (2008). What’s social about social emotions? Journal for the Theory of Social Behavior, 38, 131–56.Google Scholar
Hareli, S., & Rafaeli, A. (2008). Emotion cycles: On the social influence of emotions in organizations. Research in Organizational Behavior, 28, 3559.Google Scholar
Harré, R., & Finlay-Jones, R. (1986). Emotion talk across times. In Harré, R. (Ed.), The social construction of emotions (pp. 220–33). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Harris, C. R., & Alvarado, N. (2005). Facial expressions, smile types, and self-report during humour, tickle, and pain. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 655–69.Google Scholar
Hassin, R. R., Aviezer, H., & Bentin, S. (2013). Inherently ambiguous: Facial expressions of emotions, in context. Emotion Review, 5, 60–5.Google Scholar
Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Rapson, R. L. (1994). Emotional contagion. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hayes, S. C., Wilson, K. G., Gifford, E. V., Follette, V. M., & Strosahl, K. (1996). Experiential avoidance and behavioral disorders: A functional dimensional approach to diagnosis and treatment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 1152–68.Google Scholar
Heider, A., & Warner, R. S. (2010). Bodies in sync: Interaction Ritual Theory applied to Sacred Harp singing. Sociology of Religion, 71, 7697.Google Scholar
Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Heinemann, L. V., & Heinemann, T. (2017). Burnout research: Emergence and scientific investigation of a contested diagnosis. Sage Open, 7, article 2158244017697154.Google Scholar
Henkel, A. P., Boegershausen, J., Rafaeli, A., & Lemmink, J. (2017). The social dimension of service interactions: Observer reactions to customer incivility. Journal of Service Research, 20, 120–34.Google Scholar
Hennig-Thurau, T., Groth, M., Paul, M., & Gremler, D. D. (2006). Are all smiles created equal? How emotional contagion and emotional labor affect service relationships. Journal of Marketing, 70, 5873.Google Scholar
Henriques, J. (2010). The vibrations of affect and their propagation on a night out on Kingston’s dancehall scene. Body & Society, 16, 5789.Google Scholar
Hepburn, A., & Potter, J. (2012). Crying and crying responses. In Peräkylä, A., & Sorjonen, M.-L. (Eds.), Emotion in interaction (pp. 195211). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hess, U. (2014). Anger is a positive emotion. In Parrott, W. G. (Ed.), The positive side of negative emotions (pp. 5575). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Hess, U., Adams, R. B., Grammer, K., & Kleck, R. E. (2009). Face gender and emotion expression: Are angry women more like men? Journal of Vision, 9, 18.Google Scholar
Hess, U., Adams, R. B. Jr., & Kleck, R. E. (2005). Who may frown and who should smile? Dominance, affiliation, and the display of happiness and anger. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 515–36.Google Scholar
Hess, U., Adams, R. B. Jr., & Kleck, R. E. (2009). The face is not an empty canvas: How facial expressions interact with facial appearance. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society London B, 364, 3497–504.Google Scholar
Hess, U., Banse, R., & Kappas, A. (1995). The intensity of facial expression is determined by underlying affective state and social situation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 280–8.Google Scholar
Hess, U., & Blairy, S. (2001). Facial mimicry and emotional contagion to dynamic emotional facial expressions and their influence on decoding accuracy. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 40, 129–41.Google Scholar
Hess, U., Blaison, C., & Kafetsios, K., (2016). Judging facial emotion expressions in context: The influence of culture and self-construal orientation. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 40, 5564.Google Scholar
Hess, U., & Fischer, A. (2013). Emotional mimicry as social regulation. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 17, 142–57.Google Scholar
Hess, U., & Fischer, A. (2016). Emotional mimicry in social context. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hess, U., & Hareli, S. (2018). On the malleability of the meaning of contexts: The influence of another person’s emotion expressions on situation perception. Cognition and Emotion, 32, 185–91.Google Scholar
Heyes, C. M. (2005). Imitation by association. In Hurley, S. & Chater, N. (Eds.), Perspectives on imitation: From neuroscience to social science (pp. 157–76). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hinde, R. A. (1981). Animal signals: Ethological and games-theory approaches are not incompatible. Animal Behaviour, 29, 535–42.Google Scholar
Hinde, R. A. (1985). Was ‘the expression of the emotions’ a misleading phrase? Animal Behavior, 33, 985–92.Google Scholar
Hobson, P. (2002). The cradle of thought: Exploring the origins of thinking. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Hochschild, A. R. (1979). Emotion work, feeling rules, and social structure. American Journal of Sociology, 85, 551–75.Google Scholar
Hochschild, A. R. (1983). The managed heart: Commercialization of human feeling. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Hodges, S. D., & Biswas-Diener, R. (2007). Balancing the empathy expense account: Strategies for regulating empathic response. In Farrow, T. F. D. & Woodruff, P. W. R. (Eds.), Empathy in mental illness (pp. 389405). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hofmann, W., De Houwer, J., Perugini, M., Baeyens, F., & Crombez, G. (2010). Evaluative conditioning in humans: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 390421.Google Scholar
Hogg, M. (2001). A social identity theory of leadership. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5, 184200.Google Scholar
Hogg, M. A., Turner, J. C., & Davidson, B. (1990). Polarized norms and social frames of reference: A test of the self-categorization theory of group polarization. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 11, 77100.Google Scholar
Hogg, M., & Van Knippenberg, D. (2003). Social identity and leadership processes in groups. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 152.Google Scholar
Holman, D. (2016). How does customer affiliative behaviour shape the outcomes of employee emotion regulation? A daily diary study of supermarket checkout operators. Human Relations, 69, 1139–62.Google Scholar
Holodynski, M., & Friedlmeier, W. (2006). Development of emotions and emotion regulation. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Hoogland, C. E., Schurtz, D. R., Cooper, C. M., Combs, D. J., Brown, E. G., & Smith, R. H. (2015). The joy of pain and the pain of joy: In-group identification predicts schadenfreude and gluckschmerz following rival groups’ fortunes. Motivation and Emotion39, 260–81.Google Scholar
Hopkins, N., Reicher, S., Khan, S. S., Tewari, S., Srinivasan, N., & Stevenson, C. (2016). Explaining effervescence: Investigating the relationship between shared social identity and positive experience in crowds. Cognition and Emotion, 30, 2032.Google Scholar
Hornsey, M. J., Okimoto, T. G., & Wenzel, M. (2017). The appraisal gap: Why victim and transgressor groups disagree on the need for a collective apology. European Journal of Social Psychology, 47, 135–47.Google Scholar
Houston, L., III, Grandey, A., & Sawyer, K. (2018). Who cares if ‘service with a smile’ is authentic? An expectancy-based model of customer race and differential service reactions. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 144, 8596.Google Scholar
Howell, A. J., Turowski, J. B., & Buro, K. (2012). Guilt, empathy, and apology. Personality and Individual Differences, 53, 917–22.Google Scholar
Howell, J. M., & Shamir, B. (2005). The role of followers in the charismatic leadership process: Relationships and their consequences. Academy of Management Review, 30, 96112.Google Scholar
Hülsheger, U. R., Lang, J. W. B., & Maier, G. W. (2010). Emotional labor, strain, and performance: Testing reciprocal relationships in a longitudinal panel study. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 15, 505–21.Google Scholar
Hülsheger, U. R., & Schewe, A. F. (2011). On the costs and benefits of emotional labor: A meta-analysis of three decades of research. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 16, 361–89.Google Scholar
Hulsman, R. L., Pranger, S., Koot, S., Fabriek, M., Karemaker, J. M., & Smets, E. M. A. (2010). How stressful is doctor-patient communication? Physiological and psychological stress of medical students in simulated history taking and bad-news consultations. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 77, 2634.Google Scholar
Humphrey, R. H., Burch, G. F., and Adams, L. L. (2016). The benefits of merging leadership research and emotions research. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, article 1022.Google Scholar
Humphrey, R. H., Pollack, J. M., & Hawver, T. (2008). Leading with emotional labor. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 23, 151–68.Google Scholar
Huntsinger, J. R., Isbell, L. M., & Clore, G. L. (2014). The affective control of thought: Malleable, not fixed. Psychological Review, 121, 600–18.Google Scholar
Huntsinger, J. R., & Ray, C. (2016). A flexible influence of affective feelings on creative and analytic performance. Emotion, 16, 826–37.Google Scholar
Hutcherson, C. A., & Gross, J. J. (2011). The moral emotions: A social-functionalist account of anger, disgust, and contempt. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100, 719–37.Google Scholar
Hutchins, E. (1995). Cognition in the wild. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hutson, S. P., Hall, J. M., & Pack, F. L. (2015). Survivor guilt: Analyzing the concept and its contexts. ANS Advances in Nursing Science, 38, 2033.Google Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Hedges, L. V., & Vevea, J. L. (2000). Why do categories affect stimulus judgment? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 220–41.Google Scholar
Ioannou, M., Al-Ramiah, A., & Hewstone, M. (2017). An experimental comparison of direct and indirect intergroup contact. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 76, 393403.Google Scholar
Isen, A. M., Daubman, K. A., & Nowicki, G. P. (1987). Positive affect facilitates creative problem solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 1122–31.Google Scholar
Ito, T. A., Chino, K. W., Devine, P. G., Lorig, T. S., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2006). The influence of facial feedback on race bias. Psychological Science, 17, 256–61.Google Scholar
Iyer, A., & Leach, C. W. (2008). Emotion in intergroup relations. European Review of Social Psychology, 19, 86125.Google Scholar
Iyer, A., Schmader, T., & Lickel, B. (2007). Why individuals protest the perceived transgressions of their country: The role of anger, shame, and guilt. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 572–87.Google Scholar
Izard, C. (1971). The face of emotion. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Jack, R. E., Garrod, O. G. B., Yu, H., Caldara, R., & Schyns, P. G. (2012). Facial expressions of emotion are not culturally universal. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109, 7241–4.Google Scholar
Jahoda, M. (1989). Why a non-reductionist social psychology is almost too difficult to be tackled but too fascinating to be left alone. British Journal of Social Psychology, 28, 71–8.Google Scholar
Jakobs, E., Manstead, A. S. R., & Fischer, A. H. (1999). Social motives and emotional feelings as determinants of facial displays: The case of smiling. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 424–35.Google Scholar
Jakobs, E., Manstead, A. S. R., & Fischer, A. H. (2001). Social context effects on acial activity in a negative emotional setting. Emotion, 1, 5169.Google Scholar
James, W. (1884). What is an emotion? Mind, 9, 188205.Google Scholar
James, W. (1898). The principles of psychology (Vol. 2). London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Jäncke, L., & Kaufmann, N. (1994). Facial EMG responses to odors in solitude and with an audience. Chemical Senses, 19, 99111.Google Scholar
Jänig, W. (2006). The integrative action of the autonomic nervous system: Neurobiology of homeostasis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Jefferson, G. (1985). An exercise in the transcription and analysis of laughter. In Van Dijk, A. & Teun, A. (Eds.), Handbook of discourse analysis (Vol. 3, pp. 2534). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Jefferson, G. (2004). Glossary of transcript symbols with an introduction. In Lerner, G. H. (Ed.), Conversation analysis: Studies from the first generation (pp. 1331). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Johnson, S., & Jacob, T. (2000). Sequential interactions in the marital communication of depressed men and women. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 412.Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N., & Oatley, K. (1989). The language of emotions: An analysis of a semantic field. Cognition and Emotion, 3, 81123.Google Scholar
Joinson, C. (1992). Coping with compassion fatigue. Nursing, 22, 116–22.Google Scholar
Jones, E. E., Wood, G. C., & Quattrone, G. A. (1981). Perceived variability of personal characteristics in ingroups and outgroups: The role of knowledge and evaluation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 7, 523–8.Google Scholar
Jones, S. (1996). Imitation or exploration? Young infants’ matching of adults’ oral gestures. Child Development, 67, 1952–69.Google Scholar
Judge, T. A., & Bono, J. E. (2000). Five-factor model of personality and transformational leadership. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 751–65.Google Scholar
Judge, T. A., Piccolo, R. F., & Kosalka, T. (2009). The bright and dark sides of leader traits: A review and theoretical extension of the leader trait paradigm. Leadership Quarterly, 20, 855–75.Google Scholar
Judge, T. A., Woolf, E. F., & Hurst, C. (2009). Is emotional labor more difficult for some than for others? A multi-level, experience sampling study. Personnel Psychology, 62, 5788.Google Scholar
Kafetsios, K., Hantzara, K., Anagnostopoulos, F., & Niakas, D. (2016). Doctors’ attachment orientations, emotion regulation strategies, and patient satisfaction: A multilevel analysis. Health Communication, 31, 772–7.Google Scholar
Kalokerinos, E. K., Tamir, M., & Kuppens, P. (2017). Instrumental motives in negative emotion regulation in daily life: Frequency, consistency, and predictors. Emotion, 17, 648–57.Google Scholar
Kamachi, M., Bruce, V., Mukaida, S., Gyoba, J., Yoshikawa, S., & Akamatsu, S. (2001). Dynamic properties influence the perception of facial expressions. Perception, 30, 875–87.Google Scholar
Kameda, T., Ohtsubo, Y., & Takezawa, M. (1997). Centrality in socio-cognitive networks and social influence: An illustration in a group decision-making context. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 296309.Google Scholar
Kammeyer-Mueller, J. D., Rubenstein, A. L., Long, D. M., Odio, M. A., Buckman, B. R., Zhang, Y., & Halvorsen-Ganepola, M. D. K. (2013). A meta-analytic structural model of dispositional affectivity and emotional labor. Personnel Psychology, 66, 4790.Google Scholar
Kampfe, N., & Mitte, K. (2009). What you wish is what you get? The meaning of individual variability in desired affect and affective discrepancy. Journal of Research in Personality, 43, 409–18.Google Scholar
Kappas, A. (2011). Emotion and regulation are one! Emotion Review, 3, 1725.Google Scholar
Kayyal, M., Widen, S., & Russell, J. A. (2015). Context is more powerful than we think: Contextual cues override facial cues even for valence. Emotion, 15, 287–91.Google Scholar
Keating, C. F., & Doyle, J. (2002). The faces of desirable mates and dates contain mixed social status cues. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 414–24.Google Scholar
Keltner, D. (1995). Signs of appeasement: Evidence for the distinct displays of embarrassment, amusement, and shame. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 441–54.Google Scholar
Keltner, D., & Buswell, B. N. (1997). Embarrassment: Its distinct form and appeasement function. Psychological Bulletin, 122, 250–70.Google Scholar
Keltner, D., & Cordaro, D. T. (2017). Understanding multimodal emotional expressions. In Fernandez-Dols, J.-M. & Russell, J. A. (Eds.), The science of facial expression (pp. 5775). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Keltner, D., Gruenfeld, D. H., & Anderson, C. (2003). Power, approach, and inhibition. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 265–84.Google Scholar
Keltner, D., & Haidt, J. (1999). Social functions of emotions at four levels of analysis. Cognition and Emotion, 13, 505–21.Google Scholar
Keltner, D., & Haidt, J. (2003). Approaching awe, a moral, spiritual, and aesthetic emotion. Cognition and Emotion, 17, 297314.Google Scholar
Keltner, D., Haidt, J., & Shiota, M. N. (2006). Social functionalism and the evolution of emotions. In Schaller, M., Simpson, J. A. & Kenrick, D. T. (Eds.), Evolution and social psychology (pp. 115–42). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Keltner, D., Van Kleef, G. A., Chen, S., & Kraus, M. (2008). A reciprocal influence model of social power: Emerging principles and lines of inquiry. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 151–92.Google Scholar
Kendon, A. (1967). Some functions of gaze direction in social interaction. Acta Psychologica, 32, 125.Google Scholar
Kiesler, S., Siegel, J., & McGuire, T. W. (1984). Social psychological aspects of computer-mediated communication. American Psychologist, 39, 1123–34.Google Scholar
Kim, E., & Yoon, D. J. (2012). Why does service with a smile make employees happy? A social interaction model. Journal of Applied Psychology, 97, 1059–67.Google Scholar
Kim, P. H., Mislin, A., Tuncel, E., Fehr, R., Cheshin, A., & van Kleef, G. A. (2017). Power as an emotional liability: Implications for perceived authenticity and trust after a transgression. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 146, 1379–401.Google Scholar
Klineberg, O. (1940). Social psychology. New York: Holt.Google Scholar
Klinnert, M. D., Campos, J. J., Sorce, J. F., Emde, R. N., & Svejda, M. (1983). Emotions as behavior regulators: Social referencing in infancy. In Plutchik, R. & Kellerman, H. (Eds.), Emotions: Theory, research and experience (Vol. 2, pp. 5786). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Kobayashi, H., & Kohshima, S. (1997). Unique morphology of the human eye. Nature, 387, 767–8.Google Scholar
Kokkinaki, T. S., Vasdekis, V. G. S., Koufaki, Z. E., & Trevarthen, C. B. (2017). Coordination of emotions in mother-infant dialogues. Infant and Child Development, 26, e1973.Google Scholar
Kölliker, M., Brodie, E. D. III, & Moore, A. J. (2005). The coadaptation of parental supply and offspring demand. The American Naturalist, 166, 506–16.Google Scholar
Konvalinka, I., Xygalatas, D., Bulbulia, J., Schjødt, U., Jegindø, E-M., Wallot, S., Van Ordend, G., & Roepstorff, A. (2011). Synchronized arousal between performers and related spectators in a fire-walking ritual. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108, 8514–19.Google Scholar
Koole, S. (2009). The psychology of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Cognition and Emotion, 23, 441.Google Scholar
Korb, S., Grandjean, D., & Scherer, K. R. (2010). Timing and voluntary suppression of facial mimicry to smiling faces in a go/nogo task: An EMG study. Biological Psychology, 85, 347–9.Google Scholar
Korsvold, L., Lie, H. C., Mellblom, A. V., Ruud, E., Loge, J. H., & Finset, A. (2016). Tailoring the delivery of cancer diagnosis to adolescent and young adult patients displaying strong emotions: An observational study of two cases. International Journal of Qualitative Studies on Health and Well-Being, 11, article 30763.Google Scholar
Kotsoni, E., de Haan, M., & Johnson, M. H. (2001). Categorical perception of facial expressions by 7-month-old infants. Perception, 30, 1115–25.Google Scholar
Kövecses, Z. (1990). Emotion concepts. New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Kragel, P. A., & LaBar, K. S. (2016). Decoding the nature of emotion in the brain. Trends in Cognitive Science, 20, 444–55.Google Scholar
Kramer, A. D. I., Guillory, J. E., & Hancock, J. T. (2014). Experimental evidence of massive-scale emotional contagion through social networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111, 8788–90.Google Scholar
Kraut, R. E., & Johnston, R. E. (1979). Social and emotional messages of smiling: An ethological approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1539–53.Google Scholar
Kreibig, S. D. (2010). Autonomic nervous system activity in emotion: A review. Biological Psychology, 84, 394421.Google Scholar
Kristensen, T. S., Borritz, M., Villadsen, E., & Christensen, K. B. (2005). The Copenhagen burnout inventory: A new tool for the assessment of burnout. Work and Stress, 19, 192207.Google Scholar
Kross, E., & Ayduk, Ö. (2011). Making meaning out of negative experiences by self-distancing. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 187–91.Google Scholar
Krull, D. S. (1993). Does the grist change the mill? The effect of the perceiver’s inferential goal on the process of social inference. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 340–8.Google Scholar
Krumhuber, E. G., Kappas, A., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2013). Effects of dynamic aspects of facial expressions: A review. Emotion Review, 5, 41–6.Google Scholar
Krumhuber, E. G., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2009). Can Duchenne smiles be feigned? New evidence on felt and false smiles. Emotion, 9, 807–20.Google Scholar
Kuppens, P., Van Mechelen, I., Smits, D. J. M., & De Boeck, P. (2003). The appraisal basis of anger: Specificity, necessity and sufficiency of components. Emotion, 3, 254–69.Google Scholar
Kuppens, T., Yzerbyt, V. Y., Dandache, S., Fischer, A. H., & van der Schalk, J. (2013). Social identity salience shapes group-based emotions through group-based appraisals. Cognition and Emotion, 27, 1359–77.Google Scholar
Laird, J. D. (1974). Self-attribution of emotion: The effects of expressive behavior on the quality of emotional experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 29, 473–86.Google Scholar
Laird, J. D., & Bresler, C. (1992). The process of emotional experience: A self-perception theory. In Clark, M. S. (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology 13: Emotion (pp. 213–34). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Lakin, J. L., Chartrand, T. L., & Arkin, R. M. (2008). I am too just like you: The effects of ostracism on nonconscious mimicry. Psychological Science, 19, 816–22.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, fire, and dangerous things: What categories reveal about the mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G., & Kövecses, Z. (1987). The cognitive model of anger inherent in American English. In Holland, D. & Quinn, N. (Eds.), Cultural models in language and thought (pp. 195221). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lambie, J., & Marcel, A. (2002). Consciousness and emotion experience: A theoretical framework. Psychological Review, 109, 219–59.Google Scholar
Lamm, H., & Myers, D. G. (1978). Group-induced polarization of attitudes and behavior. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 11, 145–95.Google Scholar
Lammers, J., Galinsky, A. D., Gordijn, E. H., & Otten, S. (2008). Illegitimacy moderates the effects of power on approach. Psychological Science, 19, 557–64.Google Scholar
Lane, A. M., Beedie, C. J., Davenport, T. J., & Stanley, D. M. (2011). Instrumental emotion regulation in sport: Relationships between beliefs about emotion and emotion regulation strategies used by athletes. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine and Science in Sports, 21, 445–51.Google Scholar
Larsen, R. J. (2000). Toward a science of mood regulation. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 129–41.Google Scholar
Latané, B., & Darley, J. M. (1968). Group inhibition of bystander intervention in emergencies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 10, 215–21.Google Scholar
Lazarus, R. S. (1966). Psychological stress and the coping process. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Lazarus, R.S. (1984). On the primacy of cognition. American Psychologist, 39, 124–9.Google Scholar
Lazarus, R.S. (1991a). Emotion and adaptation. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lazarus, R.S. (1991b). Cognition and motivation in emotion. American Psychologist, 46, 352–67.Google Scholar
Lazarus, R.S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Leach, C. W., Iyer, A., & Pedersen, A. (2006). Guilt and anger about in-group advantage as explanations of the willingness for political action. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 1232–45.Google Scholar
Leach, C. W., & Spears, R. (2009). Dejection at in-group defeat and schadenfreude toward second-and third-party out-groups. Emotion9, 659–65.Google Scholar
Leach, C. W., Spears, R., Branscombe, N. R., & Doosje, B. (2003). Malicious pleasure: Schadenfreude at the suffering of an outgroup. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 932–43.Google Scholar
Leary, M. R. (1983). A brief version of the Fear of Negative Evaluation Scale. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 9, 371–5.Google Scholar
Leary, M. R., Britt, T. W., Cutlip , W. D., & Templeton, J. L. (1992). Social blushing. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 446–60.Google Scholar
Leary, M. R, Landel, J. L., & Patton, K. M. (1996). The motivated expression of embarrassment following a self-presentational predicament. Journal of Personality, 64, 619–36.Google Scholar
Le Bon, G. (1895, tr. 1947). The crowd: A study of the popular mind. London: Ernest Benn.Google Scholar
Lee, D. H., Susskind, J. M., & Anderson, A. K. (2013). Social transmission of the sensory benefits of eye widening in fear expression. Psychological Science, 24, 957–65.Google Scholar
Leighton, J., Bird, G., Orsini, C., & Heyes, C. M. (2010). Social attitudes modulate automatic imitation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 905–10.Google Scholar
Lejuez, C. W., Read, J. P., Kahler, C. W., Richards, J. B., Ramsey, S. E., Stuart, G. L., Strong, D. R., & Brown, R. A. (2002). Evaluation of a behavioral measure of risk taking: The Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8, 7584.Google Scholar
Lelieveld, G.-J., Van Dijk, E., Van Beest, I., Steinel, W., & Van Kleef, G. A. (2011). Disappointed in you, angry about your offer: Distinct negative emotions induce concessions via different mechanisms. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 635–41.Google Scholar
Lelieveld, G.-J., Van Dijk, E., Van Beest, I., & Van Kleef, G. A. (2012). Why anger and disappointment affect others’ bargaining behavior differently: The moderating role of power and the mediating role of reciprocal and complementary emotions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38, 1209–21.Google Scholar
Lelieveld, G.-J., Van Dijk, E., Van Beest, I., & Van Kleef, G. A. (2013). Does communicating disappointment in negotiations help or hurt? Solving an apparent inconsistency in the social-functional approach to emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 105, 605–20.Google Scholar
Levenson, R. W. (1988). Emotion and the autonomic nervous system: A prospectus for research on autonomic specificity. In Wagner, H. L. (Ed.), Social psychophysiology and emotion: Theory and clinical applications (pp. 1742). Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
Levenson, R. W., & Gottman, J. M. (1983). Marital interaction: Physiological linkage and affective exchange. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 587–97.Google Scholar
Leventhal, H., & Mace, W. (1970). The effect of laughter on evaluation of a slapstick movie. Journal of Personality, 38, 1630.Google Scholar
Leventhal, H., & Scherer, K. R. (1987). The relationship of emotion and cognition: A functional approach to a semantic controversy. Cognition and Emotion, 1, 328.Google Scholar
Levinson, S. C. (1983). Pragmatics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Levy, R. (1973). Tahitians. Chicago, IL: Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
Lewis, K. M. (2000). When leaders display emotion: How followers respond to negative emotional expression of male and female leaders. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 21, 221−34.Google Scholar
Lewis, M. D., & Liu, Z. (2011). Three time-scales of neural self-organization underlying human basic and non-basic emotions. Emotion Review, 3, 416–23.Google Scholar
Leyens, J.-P., Paladino, P. M., Rodriguez-Torres, R., Vaes, J., Demoulin, S., Rodriguez-Perez, A., & Gaunt, R. (2000). The emotional side of prejudice: The role of secondary emotions. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4, 186–97.Google Scholar
Leyens, J.-P., Rodriguez-Perez, A., Rodriguez-Torres, R., Gaunt, R., Paladino, M. P., Vaes, J., & Demoulin, S. (2001). Psychological essentialism and the differential attribution of uniquely human emotions to ingroups and outgroups. European Journal of Social Psychology, 31, 395411.Google Scholar
Lipps, T. (1907). Das Wissen von fremden Ichen. In Lipps, T. (Ed.), Psychologische Untersuchungen (Band 1, pp. 694722). Leipzig: Engelmann.Google Scholar
Lief, H. I., & Fox, R. C. (1963). Training for ‘detached concern’ in medical students. In Lief, H. I., Lief, V. F. & Lief, N. R. (Eds.), The psychological basis of medical practice (pp. 1235). New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Little, L. M., Gooty, J., & Williams, M. (2016). The role of leader emotion management in leader-member exchange and follower outcomes. Leadership Quarterly, 27, 8597.Google Scholar
Lively, K. J. (2000). Reciprocal emotion management: Working together to maintain stratification in private law firms. Work and Occupations, 27, 3263.Google Scholar
Livingstone, A. G., Spears, R., Manstead, A. S. R., Bruder, M., & Shepherd, L. (2011). We feel, therefore we are: Emotion as a basis for self-categorization and social action. Emotion, 11, 754–67.Google Scholar
Livingstone, A. G., Shepherd, L., Spears, R., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2016). ‘Fury, us’: Anger as a basis for new group self‐categories. Cognition and Emotion, 30, 183–92.Google Scholar
Lukács, G. (1971). History and class consciousness: Studies in Marxist dialectics. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Lutz, C. A. (1982). The domain of emotion words on Ifaluk. American Ethnologist, 9, 113–28.Google Scholar
Lutz, C. A. (1988). Unnatural emotions: Everyday sentiments on a micronesian atoll and their challenge to western theory. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lutz, C. A., & Abu-Lughod, L. (1990). Language and the politics of emotion. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Mackie, D. M. (1986). Social identification effects in group polarization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 720–8.Google Scholar
Mackie, D. M., & Cooper, J. (1984). Attitude polarization: Effects of group membership. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 575–86.Google Scholar
Mackie, D. M., Devos, T., & Smith, E. R. (2000). Intergroup emotions: Explaining offensive action tendencies in an intergroup context. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 602–16.Google Scholar
Mackintosh, N. J. (2011). IQ and human intelligence (2nd edition). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Mahoney, S., Sladek, R. M., & Neild, T. (2016). A longitudinal study of empathy in preclinical and clinical medical students and clinical supervisors. BMC Medical Education, 16, 270.Google Scholar
Mann, L., Feddes, A. R., Doosje, B., & Fischer, A. H. (2016). Withdraw or affiliate? The role of humiliation during initiation rituals. Cognition and Emotion, 30, 80100.Google Scholar
Manstead, A. S. R., & Fischer, A. H. (2001). Social appraisal: The social world as object of and influence on appraisal processes. In Scherer, K. R., Schorr, A. & Johnstone, T. (Eds.), Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, methods, research (pp. 221–32). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Manstead, A. S. R., & Wagner, H. L. (1981). Arousal, cognition and emotion: An appraisal of two-factor theory. Current Psychological Reviews, 1, 3554.Google Scholar
Maringer, M., Krumhuber, E. G., Fischer, A. H., & Niedenthal, P. M. (2011). Beyond smile dynamics: Mimicry and beliefs in judgments of smiles. Emotion, 11, 181–7.Google Scholar
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224–53.Google Scholar
Marsh, A. A., Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2003). Nonverbal ‘accents’: Cultural differences in facial expressions of emotion. Psychological Science, 14, 373–6.Google Scholar
Marshall, G., & Zimbardo, P. G. (1979). Affective consequences of inadequately explained physiological arousal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 970–88.Google Scholar
Martin, J., Rychlowska, M., Wood, A., & Niedenthal, P. (2017). Smiles as multipurpose social signals. Trends in Cognitive Science, 21, 864–77.Google Scholar
Maslach, C. (1978). The client role in staff burn-out. Journal of Social Issues, 34, 111–24.Google Scholar
Maslach, C. (1979). Negative emotional biasing of unexplained arousal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 953–69.Google Scholar
Maslach, C. (1982). Burnout: The cost of caring. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Maslach, C., & Jackson, S. E. (1981). The measurement of experienced burnout. Journal of Occupational Behavior, 2, 99113.Google Scholar
Maslach, C., Jackson, S. E., & Leiter, M. P. (1996). MBI: The Maslach Burnout Inventory: Manual. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Maslach, C., & Leiter, M. P. (1997). The truth about burnout. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Maslach, C., & Pines, A. (1977). The burn-out syndrome in the day care setting. Child Care Quarterly, 6, 100–13.Google Scholar
Masters, W. H., & Johnson, V. E. (1966). Human sexual response. Boston: Little, Brown and Co.Google Scholar
Masuda, T., Ellsworth, P. C., Mesquita, B., Leu, J., Tanida, S., & Van de Veerdonk, E. (2008). Placing the face in context: Cultural differences in the perception of facial emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 365–81.Google Scholar
Masuda, T., Wang, H., Ishii, K., & Ito, K. (2012). Do surrounding figures’ emotions affect judgment of the target figure’s emotion? Comparing the eye-movement patterns of European Canadians, Asian Canadians, Asian international students, and Japanese. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 6, article 72.Google Scholar
Matsumoto, D., & Ekman, P. (1988). Japanese and Caucasian facial expressions of emotion (JACFEE) [Slides]. San Francisco: San Francisco State University, Department of Psychology, Intercultural and Emotion Research Laboratory.Google Scholar
Matsumoto, D., & Hwang, H. C. (2017). Methodological issues regarding cross-cultural studies of judgments of facial expressions. Emotion Review, 9, 375–82.Google Scholar
Matsumoto, D., & Willingham, B. (2006). The thrill of victory and the agony of defeat: Spontaneous expressions of medal winners of the 2004 Athens Olympic Games. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 568–81.Google Scholar
Matsumoto, D., & Willingham, B. (2009). Spontaneous facial expressions of emotion of blind individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 110.Google Scholar
Mayer, A., & Traüble, B. E. (2012). Synchrony in the onset of mental state understanding across cultures: A study among children in Samoa. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 37, 21–8.Google Scholar
Mazziotta, A., Mummendey, A., & Wright, S. C. (2011). Vicarious intergroup contact effects: Applying social-cognitive theory to intergroup contact research. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 14, 255–74.Google Scholar
McGrath, J. E., & Kelly, J. R. (1986). Time and human interaction: Toward a social psychology of time. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
McGuire, G. M. (2007). Intimate work: A typology of the social support that workers provide to their network members. Work and Occupations, 34, 125–47.Google Scholar
McIntosh, D. M. (1996). Facial feedback hypothesis: Evidence, implications, and directions. Motivation and Emotion, 20, 121–47.Google Scholar
McNeill, A., Pehrson, S., & Stevenson, C. (2017). The rhetorical complexity of competitive and common victimhood in conversational discourse. European Journal of Social Psychology, 47, 167–79.Google Scholar
McNeill, W. (1995). Keeping together in time. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self, and society. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Medler-Liraz, H., & Seger-Guttmann, T. (2018). Authentic emotional displays, leader-member exchange, and emotional exhaustion. Journal of Leadership and Organization Studies, 25, 7684.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198, 7578.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1983). Newborn infants imitate adult facial gestures. Child Development, 54, 702–9.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1997). Explaining facial imitation: a theoretical model. Early Development and Parenting, 6, 179–92.Google Scholar
Mesmer-Magnus, J. R., DeChurch, L. A., & Wax, A. (2012). Moving emotional labor beyond surface and deep acting: A discordance-congruence perspective. Organizational Psychology Review, 2, 653.Google Scholar
Messinger, D. S., Fogel, A., & Dickson, K. L. (1999). What’s in a smile? Developmental Psychology, 33, 701–8.Google Scholar
Miller, R. S. (1992). The nature and severity of self-reported embarrassing circumstances. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18, 190–8.Google Scholar
Miller, R. S. (2004). Emotion as adaptive interpersonal communication: The case of embarrassment. In Tiedens, L. Z. & Leach, C. W. (Eds.), The social life of emotions (pp. 87104). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Mori, M. (1970, tr. 2012, K. F. MacDorman & N. Kageki).The uncanny valley (Bukimi no tani). IEEE Robotics and Automation, 19, 98100.Google Scholar
Morris, J. A., & Feldman, D. C. (1996). The dimensions, antecedents, and consequences of emotional labor. Academy of Management Review, 21, 9861010.Google Scholar
Morsbach, H., & Tyler, W. J. (1986). A Japanese emotion: Amae. In Harré, R. (Ed.), The social construction of emotion (pp. 289308). New York: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Moscovici, S., & Zavalloni, M. (1969). The group as a polarizer of attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 12, 125–35.Google Scholar
Mowrer, O. H. (1947). On the dual nature of learning: A reinterpretation of conditioning and problem solving. Harvard Educational Review, 17, 102–48.Google Scholar
Mumenthaler, C., & Sander, D. (2012). Social appraisal influences recognition of emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102, 1118–35.Google Scholar
Mumenthaler, C., & Sander, D. (2015). Automatic integration of social information in emotion recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144, 392–9.Google Scholar
Munn, N. L. (1940). The effect of knowledge of the situation upon judgment of emotion from facial expressions. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 35, 324–38.Google Scholar
Murray, L., & Trevarthen, C. (1985). Emotional regulation on interactions between two-month-olds and their mothers. In Field, T. M. & Fox, N. A. (Eds.), Social perception in infants (pp. 177–97). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Myers, D. G., Bach, P. J., & Schreiber, B. V. (1974). Normative and informational effects of group interaction. Sociometry, 37, 275–86.Google Scholar
Nadler, A., & Shnabel, N. (2015). Intergroup reconciliation: Instrumental and socio-emotional processes and the needs-based model. European Review of Social Psychology, 26, 93125.Google Scholar
Nagy, E., & Molnár, P. (2004). Homo imitans or homo provocans? Human imprinting model of neonatal imitation. Infant Behavior and Development, 27, 5463.Google Scholar
Nakamura, M., Buck, R., & Kenny, D. A. (1990). Relative contributions of expressive behavior and contextual information to the judgment of the emotional state of another. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 1032–9.Google Scholar
Negishi, K. (2012). Smiling in the post-Fordist ‘affective’ economy. Transformations, 22.Google Scholar
Nelissen, R. M. A., & Zeelenberg, M. (2009). When guilt evokes self-punishment: evidence for the existence of a Dobby Effect. Emotion, 9, 118–22.Google Scholar
Nelson, G. M., & Beach, S. R. H. (1990). Sequential interaction in depression: Effects of depressive behavior on spousal aggression. Behavior Therapy, 21, 167–82.Google Scholar
Nelson, N. L., & Russell, J. A. (2013). Universality revisited. Emotion Review, 5, 815.Google Scholar
Netzer, L., van Kleef, G. A., & Tamir, M. (2015). Interpersonal instrumental emotion regulation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 58, 124135.Google Scholar
Neumann, M., Edelhäuser, F., Tauschel, D., Fischer, M. R., Wirtz, M., Woopen, C., Haramati, A., & Scheffer, C. (2011). Empathy decline and its reasons: A systematic review of studies with medical students and residents. Academic Medicine, 86, 9961009.Google Scholar
Neville, F., & Reicher, S. (2011). The experience of collective participation: Shared identity, relatedness and emotionality. Contemporary Social Science, 6, 377–96.Google Scholar
Newcombe, M. J., & Ashkanasy, N. M. (2002). The role of affect and affective congruence in perceptions of leaders: An experimental study. Leadership Quarterly, 13, 601–14.Google Scholar
Nezlek, J. B., Vansteelandt, K., Van Mechelen, I., & Kuppens, P. (2008). Appraisal-emotion relationships in everyday life. Emotion, 8, 145–50.Google Scholar
Niedenthal, P. M., Mermillod, M., Maringer, M., & Hess, U. (2010). The Simulation of Smiles (SIMS) model: Embodied simulation and the meaning of facial expression. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 417–33.Google Scholar
Niedenthal, P. M., Wood, A., Rychlowska, M., & Korb, S. (2017). Embodied simulation in decoding facial expression. In Fernández-Dols, J.-M. & Russell, J. A. (Eds.), The science of facial expression (pp. 397413). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Niederland, W. G. (1961). The problem of the survivor. Journal of the Hillside Hospital, 10, 233–47.Google Scholar
Niven, K. (2016). Why do people engage in interpersonal emotion regulation at work? Organizational Psychology Review, 6, 305–23.Google Scholar
Niven, K. (2017). The four key characteristics of interpersonal emotion regulation. Current Opinion in Psychology, 17, 8993.Google Scholar
Niven, K., Totterdell, P. A., & Holman, D. (2009). A classification of controlled interpersonal affect regulation strategies. Emotion, 9, 498509.Google Scholar
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B. E., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Rethinking rumination. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 400–24.Google Scholar
Noor, M., Vollhardt, J. R., Mari, S., & Nadler, A. (2017). The social psychology of collective victimhood. European Journal of Social Psychology, 47, 121–34.Google Scholar
Novelli, D., Drury, J., Reicher, S., & Stott, C. (2013). Crowdedness mediates the effect of social identification on positive emotion in a crowd: A survey of two crowd events. PLoS One, 8, e78983.Google Scholar
Oakes, P. J. (1987). The salience of social categories. In Turner, J. C., Hogg, M. A., Oakes, P. J., Reicher, S. D. & Wetherell, M. S. (Eds.), Rediscovering the social group: A self-categorisation theory (pp. 117–41). Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Oatley, K., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1987). Towards a cognitive theory of emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 1, 2950.Google Scholar
Öhman, A., Eriksson, A., & Olofsson, C. (1975). One-trial learning and superior resistance to extinction of autonomic responses conditioned to potentially phobic stimuli. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 88, 619–27.Google Scholar
O’Malley, M. N., & Greenberg, J. (1983). Sex differences in restoring justice: The down payment effect. Journal of Research in Personality, 17, 174–85.Google Scholar
Oostenbroek, J., Suddendorf, T., Nielsen, M., Redshaw, J., Kennedy-Costantini, S., Davis, J., et al. (2016). Comprehensive longitudinal study challenges the existence of neonatal imitation in humans. Current Biology, 26, 1334–8.Google Scholar
Orne, M. T. (1962). On the social psychology of the psychology experiment: With particular reference to demand characteristics and their implications. American Psychologist, 17, 776–83.Google Scholar
Ortony, A., Clore, G. L., & Collins, A. (1988). The cognitive structure of emotions. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ortony, A., Clore, G. L., & Foss, M. (1987). The referential structure of the affective lexicon. Cognitive Science, 11, 361–84.Google Scholar
Osgood, C. E. (1962). Studies on the generality of affective meaning systems. American Psychologist, 17, 1028.Google Scholar
Osgood, C. E. (1969). On the whys and wherefores of E, P, and A. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 12, 194–9.Google Scholar
Osgood, C. E., Suci, G. J., & Tannebaum, P. H. (1957). The measurement of meaning. Urbana: University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
Overall, N. C., Girme, Y. U., Lemay, E. P. Jr., & Hammond, M. D. (2014). Attachment anxiety and reactions to relationship threat: The benefits and costs of inducing guilt in romantic partners. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106, 235–56.Google Scholar
Overbeck, J. R., Neale, M. A., & Govan, C. L. (2010). I feel, therefore you act: Intrapersonal and interpersonal effects of emotion on negotiation as a function of social power. Organizational Behavior & Human Decision Processes, 112, 126–39.Google Scholar
Oyama, S. (1985). The ontogeny of information: Developmental systems and evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Páez, D., Rimé, B., Basabe, N., Wlodarczyk, A., & Zumeta, L. (2015). Psychosocial effects of perceived emotional synchrony in collective gatherings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108, 711–29.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B. (1991). Emotional stylists: Strategies of expressive management among trainee hairdressers. Cognition and Emotion, 5, 419–34.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B. (1995). Ideas and realities of emotion. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B. (1996). Emotions are social. British Journal of Psychology, 87, 663–83.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B. (1997). Untangling the appraisal-emotion connection. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 1, 6279.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B. (1999). Relations and dissociations between appraisal and emotion ratings in reasonable and unreasonable anger and guilt. Cognition and Emotion, 13, 347–85.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B. (2001a). Putting appraisal in context. In Scherer, K. R., Schorr, A. & Johnstone, T. (Eds.), Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, research, application (pp. 173–86). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B. (2001b). Anger on and off the road. British Journal of Psychology, 92, 507–26.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B. (2011). Interpersonal emotion transfer: Contagion and social appraisal. Personality and Social Psychology Compass, 5, 428–39.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B. (2012). Piecing together emotion: Sites and time-scales for social construction. Emotion Review, 4, 290–8.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B. (2013). Journeys to the center of emotion. Emotion Review, 5, 180–4.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B. (2017). Interpersonal effects and functions of facial activity. In Fernández-Dols, J.-M. & Russell, J. A. (Eds.), The science of facial expression (pp. 435–56). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B. (2018). Emotion. In Davey, G. (Ed.), Psychology (pp. 479528). Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B. (2019). Calibrating emotional orientations: Social appraisal and other kinds of relation alignment. In Dukes, D. & Clément, F. (Eds.), Foundations of affective social learning: Conceptualising the transmission of social value (pp. 117141). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B., Fischer, A., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2005). Emotion in social relations: Cultural, group, and interpersonal processes. Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B., & Illingworth, S. (2009). Guilt in response to blame from others. Cognition and Emotion, 23, 1589–614.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B., & Lea, M. F. (1991). Investigating personal constructs of emotion, British Journal of Psychology, 82, 7386.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B., & Lea, M. (2011). Video-linking emotions. In Kappas, A. & Krämer, N. (Eds.), Face-to-face communication over the internet: Issues, research, challenges (pp. 100–26). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B., & Manstead, A. S. R. (1992). Appraisal as a cause of emotion. Review of Personality and Social Psychology, 13, 122–49.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B., & Manstead, A. S. R. (1993). Making sense of emotion in stories and social life. Cognition and Emotion, 7, 295323.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B., Phiri, N., & Simons, G. (2012). Bursting with anxiety: Adult social referencing in an interpersonal Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). Emotion, 12, 817–26.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B., Roper, A., & Simons, G. (2008). Appraisal ratings in diary reports of reasonable and unreasonable anger. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 82–7.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B., & Simons, G. (2009). Affecting others: Social appraisal and emotion contagion in everyday decision-making. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35, 1071–84.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B., & Simons, G. (2012). Worry spreads: Interpersonal transfer of problem-related anxiety. Cognition and Emotion, 26, 462–79.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B., Simons, G., & Niven, K. (2016). Sharing concerns: Interpersonal worry regulation in romantic couples. Emotion, 16, 449–58.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B., & Totterdell, P. (1997). Deliberate affect-regulation strategies: Preliminary data concerning reported effectiveness and frequency of use. In Frijda, N. H. (Ed.), Proceedings of the 9th Meeting of the International Society for Research on Emotions (pp. 401–5). Storrs, CT: ISRE publications.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B., & Totterdell, P. (1999). Classifying affect-regulation strategies. Cognition and Emotion, 13, 277303.Google Scholar
Parrott, W. G. (1993). Beyond hedonism: Motives for inhibiting good moods and for maintaining bad moods. In Wegner, D. M. & Pennebaker, J. W. (Eds.), Handbook of mental control (pp. 278305). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Parrott, W. (2001). Implications of dysfunctional emotion for understanding how emotions function. Review of General Psychology, 5, 180–6.Google Scholar
Pasquarelli, B., & Bull, N. (1951). Experimental investigation of the body-mind continuum in affective states. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 113, 512–21.Google Scholar
Pearce, E., Launay, J., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2015). The ice-breaker effect: Singing mediates fast social bonding. Royal Society Open Science, 2, 150221.Google Scholar
Pehrson, S., Stevenson, C., Muldoon, O. T., & Reicher, S. D. (2013). Is everyone Irish on St Patrick’s Day? Divergent expectations and experiences of collective self-objectification at a multicultural parade. British Journal of Social Psychology, 53, 249–64.Google Scholar
Pescosolido, A. T. (2002). Emergent leaders as managers of group emotion. Leadership Quarterly, 13, 583–99.Google Scholar
Pines, A., & Aronson, E. (1981). Burnout: From tedium to personal growth. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Pollak, K. I., Arnold, R. M., Jeffreys, A. S., Alexander, S. C., Olsen, M. K., Abernethy, A. P., Sugg Skinner, C., Rodriguez, K. L., & Tulsky, J. A. (2007). Oncologist communication about emotion during visits with patients with advanced cancer. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 25, 5748–52.Google Scholar
Pomerantz, A. (1984). Agreeing and disagreeing with assessments: Some features of preferred/dispreferred turn shapes. In Atkinson, J. M. & Heritage, J. (Eds.), Structures of social action: Studies in conversation analysis (pp. 79112). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Porat, R., Halperin, E., Mannheim, I., & Tamir, M. (2015). Together we cry: Social motives and preferences for group-based sadness. Cognition and Emotion, 30, 6679.Google Scholar
Porat, R., Halperin, E., & Tamir, M. (2016). What we want is what we get: Group-based emotional preferences and conflict resolution. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 110, 167–90.Google Scholar
Porter, S., & ten Brinke, L. (2008). Reading between the lies: Identifying concealed and falsified emotions in universal facial expressions. Psychological Science, 19, 508–14.Google Scholar
Porter, S., ten Brinke, L., & Wallace, B. (2012). Secrets and lies: Involuntary leakage in deceptive facial expressions as a function of emotional intensity. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 36, 2337.Google Scholar
Posner, M. I., & Keele, S. W. (1968). On the genesis of abstract ideas. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 77, 353–63.Google Scholar
Potter, J., & Wetherell, M. (1987). Discourse and social psychology: Beyond attitudes and behaviour. London: Sage.Google Scholar
Provine, R. R. (1989). Faces as releasers of contagious yawning: An approach to face detection using normal human subjects. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 27, 211–14.Google Scholar
Pugh, S. D. (2001). Service with a smile: Emotional contagion in the service encounter. Academy of Management Journal, 44, 1018–27.Google Scholar
Qu, F., Yan, W-J, Chen, Y-H, Li, K., Zhang, H., & Fu, X. (2017). ‘You should have seen the look on your face…’: Self-awareness of facial expressions. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, article 832.Google Scholar
Rafaeli, A., & Sutton, R. I. (1987). Expression of emotion as part of the work role. Academy of Management Review, 12, 2337.Google Scholar
Rao, R. P. N., & Ballard, D. H. (1999). Predictive coding in the visual cortex: A functional interpretation of some extra-classical receptive-field effects. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 7987.Google Scholar
Ray, E., & Heyes, C. (2011). Imitation in infancy: The wealth of the stimulus. Developmental Science, 14, 92105.Google Scholar
Reddish, P., Fischer, R., & Bulbulia, J. (2013). Let’s dance together: Synchrony, shared intentionality and cooperation. PLoS ONE, 8, 113.Google Scholar
Reddy, V. (2000). Coyness in early infancy. Developmental Science, 3, 186–92.Google Scholar
Reddy, V. (2008). How infants know minds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Reicher, S. D. (1987). Crowd behaviour as social action. In Turner, J. C., Hogg, M. A., Oakes, P. J., Reicher, S. D. & Wetherell, M. S. (Eds.), Rediscovering the social group: A self-categorization theory (pp. 171205). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Reisenzein, R. (1983). The Schachter theory of emotion: Two decades later. Psychological Bulletin, 94, 239–64.Google Scholar
Reisenzein, R., Studtmann, M., & Horstmann, G. (2013). Coherence between emotion and facial expression: Evidence from laboratory experiments. Emotion Review, 5, 1623.Google Scholar
Riess, H., & Kraft-Todd, G. (2014). E.M.P.A.T.H.Y.: A tool to enhance nonverbal communication between clinicians and their patients. Academic Medicine, 89, 1108–12.Google Scholar
Rimé, B. (2009). Emotion elicits the social sharing of emotion: Theory and empirical review. Emotion Review, 1, 6085.Google Scholar
Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–92.Google Scholar
Robbins, J., & Rumsey, A. (Eds.). (2008). Introduction: Cultural and linguistic anthropology and the opacity of other minds. Anthropological Quarterly, 81, 407–20.Google Scholar
Roberson, D., Damjanovic, L., & Pilling, M. (2007). Categorical perception of facial expressions: Evidence for a ‘Category Adjustment’ model. Memory & Cognition, 35, 1814–29.Google Scholar
Roberson, D., & Davidoff, J. (2000). The categorical perception of colors and facial expressions: The effect of verbal interference. Memory & Cognition, 28, 977–86.Google Scholar
Rodriguez Mosquera, P., Kahn, T., & Selya, A. (2017). American Muslims’ anger and sadness about in-group social image. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, article 2042.Google Scholar
Rolls, E. T. (1999). The brain and emotion. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rosch, E. (1973). Natural categories. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 328350.Google Scholar
Rose, A. J. (2002). Co-rumination in the friendships of girls and boys. Child Development, 73, 1830–43.Google Scholar
Rose, A. J., Carlson, W., & Waller, E. M. (2007). Prospective associations of co-rumination with friendship and emotional adjustment: Considering the socioemotional trade-offs of co-rumination. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1019–31.Google Scholar
Roseman, I. J., (1979). Cognitive aspects of emotion and emotional behavior. Paper presented at the 87th annual convention of the American Psychological Association, New York City.Google Scholar
Roseman, I. J., (1984). Cognitive determinants of emotions: A structural theory. In Shaver, P. (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology (Vol. 5, pp. 1136). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Roseman, I. J., & Evdokas, A. (2004). Appraisals cause experienced emotions: Experimental evidence. Cognition and Emotion, 18, 128.Google Scholar
Roseman, I. J., & Smith, C. A. (2001). Appraisal theory: Overview, assumptions, varieties, controversies. In Scherer, K. R., Schorr, A. & Johnstone, T. (Eds.), Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, research, application (pp. 319). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ross, E. D., & Pulusu, V. K. (2013). Posed versus spontaneous facial expressions are modulated by opposite cerebral hemispheres. Cortex, 49, 1280–91.Google Scholar
Rozin, P., & Fallon, A. E. (1987). A perspective on disgust. Psychological Review, 94, 2341.Google Scholar
Rozin, P., Lowery, L., Imada, S., & Haidt, J. (1999). The CAD triad hypothesis: A mapping between three moral emotions (contempt, anger, disgust) and three moral codes (community, autonomy, divinity). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 574–86.Google Scholar
Ruch, W. (1997). State and trait cheerfulness and the induction of exhilaration: A FACS study. European Psychologist, 2, 328–41.Google Scholar
Ruiz-Belda, M. A., Fernández-Dols, J. M., Carrera, P., & Barchard, K. (2003). Spontaneous facial expressions of happy bowlers and soccer fans. Cognition and Emotion, 17, 315–26.Google Scholar
Russell, C. L., Bard, K. A., & Adamson, L. B. (1997). Social referencing by young chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 111, 185–93.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A. (1978). Evidence of convergent validity on the dimensions of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 1152–68.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 1161–78.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A. (1983). Pancultural aspects of the human conceptual organization of emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 1281–8.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A. (1991). In defense of a prototype approach to emotion concepts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 3747.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A. (1994). Is there universal recognition of emotion from facial expression? A review of the cross-cultural studies. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 102–41.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A. (1995). Facial expressions of emotion: What lies beyond minimal universality? Psychological Bulletin, 118, 379–91.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A. (1997). Reading emotions from and into faces: Resurrecting a dimensional contextual perspective. In Russell, J. A. & Fernandez Dols, J. M. (Eds.), The psychology of facial expression (pp. 295320). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A. (2003). Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychological Review, 110, 145–72.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A. (2012). Introduction to special section: On defining emotion. Emotion Review, 4, 337.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A., & Fehr, B. (1994). Fuzzy concepts in a fuzzy hierarchy: Varieties of anger. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 186205.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A., & Fernández-Dols, J.-M. (1997). What does a facial expression mean? In Russell, J. A. & Fernández-Dols, J.-M. (Eds.), The psychology of facial expression (pp. 330). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A., Lewicka, M., & Niit, T. (1989). A cross-cultural study of a circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 848–56.Google Scholar
Rychlowska, M., Cañadas, E., Wood, A., Krumhuber, E. G., Fischer, A., & Niedenthal, P. M. (2014). Blocking mimicry makes true and false smiles look the same. PLoS ONE, 9, article e90876.Google Scholar
Sabini, J., Siepmann, M., Stein, J., & Meyerowitz, M. (2000). Who is embarrassed by what? Cognition and Emotion, 14, 213–40.Google Scholar
Sacks, H. (1992). Lectures on conversation, Vols. 1 and 2. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Sander, D., Grandjean, D., Kaiser, S., Wehrle, T., & Scherer, K. R. (2007). Interaction effects of perceived gaze direction and dynamic facial expression: Evidence for appraisal theories of emotion. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 19, 470–80.Google Scholar
Sanders, G. S., & Baron, R. S. (1977). Is social comparison irrelevant for producing choice shifts? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 13, 303–14.Google Scholar
Sauter, D. A., LeGuen, O., & Haun, D. B. M. (2011). Categorical perception of emotional facial expressions does not require lexical categories. Emotion, 11, 1479–83.Google Scholar
Scarantino, A. (2010). Insights and blindspots of the cognitivist theory of emotions. British Journal for the Philosophy of Science, 61, 729–68.Google Scholar
Scarantino, A. (2017). How to do things with emotional expressions: The theory of affective pragmatics. Psychological Inquiry, 28, 165–85.Google Scholar
Schachter, S. (1959). The psychology of affiliation. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Schachter, S. (1964). The interaction of cognitive and physiological determinants of emotional state. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 1, 4980.Google Scholar
Schachter, S., & Singer, J. E. (1962). Cognitive, social, and physiological determinants of emotional state. Psychological Review, 69, 379–99.Google Scholar
Schaubroeck, J. M., & Shao, P. (2012). The role of attribution in how followers respond to the emotional expression of male and female leaders. Leadership Quarterly, 23, 2742.Google Scholar
Schaufeli, W. B., Leiter, M. P., & Maslach, C. (2009). Burnout: 35 years of research and practice. The Career Development International, 14, 204–20.Google Scholar
Scherer, K. R. (1984). Emotion as a multicomponent process: A model and some cross-cultural data. In Shaver, P. (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology (Vol. 5, pp. 3763). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Scherer, K. R. (1992). What does facial expression express? In Strongman, K. T. (Ed.), International review of studies in emotion (Vol. 2, pp. 139–65). Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
Scherer, K. R. (1994). Toward a concept of ‘modal emotions’. In Ekman, P. & Davidson, R. J. (Eds.), The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions (pp. 2531). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Scherer, K. R. (2001). Appraisal considered as a process of multi-level sequential checking. In Scherer, K. R., Schorr, A. & Johnstone, T. (Eds.), Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, Methods, Research (pp. 92120). New York and Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Scherer, K. R., & Grandjean, D. (2008). Facial expressions allow inference of both emotions and their components. Cognition and Emotion, 22, 789801.Google Scholar
Scherer, K. R., Mortillaro, M., & Mehu, M. (2017). Facial expression is driven by appraisal and generates appraisal inference.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
Scherer, K. R., Mortillaro, M., Rotondi, I., Sergi, I., & Trznadel, S. (2018). Appraisal-driven facial actions as building blocks for emotion inference. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 114, 358–79.Google Scholar
Schmidt, K.L., Bhattacharya, S., & Denlinger, R. (2009). Comparison of deliberate and spontaneous facial movement in smiles and eyebrow raises. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 33, 3545.Google Scholar
Schouten, B. C., & Meeuwesen, L. (2006). Cultural differences in medical communication: A review of the literature. Patient Education and Counseling, 64, 2134.Google Scholar
Schlosberg, H. (1952). The description of facial expression in terms of two dimensions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 44, 229–37.Google Scholar
Schofield, P. E., Butow, P. N., Thompson, J. F., Tattersall, M. H., Beeney, L. J., & Dunn, S. M. (2003). Psychological responses of patients receiving a diagnosis of cancer. Annals of Oncology, 14, 4856.Google Scholar
Schutte, N., Toppinen, S., Kalimo, R., & Schaufeli, W. (2000). The factorial validity of the Maslach Burnout Inventory: General Survey (MBI-GS) across occupational groups. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 73, 5366.Google Scholar
Schwartz, B. (1974). Biological boundaries of learning. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 21, 183–98.Google Scholar
Schwarz, N., & Bless, H. (1991). Happy and mindless, but sad and smart? The impact of affective states on analytic reasoning. In Forgas, J. P. (Ed.), Emotion and social judgments (pp. 5571). Oxford: Pergamon.Google Scholar
Sedikides, C., & Strube, M. J. (1997). Self-evaluation: To thine own self be good, to thine own self be sure, to thine own self be true, and to thine own self be better. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 29, 209–69.Google Scholar
Sell, A., Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2009). Formidability and the logic of human anger. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106, 15073–8.Google Scholar
Semin, G. R., & Manstead, A. S. R. (1982). The social implications of embarrassment displays and restitution behaviour. European Journal of Social Psychology, 12, 367–77.Google Scholar
Sergi, I., Fiorentini, C., Trznadel, S., & Scherer, K. R. (2016). Appraisal inference from synthetic facial expressions. International Journal of Synthetic Emotions, 7, 4563.Google Scholar
Shackelford, T. K., Buss, D. M., & Bennett, K. (2002). Forgiveness or breakup: Sex differences in responses to a partner’s infidelity. Cognition and Emotion, 16, 299307.Google Scholar
Shanafelt, T. D., Gradishar, W. J., Kosty, M., et al. (2014). Burnout and career satisfaction among US oncologists. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 51, 678–86.Google Scholar
Shaver, P. R., Murdaya, U., & Fraley, R. C. (2001). Structure of the Indonesian emotion lexicon. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 4, 201–24.Google Scholar
Shaver, P., Schwartz, J., Kirson, D., & O’Connor, C. (1987). Emotion knowledge: Further exploration of a prototype approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 1061–86.Google Scholar
Shaw, J., Brown, R., & Dunn, S. (2015). The impact of delivery style on doctors’ experience of stress during simulated bad news consultations. Patient Education and Counseling, 98, 1255–9.Google Scholar
Sheppes, G., Catran, E., & Meiran, N. (2009). Reappraisal (but not distraction) is going to make you sweat: Physiological evidence for self-control effort. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 71, 91–6.Google Scholar
Sheppes, G., Scheibe, S., Suri, G., & Gross, J. J. (2011). Emotion regulation choice. Psychological Science, 22, 1391–6.Google Scholar
Shields, S. A., & MacDowell, K. A. (1987). ‘Appropriate’ emotion in politics: Judgments of a televised debate. Journal of Communication, 37, 7889.Google Scholar
Shirom, A., & Melamed, S. (2005). Does burnout affect physical health? A review of the evidence. In Antoniou, A. S. G. & Cooper, C. L. (Eds.), Research companion to organizational health psychology (pp. 599622), Edward Elgar: Cheltenham.Google Scholar
Shnabel, N., Nadler, A., Ullrich, J., Dovidio, J. F., & Carmi, D. (2009). Promoting reconciliation through the satisfaction of the emotional needs of victimized and perpetrating group members: The needs-based model of reconciliation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35, 1021–30.Google Scholar
Shore, D. M., & Heerey, E. A. (2011). The value of genuine and polite smiles, Emotion, 11, 169–74.Google Scholar
Shore, D. M., Katic, L., & Parkinson, B. (under review). Making amends and mending things: Relationship repair and practical repair independently alleviate guilt. University of Oxford, UK.Google Scholar
Shore, D. M., Rychlowska, M., van der Schalk, J., Parkinson, B., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2019). Intergroup emotional exchange: Ingroup guilt and outgroup anger increase resource allocation in trust games. Emotion, 19, 605616.Google Scholar
Shuman, V., Clark-Polner, E., Meuleman, B., Sander, D., & Scherer, K. R. (2017). Emotion perception from a componential perspective. Cognition and Emotion, 31, 4756.Google Scholar
Siegel, E. H., Sands, M. K., van den Noortgate, W., Condon, P., Chang, Y., Dy, J., Quigley, K. S., & Barrett, L. F. (2018). Emotion fingerprints or emotion populations? A meta-analytic investigation of autonomic features of emotion categories. Psychological Bulletin, 144, 343–93.Google Scholar
Simner, M. L. (1971). Newborn’s response to the cry of another infant. Developmental Psychology, 5, 136–50.Google Scholar
Sinaceur, M., Kopelman, S., Vasiljevic, D., & Haag, C. (2015). Weep and get more: When and why sadness expression is effective in negotiations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 100, 1847–71.Google Scholar
Sinaceur, M., & Tiedens, L. Z. (2006). Get mad and get more than even: When and why anger expression is effective in negotiations. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 314–22.Google Scholar
Slater, M. J., Haslam, S. A., & Steffens, N. K. (2018). Singing it for ‘us’: Team passion displayed during national anthems is associated with subsequent success. European Journal of Sport Science, 18, 541–9.Google Scholar
Sliter, M., Jex, S., Wolford, K., & McInnerney, J. (2010). How rude! Emotional labor as a mediator between customer incivility and employee outcomes. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 15, 468–81.Google Scholar
Smith, C. A. (1989). Dimensions of appraisal and physiological response in emotionJournal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 339–53.Google Scholar
Smith, C. A., & Ellsworth, P.C. (1985). Patterns of cognitive appraisal in emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 813–38.Google Scholar
Smith, C. A., & Ellsworth, P. C. (1987). Patterns of appraisal and emotion related to taking an exam. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 475–88.Google Scholar
Smith, C. A., & Lazarus, R. S. (1993). Appraisal components, core relational themes, and the emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 7, 233–69.Google Scholar
Smith, H., & Kessler, T. (2004). Group-based emotions and intergroup behavior. In Tiedens, L. Z. & Leach, C. W. (Eds.), The social life of emotions (pp. 292313). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Smith, E. R. (1993). Social identity and social emotions: Toward new conceptualizations of prejudice. In Mackie, D. M., Hamilton, D. & Lewis, D. (Eds.), Affect, cognition, and stereotyping: Interactive processes in group perception (pp. 297315). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Smith, E. R., Seger, C. R., & Mackie, D. M. (2007). Can emotions be truly group level? Evidence regarding four conceptual criteria. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 431–46.Google Scholar
Smith, K. E., Norman, G. J., & Decety, J. (2017). The complexity of empathy during medical school training: evidence for positive changes. Medical Education, 51, 1146–59.Google Scholar
Smith, R. H. (2013). Schadenfreude and the dark side of human nature. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Snyder, M., & Swann, W. B. Jr. (1978). Behavioral confirmation in social interaction: From social perception to social reality. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 14, 148–62.Google Scholar
Sorce, J. F., Emde, R. N., Campos, J., & Klinnert, M. D. (1985). Maternal emotional signaling: Its effect on the visual cliff behavior of 1 year olds. Developmental Psychology, 21, 195200.Google Scholar
Soussignan, R. (2002). Duchenne smile, emotional experience, and autonomic reactivity: A test of the facial feedback hypothesis. Emotion, 2, 5274.Google Scholar
Speisman, J. C., Lazarus, R. S., Mordkoff, A., & Davison, L. (1964). Experimental reduction of stress based on ego-defense theory. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 68, 367–80.Google Scholar
Spencer, S., & Rupp, D. E. (2009). Angry, guilty, and conflicted: Injustice toward coworkers heightens emotional labor through cognitive and emotional mechanisms. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94, 429–44.Google Scholar
Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. (1986). Relevance: Communication and cognition. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Srinivasan, M. V., Laughlin, S. B., & Dubs, A. (1982). Predictive coding: A fresh view of inhibition in the retina. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Containing Papers of a Biological Character, 216, 427–59.Google Scholar
Stamkou, E., van Kleef, G. A., Fischer, A. H., & Kret, M. E. (2016). Are the powerful really blind to the feelings of others? How hierarchical concerns shape attention to emotions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 42, 755–68.Google Scholar
Stamm, B. H. (2008). The ProQOL Test Manual (2nd edition). Baltimore: Sidran Press.Google Scholar
Steinel, W., van Kleef, G. A., & Harinck, F. (2008). Are you talking to me?! Separating the people from the problem when expressing emotions in negotiation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 362–69.Google Scholar
Stemmler, G. (1989). The autonomic differentiation of emotions revisited: Convergent and discriminant validation. Psychophysiology, 26, 617–32.Google Scholar
Stemmler, G., Heldmann, M., Pauls, C. A., & Scherer, T. (2001). Constraints for emotion specificity in fear and anger: The context counts. Psychophysiology 38, 275–91.Google Scholar
Stern, D. N. (1999). Vitality contours: The temporal contour of feelings as a basic unit for constructing the infant’s social experience. In Rochat, P. (Ed.), Early social cognition: Understanding others in the first months of life (pp. 6780). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Stern, D. N., Hofer, L., Haft, W., & Dore, J. (1985). Affect attunement: The sharing of feeling states between mother and infant by means of intermodal fluency. In Field, T. N. & Fox, N. (Eds.), Social perception in infants (pp. 249–68). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Stoner, J. A. F. (1961). A comparison of individual and group decisions involving risk. Unpublished master’s thesis. Massachusetts Institute of Technology, School of Industrial Management.Google Scholar
Stott, C., Hutchison, P., & Drury, J. (2001). ‘Hooligans’ abroad? Inter-group dynamics, social identity and participation in collective ‘disorder’ at the 1998 World Cup Finals. British Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 359–84.Google Scholar
Stott, C., Adang, O., Livingstone, A., & Schreiber, M. (2007). Variability in the collective behaviour of England fans at Euro2004: ‘Hooliganism’, public order policing and social change European Journal of Social Psychology, 37, 75100.Google Scholar
Strack, F., Martin, L. L., & Stepper, S. (1988). Inhibiting and facilitating conditions of the human smile: A non-obtrusive test of the facial feedback hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 768–77.Google Scholar
Studer, R. K., Danuser, B., & Gomez, P. (2017). Physicians’ psychophysiological stress reaction in medical communication of bad news: A critical literature review. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 120, 1422.Google Scholar
Su, L., & Levine, M. (2016). Does ‘Lie to Me’ lie to you? An evaluation of facial clues to high-stakes deception. Computer Vision and Image Understanding, 147, 5268.Google Scholar
Suchman, A. L., Markakis, K., Beckman, H. B., & Frankel, R. (1997). A model of empathic communication in the medical interview. Journal of the American Medical Association. 277, 678–82.Google Scholar
Suri, G., Sheppes, G., Young, G., Abraham, D., McRae, K., & Gross, J. J. (2018). Emotion regulation choice: The role of environmental affordances. Cognition and Emotion, 32, 963–71.Google Scholar
Sutton, R. I. (1991). Maintaining norms about expressed emotions: The case of bill collectors. Administrative Science Quarterly, 36, 245–68.Google Scholar
Sutton, R. I., & Rafaeli, A. (1988). Untangling the relationship between displayed emotions and organizational sales: The case of convenience stores. Academy of Management Journal, 31, 461–87.Google Scholar
Swann, W. B. Jr., Gómez, A., Seyle, C. D., Morales, J. F., & Huici, C. (2009). Identity fusion: The interplay of personal and social identities in extreme group behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 9951011.Google Scholar
Swann, W. B. Jr., Jetten, J., Gómez, A., Whitehouse, H., & Bastian, B. (2012). When group membership gets personal: A theory of identity fusion. Psychological Review, 119, 441–56.Google Scholar
Sy, T., & Choi, J. N. (2013). Contagious leaders and followers: Exploring multi-stage mood contagion in a leader activation and member propagation (LAMP) model. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 122, 127–40.Google Scholar
Sy, T., Côté, S., & Saavedra, R. (2005). The contagious leader: Impact of the leader’s mood on the mood of group members, group affective tone, and group processes. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 295305.Google Scholar
Sy, T., Horton, C., & Riggio, R. (2018). Charismatic leadership: Eliciting and channeling follower emotions. Leadership Quarterly, 29, 5869.Google Scholar
Tabri, N., Wohl, M. J. A., & Caouette, J. (2018). Will we be harmed, will it be severe, can we protect ourselves? Threat appraisals predict collective angst (and its consequences). European Journal of Social Psychology, 48, 7285.Google Scholar
Tajfel, H., Flament, C., Billig, M. G., & Bundy, R. P. (1971). Social categorization and inter-group behavior. European Journal of Social Psychology, 1, 149–77.Google Scholar
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In Worchel, S. & Austin, W. G. (Eds.), The psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 3347). Monterey, CA: Brooks-Cole.Google Scholar
Tajfel, H., & Wilkes, A. L. (1963). Classification and quantitative judgement. British Journal of Psychology, 54, 101–14.Google Scholar
Tamietto, M., Castelli, L., Vighetti, S., Perozzo, P., Geminiani, G., Weiskrantz, L., & de Gelder, B. (2009). Unseen facial and bodily expressions trigger fast emotional reactions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106, 17661–6.Google Scholar
Tamir, M. (2009). What do people want to feel and why? Pleasure and utility in emotion regulation. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 101–5.Google Scholar
Tamir, M. (2016). Why do people regulate their emotions? A taxonomy of motives in emotion regulation. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 20, 199222.Google Scholar
Tamir, M., Ford, B. Q., & Gilliam, M. (2013). Evidence for utilitarian motives in emotion regulation. Cognition and Emotion, 27, 483–91.Google Scholar
Tamir, M., Mitchell, C., & Gross, J. J. (2008). Hedonic and instrumental motives in anger regulation. Psychological Science, 19, 324–8.Google Scholar
Tarr, B., Launay, J., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2016). Silent disco: Dancing in synchrony leads to elevated pain thresholds and social closeness. Evolution and Human Behavior, 37, 343–9.Google Scholar
Taylor, S. E., Fiske, S. T., Etcoff, N. L., & Ruderman, A. J. (1978). Categorical and contextual bases of person memory and stereotyping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 778–93.Google Scholar
Tetlock, P. E. (1986). Is self-categorization theory the solution to the level-of-analysis problem? British Journal of Social Psychology, 25, 255–6.Google Scholar
Thayer, R. E., Newman, J. R., & McClain, T. M. (1994). Self-regulation of mood: Strategies for changing a bad mood, raising energy, and reducing tension. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 910–25.Google Scholar
Thirioux, B., Birault, F., & Jaafari, N. (2016). Empathy is a protective factor of burnout in physicians: New neuro-phenomenological hypotheses regarding empathy and sympathy in care relationship. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, article 763.Google Scholar
Thompson, R. A. (1987). Empathy and emotional understanding: The early development of empathy. In Eisenberg, N. & Strayer, J. (Eds.), Empathy and its development (pp. 119–43). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Thorstenson, C. A., Elliot, A. J., Pazda, A. D., Perrett, D. I., & Xiao, D. (2018). Emotion-color associations in the context of the face. Emotion, 18, 1032–42.Google Scholar
Tidd, K. L., & Lockard, J. S. (1978). Monetary significance of the affiliative smile: A case for reciprocal altruism. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 11, 344–6.Google Scholar
Tiedens, L. Z. (2001). Anger and advancement versus sadness and subjugation: The effect of negative emotion expressions on social status conferral. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 8694.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Call, J. (1997). Primate cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Carpenter, M. (2007). Shared intentionality. Developmental Science, 10, 121–5.Google Scholar
Tomkins, S. S. (1962). Affect, imagery and consciousness. Vol. 1: The positive affects. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Tomkins, S. S., & McCarter, R. (1964). What and where are the primary affects? Some evidence for a theory. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 18, 119–58.Google Scholar
Tomkins, S. S. (1981). The role of facial response in the experience of emotion: A reply to Tourangeau and Ellsworth. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 355–7.Google Scholar
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1990). The past explains the present: Emotional adaptations and the structure of ancestral environments. Ethology and Sociobiology, 11, 375424.Google Scholar
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2008). The evolutionary psychology of the emotions and their relationship to internal regulatory variables. In Lewis, M., Haviland-Jones, J. M. & Barrett, L. F. (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (3rd edition, pp. 114–37). New York: Guilford Pres.Google Scholar
Totterdell, P. (2000). Catching moods and hitting runs: Mood linkage and subjective performance in professional sport teams. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 848–59.Google Scholar
Totterdell, P., & Holman, D. (2003). Emotion regulation in customer service roles: Testing a model of emotional labor. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 8, 5573.Google Scholar
Totterdell, P., Kellett, S., Teuchmann, K., & Briner, R. B. (1998). Evidence of mood linkage in work groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1504–15.Google Scholar
Tourangeau, R., & Ellsworth, P. C. (1979). The role of facial response in the experience of emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1519–31.Google Scholar
Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2004). Show your pride: Evidence for a discrete emotion expression. Psychological Science, 15, 194–7.Google Scholar
Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2008). The nonverbal expression of pride: Evidence for cross-cultural recognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 516–30.Google Scholar
Traut-Mattausch, E., Wagner, S., Pollatos, O., & Jonas, E. (2015). Complaints as starting point for vicious cycles in customer-employee-interactions. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, article 1454.Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C. (1993). The function of emotions in early infant communication and development. In Nadel, J. & Camaioni, L. (Eds.), New perspectives in early communicative development (pp. 4881). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Tronick, E. Z. (1989). Emotions and emotional communication in infants. American Psychologist, 44, 112–19.Google Scholar
Tsai, J. L., Knutson, B. K., & Fung, H. H. (2006). Cultural variation in affect valuation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 288307.Google Scholar
Tsai, W.-C., & Huang, Y.-M. (2002). Mechanisms linking employee affective delivery and customer behavioral intentions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 1001–08.Google Scholar
Tully, E. C., Ames, A. M., Garcia, S. E., & Donohue, M. R. (2016). Quadratic associations between empathy and depression as moderated by emotion dysregulation. Journal of Psychology, 150, 1535.Google Scholar
Turner, J. C. (1991). Social influence. Milton Keynes: Open University Press.Google Scholar
Turner, J. C., Hogg, M. A., Oakes, P. J., Reicher, S., & Wetherell, M. S. (1987). Rediscovering the social group: A self-categorisation theory. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Turner, J. C., & Oakes, P. J. (1986). The significance of the social identity concept for social psychology with reference to individualism, interactionism and social influence. British Journal of Social Psychology, 25, 237–52.Google Scholar
Turner, J. C., Wetherell, M. S., & Hogg, M. A. (1989). Referent informational influence and group polarization. British Journal of Social Psychology, 28, 135–47.Google Scholar
Tybur, J. M., Lieberman, D., & Griskevicius, V. (2009). Microbes, mating, and morality: Individual differences in three functional domains of disgust. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 103–22.Google Scholar
Van der Schalk, J., Kuppens, T., Bruder, M., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2015). The social power of regret: The effect of social appraisal and anticipated emotions on fair and unfair allocations in resource dilemmas. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144, 151–7.Google Scholar
Van Dijk, W. W., Ouwerkerk, J. W., Wesseling, Y. M., & Van Koningsbruggen, G. M. (2011). Towards understanding pleasure at the misfortunes of others: The impact of self-evaluation threat on schadenfreude. Cognition and Emotion, 25, 360–8.Google Scholar
Van Doorn, E. A., van Kleef, G. A., & van der Pligt, J. (2015). Deriving meaning from others’ emotions: Attribution, appraisal, and the use of emotions as social information. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1077.Google Scholar
Van Dulmen, S., Tromp, F., Grosfeld, F., ten Cate, O., & Bensing, J. (2007). The impact of assessing simulated bad news consultations on medical students’ stress response and communication performance. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 32, 943–50.Google Scholar
Vangelisti, A. L., Daly, J. A., & Rudnick, J. R. (1991). Making people feel guilty in conversations: Techniques and correlates. Human Communication Research, 18, 339.Google Scholar
Van Kleef, G. A. (2016). The interpersonal dynamics of emotion: Toward an integrative theory of emotions as social information. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Van Kleef, G. A., & Côté, S. (2007). Expressing anger in conflict: When it helps and when it hurts. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 1557–69.Google Scholar
Van Kleef, G. A., De Dreu, C. K. W., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2004a). The interpersonal effects of anger and happiness in negotiations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 5776.Google Scholar
Van Kleef, G. A., De Dreu, C. K. W., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2004b). The interpersonal effects of emotions in negotiations: A motivated information processing approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 510–28.Google Scholar
Van Kleef, G. A., De Dreu, C. W., Pietroni, D., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2006). Power and emotion in negotiation: Power moderates the interpersonal effects of anger and happiness on concession making. European Journal of Social Psychology, 36, 557–81.Google Scholar
Van Kleef, G. A., Homan, A. C., Beersma, B., & van Knippenberg, D. (2010). On angry leaders and agreeable followers: How leaders’ emotions and followers’ personalities shape motivation and team performance. Psychological Science, 21, 1827–34.Google Scholar
Van Kleef, G. A., Oveis, C., van der Löwe, I., & Keltner, D. (2008). Power, distress, and compassion: Turning a blind eye to the suffering of others. Psychological Science, 19, 1315–22.Google Scholar
Van Vendeloo, S. N., Godderis, L., Brand, P. L. P., Verheyen, K. C. P. M., Rowell, S. A., & Hoekstra, H. (2018). Resident burnout: evaluating the role of the learning environment. BMC Medical Education, 18, article 54.Google Scholar
Veil, C. (1959). Les états d’épuisement. Le Concours Médical, 23, 2675–81.Google Scholar
Venkataramani, V., Green, S. G., & Schleicher, D. J. (2010). Well-connected leaders: The impact of leaders’ social network ties on LMX and members’ work attitudes. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95, 1071–84.Google Scholar
Vergauwe, J., Wille, B., Hofmans, J., Kaiser, R. B., & De Fruyt, F. (2018). The double-edged sword of leader charisma: Understanding the curvilinear relationship between charismatic personality and leader effectiveness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 114, 110–30.Google Scholar
Viki, G. T., & Abrams, D. (2003). Infra humanization: Ambivalent sexism and the attribution of primary and secondary emotions to women. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 492–9.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Wagenmakers, E.-J., Beek, T., Dijkhoff, L. et al. (2016). Registered replication report: Strack, Martin, & Stepper (1988). Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11, 917–28.Google Scholar
Wagner, D. T., Barnes, C. M., & Scott, B. A. (2014). Driving it home: How workplace emotional labor harms employee home life. Personnel Psychology, 67, 487516.Google Scholar
Wagner, H. L., & Smith, J. (1991). Facial expression in the presence of friends and strangers. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 15, 201–14.Google Scholar
Waldman, D. A., Ramirez, G. G., House, R. J., & Puranam, P. (2001). Does leadership matter? CEO leadership attributes and profitability under conditions of perceived environmental uncertainty. Academy of Management Journal, 44, 134–43.Google Scholar
Wallach, M. A., Kogan, N., & Bem, D. J. (1962). Group influence on individual risk taking. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 65, 7586.Google Scholar
Walle, E., Reschke, P. J., & Knothe, J. M. (2017). Social referencing: Defining and delineating a basic process of emotion. Emotion Review, 9, 245–52.Google Scholar
Wang, K. L., & Groth, M. (2014). Buffering the negative effects of employee surface acting: The moderating role of employee-customer relationship strength and personalized services. Journal of Applied Psychology, 99, 341–50.Google Scholar
Webb, T. L., Miles, E., & Sheeran, P. (2012). Dealing with feeling: A meta-analysis of the effectiveness of strategies derived from the process model of emotion regulation. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 775808.Google Scholar
Weber, M. (1924). The theory of social and economic organizations. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Weiner, B., Russell, D., & Lerman, D. (1979). The cognition-emotion process in achievement-related contexts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1211–20.Google Scholar
Weisbuch, M., & Ambady, N. (2008). Affective divergence: Automatic responses to others’ emotions depend on group membership. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 1063–79.Google Scholar
Whitehouse, H. (1996). Rites of terror: Emotion, metaphor, and memory in Melanesian initiation cults. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute, 2, 703–15.Google Scholar
Williams, K. D. (2009). Ostracism: A temporal need–threat model. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 275314.Google Scholar
Wierzbicka, A. (1991). Talking about emotions: Semantics, culture, and cognition. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 285319.Google Scholar
Wierzbicka, A. (1994). Emotion, language, and cultural scripts. In Kitayama, S. & Markus, H. R. (Eds.), Emotion and culture: Empirical studies of mutual influence (pp. 133–96). Washington, DC: APA.Google Scholar
Wierzbicka, A. (1999). Emotions across languages and cultures: Diversity and universals. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wieser, M. J., & Brosch, T. (2012). Faces in context: a review and systematization of contextual influences on affective face processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, article 471.Google Scholar
Wiltermuth, S. S., & Heath, C. (2009). Synchrony and cooperation. Psychological Science, 20, 15.Google Scholar
Wittgenstein, L. (1953). Philosophical investigations. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Wohl, M. J. A., Matheson, K., Branscombe, N. R., & Anisman, H. (2013). Victim and perpetrator groups’ responses to the Canadian government’s apology for the head tax on Chinese immigrants and the moderating influence of collective guilt. Political Psychology, 34, 713–29.Google Scholar
Wölfer, R., Faber, N. S., & Hewstone, M. (2015). Social network analysis in the science of groups. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 19, 4561.Google Scholar
Wondra, J. D., & Ellsworth, P. C. (2015). An appraisal theory of empathy and other vicarious emotional experiences. Psychological Review, 122, 411–28.Google Scholar
Wong, E., Tschan, F., & Semmer, N. K. (2017). Effort in emotion work and well-being: The role of goal attainment. Emotion, 17, 6777.Google Scholar
Wood, A., Lupyan, G., Sherrin, S., & Niedenthal, P. (2016). Altering sensorimotor feedback disrupts visual discrimination of facial expressions. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 23, 1150–6.Google Scholar
Wright, S. C., Aron, A., McLaughlin-Volpe, T., & Ropp, S. A. (1997). The extended contact effect: Knowledge of cross-group friendships and prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 7390.Google Scholar
Wróbel, M., & Imbir, K. K. (2019). Broadening the perspective on emotional contagion and emotional mimicry: The correction hypothesis. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 14, 437–51.Google Scholar
Wundt, W. (1897). Outlines of psychology. Leipzig: Wilhelm Engelmann.Google Scholar
Xygalatas, D., Konvalinka, I., Bulbulia, J., & Roepstorff, A. (2011). Quantifying collective effervescence: Heart-rate dynamics at a firewalking ritual. Communicative and Integrative Biology, 4, 735–8.Google Scholar
Yam, P. C., & Parkinson, B. (under review). Social functions of intergroup schadenfreude during and after an international soccer tournament. University of Oxford, UK.Google Scholar
Yan, W., Wu, Q., Liang, J., Chen, Y., & Fu, X. (2013). How fast are the leaked facial expressions? The duration of micro-expressions. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 37, 217–30.Google Scholar
Yik, M. S. M., & Russell, J. A. (1999). Interpretation of faces: A cross-cultural study of a prediction from Fridlund’s theory. Cognition and Emotion, 13, 93104.Google Scholar
Yzerbyt, V., Dumont, M., Wigboldus, D., & Gordijn, E. (2003). I feel for us: The impact of categorization and identification on emotions and action tendencies. British Journal of Social Psychology, 42, 533–49.Google Scholar
Yzerbyt, V., Kuppens, T., & Mathieu, B. (2016). When talking makes you feel like a group: The emergence of group-based emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 30, 3350.Google Scholar
Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35, 151–75.Google Scholar
Zaki, J., & Williams, W. C. (2013). Interpersonal emotion regulation. Emotion, 13, 803-810.Google Scholar
Zajonc, R. B. (1984). On the primacy of affect. American Psychologist, 39, 117–23.Google Scholar
Zhan, Y., Wang, M., & Shi, J. (2016). Interpersonal process of emotional labor: The role of negative and positive customer treatment. Personnel Psychology, 69, 525–57.Google Scholar
Zimbardo, P. (1969). The human choice: Individuation, reason, and order versus deindividuation, impulse, and chaos. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 17, 237307.Google Scholar
Zimbardo, P. G., Ebbeson, E. B., & Maslach, C. (1977). Influencing attitudes and changing behavior (2nd edition). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Zimmermann, B. K., Dormann, C., & Dollard, M. F. (2011). On the positive aspects of customers: Customer-initiated support and affective crossover in employee-customer dyads. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 20, 3157.Google Scholar
Zuckerman, M., Klorman, R., Larrance, D. T., & Spiegel, N. H. (1981). Facial, autonomic, and subjective components of emotion: The facial feedback hypothesis versus the externalizer-internalizer distinction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 929–44.Google Scholar
Zwingmann, J., Baile, W. F., Schmier, J. W., Jürg, B., & Keller, M. (2017). Effects of patient-centered communication on anxiety, negative affect, and trust in the physician in delivering a cancer diagnosis: A randomized, experimental study. Cancer, 123, 3167–75.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×