Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-ndw9j Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-02T22:18:17.882Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

13 - Neurocognitive Markers of Developmental Dyslexia

from Part II - Cross-Linguistic Perspectives on Developmental Dyslexia

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 September 2019

Ludo Verhoeven
Affiliation:
Radboud Universiteit Nijmegen
Charles Perfetti
Affiliation:
University of Pittsburgh
Kenneth Pugh
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
Get access

Summary

In the history of human evolution, reading and writing skills were developed about 5,400 years ago (Dehaene, 2009), which is fairly late in the context of the 350,000–150,000 years of history of human speech (Perreault & Mathew, 2012). In terms of ontogeny, the acquisition of reading also follows that of speech over the course of the child’s early years. Nonetheless, reading has become one of the most important cognitive functions for daily life and reading difficulties can severely limit an individual’s ability to thrive in the modern world (Calfee, 1982; Rawson, 1978). Across languages and cultures, approximately 5–20 percent of the population (depending on definitional criteria) is affected by developmental dyslexia (DD), a specific reading impairment presents in the absence of other cognitive impairments (S. E. Shaywitz & Shaywitz, 2003).

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ackerman, P. T., Dykman, R. A., Oglesby, D. M., & Newton, J. E. (1994). EEG power spectra of children with dyslexia, slow learners, and normally reading children with ADD during verbal processing. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 27, 619630.Google Scholar
Baillieux, H., Vandervliet, E. J., Manto, M. et al. (2009). Developmental dyslexia and widespread activation across the cerebellar hemispheres. Brain and Language, 108, 122132.Google Scholar
Banai, K., Hornickel, J., Skoe, E. et al. (2009). Reading and subcortical auditory function. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 26992707.Google Scholar
Bar-Kochva, I., & Breznitz, Z. (2014). Reading proficiency and adaptability in orthographic processing: An examination of the effect of type of orthography read on brain activity in regular and dyslexic readers. PLoS One, 9, e86016. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0086016.Google Scholar
Binder, J. (2009). fMRI of language systems. In Filippi, M. (Ed.), fMRI techniques and protocols (Vol. 41, pp. 323351). Totowa, NJ: Humana Press.Google Scholar
Binder, J., & Price, C. J. (2001). Functional neuroimaging of language. In Cabeza, R. & Kingstone, A. (Eds.), Handbook of functional neuroimaging of cognition (pp. 187251). Cambridge, MA: MIT press.Google Scholar
Blackmon, K., Barr, W. B., Kuzniecky, R. et al. (2010). Phonetically irregular word pronunciation and cortical thickness in the adult brain. Neuroimage, 51, 14531458.Google Scholar
Boets, B., Op de Beeck, H. P., Vandermosten, M. et al. (2013). Intact but less accessible phonetic representations in adults with dyslexia. Science, 342, 12511254.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Booth, J. R., Burman, D. D., Van Santen, F. W. et al. (2001). The development of specialized brain systems in reading and oral-language. Child Neuropsychology, 7, 119141.Google Scholar
Booth, J. R., Lu, D., Burman, D. D. et al. (2006). Specialization of phonological and semantic processing in Chinese word reading. Brain Research, 1071, 197207.Google Scholar
Bosse, M.-L., Tainturier, M. J., & Valdois, S. (2007). Developmental dyslexia: The visual attention span deficit hypothesis. Cognition, 104, 198230.Google Scholar
Brady, S. A., & Shankweiler, D. P. (1991). Phonological processes in literacy: A tribute to Isabelle Y. Liberman. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Braze, D., Mencl, W. E., & Tabor, W. et al. (2011). Unification of sentence processing via ear and eye: An fMRI study. Cortex, 47, 416431.Google Scholar
Bruck, M. (1992). Persistence of dyslexics’ phonological awareness deficits. Developmental Psychology, 28, 874886.Google Scholar
Brunswick, N., McCrory, E., Price, C., Frith, C., & Frith, U. (1999). Explicit and implicit processing of words and pseudowords by adult developmental dyslexics. Brain, 122, 19011917.Google Scholar
Calfee, R. (1982). Literacy and illiteracy: Teaching the nonreader to survive in the modern world. Annals of Dyslexia, 32, 7191. doi: http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1007/BF02647954.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cao, F., Bitan, T., Chou, T. L., Burman, D. D., & Booth, J. R. (2006). Deficient orthographic and phonological representations in children with dyslexia revealed by brain activation patterns. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 10411050.Google Scholar
Cao, F., Lee, R., Shu, H. et al. (2010). Cultural constraints on brain development: Evidence from a developmental study of visual word processing in Mandarin Chinese. Cerebral Cortex, 20, 12231233.Google Scholar
Caravolas, M. (2005). The nature and causes of dyslexia in different languages. In Snowling, M. & Hulme, C. (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 336355). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Caravolas, M., Lervåg, A., Defior, S., Málková, G. S., & Hulme, C. (2013). Different patterns, but equivalent predictors, of growth in reading in consistent and inconsistent orthographies. Psychological Science, 24, 13981407.Google Scholar
Caravolas, M., Lervåg, A., Mousikou, P. et al. (2012). Common patterns of prediction of literacy development in different alphabetic orthographies. Psychological Science, 23, 678686.Google Scholar
Caravolas, M., Volín, J., & Hulme, C. (2005). Phoneme awareness is a key component of alphabetic literacy skills in consistent and inconsistent orthographies: Evidence from Czech and English children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92, 107139.Google Scholar
Castro‐Caldas, A., Miranda, P. C., Carmo, I. et al. (1999). Influence of learning to read and write on the morphology of the corpus callosum. European Journal of Neurology, 6, 2328.Google Scholar
Church, J. A., Balota, D. A., Petersen, S. E., & Schlaggar, B. L. (2011). Manipulation of length and lexicality localizes the functional neuroanatomy of phonological processing in adult readers. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 14751493.Google Scholar
Church, J. A., Coalson, R. S., Lugar, H. M., Petersen, S. E., & Schlaggar, B. L. (2008). A developmental fMRI study of reading and repetition reveals changes in phonological and visual mechanisms over age. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 20542065.Google Scholar
Cohen, L., Lehéricy, S., Chochon, F. et al. (2002). Language‐specific tuning of visual cortex? Functional properties of the Visual Word Form Area. Brain, 125, 10541069.Google Scholar
Dehaene, S. (2009). Reading in the brain: The new science of how we read. London: Penguin Publishing Group.Google Scholar
Dehaene, S., & Cohen, L. (2011). The unique role of the visual word form area in reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15, 254262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dehaene, S., Cohen, L., Morais, J., & Kolinsky, R. (2015). Illiterate to literate: Behavioural and cerebral changes induced by reading acquisition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16, 234244.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Démonet, J.-F., Taylor, M. J., & Chaix, Y. (2004). Developmental dyslexia. The Lancet, 363, 14511460.Google Scholar
Démonet, J.-F., Thierry, G., & Cardebat, D. (2005). Renewal of the neurophysiology of language: Functional neuroimaging. Physiological Reviews, 85, 4995.Google Scholar
DeWitt, I., & Rauschecker, J. P. (2013). Wernicke’s area revisited: Parallel streams and word processing. Brain and Language, 127, 181191.Google Scholar
Dhar, M., Been, P. H., Minderaa, R. B., & Althaus, M. (2010). Reduced interhemispheric coherence in dyslexic adults. Cortex, 46, 794798.Google Scholar
Díaz, B., Hintz, F., Kiebel, S. J., & von Kriegstein, K. (2012). Dysfunction of the auditory thalamus in developmental dyslexia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109, 1384113846.Google Scholar
Diehl, J. J., Frost, S. J., & Sherman, G. (2014). Neural correlates of language and non-language visuospatial processing in adolescents with reading disability. Neuroimage, 101, 653666.Google Scholar
Dong, Y., Nakamura, K., & Okada, T. (2005). Neural mechanisms underlying the processing of Chinese words: An fMRI study. Neuroscience Research, 52, 139145.Google Scholar
Dujardin, T., Etienne, Y., Contentin, C. et al. (2011). Behavioral performances in participants with phonological dyslexia and different patterns on the N170 component. Brain and Cognition, 75, 91100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
El-Ansary, A., & Al-Ayadhi, L. (2014). GABAergic/glutamatergic imbalance relative to excessive neuroinflammation in autism spectrum disorders. Journal Neuroinflammation, 11, 189. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s12974-014-0189-0.Google Scholar
Finn, E. S., Shen, X., & Holahan, J. M. (2014). Disruption of functional networks in dyslexia: A whole-brain, data-driven analysis of connectivity. Biological Psychiatry, 76, 397404.Google Scholar
Fletcher, J. M., Shaywitz, S. E., Shankweiler, D. P. et al. (1994). Cognitive profiles of reading disability: Comparisons of discrepancy and low achievement definitions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 623.Google Scholar
Frost, R. (2012). Towards a universal model of reading. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35, 263279.Google Scholar
Frost, R., Katz, L., & Bentin, S. (1987). Strategies for visual word recognition and orthographical depth: A multilingual comparison. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 104115. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.13.1.104.Google ScholarPubMed
Frost, S. J., Landi, N., & Mencl, W. E. et al. (2009). Phonological awareness predicts activation patterns for print and speech. Annals of Dyslexia, 59, 7897.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gabrieli, J. D. (2009). Dyslexia: A new synergy between education and cognitive neuroscience. Science, 325, 280283.Google Scholar
Giraud, A.-L., & Ramus, F. (2013). Neurogenetics and auditory processing in developmental dyslexia. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23, 3742.Google Scholar
Goswami, U., Thomson, J., Richardson, U. et al. (2002). Amplitude envelope onsets and developmental dyslexia: A new hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 99, 1091110916.Google Scholar
Goswami, U., Wang, H.-L. S., Cruz, A. et al. (2011). Language-universal sensory deficits in developmental dyslexia: English, Spanish, and Chinese. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 325337.Google Scholar
Goswami, U., Ziegler, J. C., & Richardson, U. (2005). The effects of spelling consistency on phonological awareness: A comparison of English and German. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92, 345365.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graves, W. W., Binder, J. R., Desai, R. H. et al. (2014). Anatomy is strategy: Skilled reading differences associated with structural connectivity differences in the reading network. Brain and Language, 133, 113.Google Scholar
Griffith, P. L., & Olson, M. W. (1992). Phonemic awareness helps beginning readers break the code. The Reading Teacher, 45, 516523.Google Scholar
Hampson, M., Tokoglu, F., Sun, Z. et al. (2006). Connectivity–behavior analysis reveals that functional connectivity between left BA39 and Broca’s area varies with reading ability. Neuroimage, 31, 513519.Google Scholar
Helenius, P., Uutela, K., & Hari, R. (1999). Auditory stream segregation in dyslexic adults. Brain, 122, 907913.Google Scholar
Ho, C. S.-H., Chan, D. W., Chung, K. K., Lee, S.-H., & Tsang, S.-M. (2007). In search of subtypes of Chinese developmental dyslexia. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 97, 6183.Google Scholar
Hoeft, F., Hernandez, A., McMillon, G. et al. (2006). Neural basis of dyslexia: A comparison between dyslexic and nondyslexic children equated for reading ability. The Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 1070010708.Google Scholar
Hoeft, F., Meyler, A., Hernandez, A. et al. (2007). Functional and morphometric brain dissociation between dyslexia and reading ability. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104, 42344239.Google Scholar
Hornickel, J., Anderson, S., Skoe, E., Yi, H.-G., & Kraus, N. (2012). Subcortical representation of speech fine structure relates to reading ability. Neuroreport, 23, 69.Google Scholar
Hornickel, J., Skoe, E., Nicol, T., Zecker, S., & Kraus, N. (2009). Subcortical differentiation of stop consonants relates to reading and speech-in-noise perception. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106, 1302213027.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horwitz, B., Rumsey, J. M., & Donohue, B. C. (1998). Functional connectivity of the angular gyrus in normal reading and dyslexia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 95, 89398944.Google Scholar
Hosseini, S. H., Black, J. M., Soriano, T. et al. (2013). Topological properties of large-scale structural brain networks in children with familial risk for reading difficulties. Neuroimage, 71, 260274.Google Scholar
Hu, W., Lee, H. L., & Zhang, Q. (2010). Developmental dyslexia in Chinese and English populations: Dissociating the effect of dyslexia from language differences. Brain, 133, 16941706.Google Scholar
Jeon, H.-A. (2012). Effect of lexical proficiency on reading strategies used for shallow and deep orthographies. Neuroreport, 23, 979983.Google Scholar
Kamii, C., & Manning, M. (2002). Phonemic awareness and beginning reading and writing. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 17, 3846.Google Scholar
Kast, M., Elmer, S., Jancke, L., & Meyer, M. (2010). ERP differences of pre-lexical processing between dyslexic and non-dyslexic children. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 77, 5969.Google Scholar
Katz, L., & Frost, R. (1992). The reading process is different for different orthographies: The orthographic depth hypothesis. Advances in Psychology, 94, 6784.Google Scholar
Kita, Y., Yamamoto, H., Oba, K. et al. (2013). Altered brain activity for phonological manipulation in dyslexic Japanese children. Brain, 136, 36963708.Google Scholar
Klimesch, W., Doppelmayr, M., Wimmer, H., Gruber, W. et al. (2001). Alpha and beta band power changes in normal and dyslexic children. Clinical Neurophysiology, 112, 11861195.Google Scholar
Klimesch, W., Doppelmayr, M., Wimmer, H. E. A., Schwaiger, J. et al. (2001). Theta band power changes in normal and dyslexic children. Clinical Neurophysiology, 112, 11741185.Google Scholar
Klingberg, T., Hedehus, M., & Temple, E. (2000). Microstructure of temporo-parietal white matter as a basis for reading ability: evidence from diffusion tensor magnetic resonance imaging. Neuron, 25, 493500.Google Scholar
Kuo, W.-J., Yeh, T.-C., Lee, C.-Y. et al. (2003). Frequency effects of Chinese character processing in the brain: An event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage, 18, 720730.Google Scholar
Kuo, W.-J., Yeh, T.-C., Lee, J.-R. et al. (2004). Orthographic and phonological processing of Chinese characters: An fMRI study. Neuroimage, 21, 17211731.Google Scholar
Lallier, M., Thierry, G., Tainturier, M.-J. et al. (2009). Auditory and visual stream segregation in children and adults: An assessment of the amodality assumption of the “sluggish attentional shifting” theory of dyslexia. Brain Research, 1302, 132147.Google Scholar
Landerl, K., Ramus, F., Moll, K. et al. (2013). Predictors of developmental dyslexia in European orthographies with varying complexity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54, 686694. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12029.Google Scholar
Lee, C.-Y., Tsai, J.-L., Kuo, W.-J. et al. (2004). Neuronal correlates of consistency and frequency effects on Chinese character naming: An event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage, 23, 12351245.Google Scholar
Lehongre, K., Morillon, B., Giraud, A.-L., & Ramus, F. (2013). Impaired auditory sampling in dyslexia: Further evidence from combined fMRI and EEG. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 454.Google Scholar
Lehongre, K., Ramus, F., Villiermet, N., Schwartz, D., & Giraud, A.-L. (2011). Altered low-gamma sampling in auditory cortex accounts for the three main facets of dyslexia. Neuron, 72, 10801090.Google Scholar
Liberman, A. M. (1996). Speech: A special code. London: MIT press.Google Scholar
Liberman, I. Y., Shankweiler, D., & Liberman, A. M. (1989). The alphabetic principle and learning to read. In Shankweiler, D. & Liberman, I. Y. (Eds.), International academy for research in learning disabilities monograph series, no. 6. Phonology and reading disability: Solving the reading puzzle (pp. 133). Ann Arbor, MI: The University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
Liu, C., Zhang, W.-T., Tang, Y.-Y. et al. (2008). The visual word form area: Evidence from an fMRI study of implicit processing of Chinese characters. Neuroimage, 40, 13501361.Google Scholar
Lobier, M. A., Peyrin, C., Pichat, C., Le Bas, J.-F., & Valdois, S. (2014). Visual processing of multiple elements in the dyslexic brain: Evidence for a superior parietal dysfunction. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 479.Google Scholar
Lyytinen, H., Aro, M., Eklund, K. et al. (2004). The development of children at familial risk for dyslexia: Birth to early school age. Annals of Dyslexia, 54, 184220.Google Scholar
Maisog, J. M., Einbinder, E. R., Flowers, D. L., Turkeltaub, P. E., & Eden, G. F. (2008). A Meta‐analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of dyslexia. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1145, 237259.Google Scholar
Manent, J.-B., & Represa, A. (2007). Neurotransmitters and brain maturation: Early paracrine actions of GABA and glutamate modulate neuronal migration. The Neuroscientist, 13, 268279.Google Scholar
Mann, V., & Wimmer, H. (2002). Phoneme awareness and pathways into literacy: A comparison of German and American children. Reading and Writing, 15, 653682. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1020984704781.Google Scholar
McBride-Chang, C., Cho, J.-R., Liu, H. et al. (2005). Changing models across cultures: Associations of phonological awareness and morphological structure awareness with vocabulary and word recognition in second graders from Beijing, Hong Kong, Korea, and the United States. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92, 140160.Google Scholar
McBride-Chang, C., Shu, H., Zhou, A. et al. (2003). Morphological awareness uniquely predicts young children’s Chinese character recognition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 743751.Google Scholar
McNorgan, C., Randazzo-Wagner, M., & Booth, J. R. (2013). Cross-modal integration in the brain is related to phonological awareness only in typical readers, not in those with reading difficulty. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 388. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00388.Google Scholar
Meng, X., Tian, X., Jian, J., & Zhou, X. (2007). Orthographic and phonological processing in Chinese dyslexic children: An ERP study on sentence reading. Brain Research, 1179, 119130.Google Scholar
Moll, K., Ramus, F., Bartling, J. et al. (2014). Cognitive mechanisms underlying reading and spelling development in five European orthographies. Learning and Instruction, 29, 6577.Google Scholar
Nakamura, K., Kuo, W.-J., Pegado, F. et al. (2012). Universal brain systems for recognizing word shapes and handwriting gestures during reading. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109, 2076220767. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1217749109.Google Scholar
Newman, E. H., Tardif, T., Huang, J. Y., & Shu, H. (2011). Phonemes matter: The role of phoneme-level awareness in emergent Chinese readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 242259.Google Scholar
Paulesu, E., Danelli, L., & Berlingeri, M. (2014). Reading the dyslexic brain: Multiple dysfunctional routes revealed by a new meta-analysis of PET and fMRI activation studies. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 830. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00830.Google Scholar
Paulesu, E., Démonet, J.-F., Fazio, F. et al. (2001). Dyslexia: Cultural diversity and biological unity. Science, 291, 21652167.Google Scholar
Paulesu, E., Frith, U., Snowling, M. et al. (1996). Is developmental dyslexia a disconnection syndrome. Brain, 119, 143157.Google Scholar
Paulesu, E., McCrory, E., Fazio, F. et al. (2000). A cultural effect on brain function. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 9196.Google Scholar
Perreault, C., & Mathew, S. (2012). Dating the origin of language using phonemic diversity. PLoS One, 7, e35289. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0035289.Google Scholar
Petroff, O. A. (2002). Book review: GABA and glutamate in the human brain. The Neuroscientist, 8, 562573.Google Scholar
Power, A. J., Mead, N., Barnes, L., & Goswami, U. (2013). Neural entrainment to rhythmic speech in children with developmental dyslexia. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 777.Google Scholar
Preston, J. L., Molfese, P. J., Frost, S. J. et al. (2016). Print-speech convergence predicts future reading outcomes in early readers. Psychological Science, 27, 7584.Google Scholar
Price, C. J. (2000). The anatomy of language: Contributions from functional neuroimaging. Journal of Anatomy, 197, 335359.Google Scholar
Price, C. J. (2012). A review and synthesis of the first 20 years of PET and fMRI studies of heard speech, spoken language and reading. Neuroimage, 62, 816847.Google Scholar
Pugh, K. R., Frost, S. J., Rothman, D. L. et al. (2014). Glutamate and choline levels predict individual differences in reading ability in emergent readers. The Journal of Neuroscience, 34, 40824089.Google Scholar
Pugh, K. R., Frost, S. J., Sandak, R. et al. (2008). Effects of stimulus difficulty and repetition on printed word identification: An fMRI comparison of nonimpaired and reading-disabled adolescent cohorts. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 11461160.Google Scholar
Pugh, K. R., Landi, N., Preston, J. L. et al. (2013). The relationship between phonological and auditory processing and brain organization in beginning readers. Brain and Language, 125, 173183. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2012.04.004.Google Scholar
Pugh, K. R., Mencl, W. E., Jenner, A. R. et al. (2000). Functional neuroimaging studies of reading and reading disability (developmental dyslexia). Mental Retardation & Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 6, 207213.Google Scholar
Pugh, K. R., Mencl, W. E., Shaywitz, B. A. et al. (2000). The angular gyrus in developmental dyslexia: Task-specific differences in functional connectivity within posterior cortex. Psychological Science, 11, 5156.Google Scholar
Pugh, K. R., Sandak, R., Frost, S. J., Moore, D. L., & Mencl, W. E. (2006). Neurobiological investigations of skilled and impaired reading. In Dickinson, D. K. & Neuman, S. B. (Eds.), Handbook of early literacy research (Vol. 2, pp. 6474). New York, NY: Guilford Publications.Google Scholar
Pugh, K. R., Shaywitz, B. A., Shaywitz, S. E. et al. (1996). Cerebral organization of component processes in reading. Brain, 119, 12211238.Google Scholar
Ramus, F. (2003). Developmental dyslexia: Specific phonological deficit or general sensorimotor dysfunction? Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 13, 212218.Google Scholar
Rao, C., & Singh, N. C. (2015). Visuospatial complexity modulates reading in the brain. Brain and Language, 141, 5061.Google Scholar
Rawson, M. (1978). Dyslexia and learning disabilities: Their relationship. Bulletin of the Orton Society, 28, 4361. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02653425.Google Scholar
Richlan, F. (2012). Developmental dyslexia: Dysfunction of a left hemisphere reading network. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 120.Google Scholar
Richlan, F. (2014). Functional neuroanatomy of developmental dyslexia: The role of orthographic depth. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 347. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00347.Google Scholar
Richlan, F., Kronbichler, M., & Wimmer, H. (2009). Functional abnormalities in the dyslexic brain: A quantitative meta‐analysis of neuroimaging studies. Human Brain Mapping, 30, 32993308.Google Scholar
Richlan, F., Kronbichler, M., & Wimmer, H. (2011). Meta-analyzing brain dysfunctions in dyslexic children and adults. Neuroimage, 56, 17351742.Google Scholar
Rieben, L., & Perfetti, C. A. (1991). Learning to read: Basic research and its implications. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Rueckl, J. G., Paz-Alonso, P. M., & Molfese, P. J. et al. (2015). Universal brain signature of proficient reading: Evidence from four contrasting languages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112, 1551015515.Google Scholar
Rumsey, J. M., Andreason, P., & Zametkin, A. J. (1992). Failure to activate the left temporoparietal cortex in dyslexia: An oxygen 15 positron emission tomographic study. Archives of Neurology, 49, 527534. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archneur.1992.00530290115020.Google Scholar
Salmelin, R., Kiesilä, P., Uutela, K., Service, E., & Salonen, O. (1996). Impaired visual word processing in dyslexia revealed with magnetoencephalography. Annals of Neurology, 40, 157162.Google Scholar
Sandak, R., Mencl, W. E., Frost, S. J. et al. (2004). The neurobiology of adaptive learning in reading: A contrast of different training conditions. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 4, 6788.Google Scholar
Sarkari, S., Simos, P., Fletcher, J. et al. (2002). The emergence and treatment of developmental reading disability: Contributions of functional brain imaging. Seminars in Pediatric Neurology, 9, 227236.Google Scholar
Scarborough, H. S. (1998). Early identification of children at risk for reading disabilities: Phonological awareness and some other promising predictors. In Shapiro, B. K., Accardo, P. J., & Capute, A. J. (Eds.), Specific reading disability: A view of the spectrum (pp. 75119). Timonium, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Schlaggar, B. L., Brown, T. T., Lugar, H. M. et al. (2002). Functional neuroanatomical differences between adults and school-age children in the processing of single words. Science, 296, 14761479.Google Scholar
Schurz, M., Wimmer, H., Richlan, F. et al. (2014). Resting-state and task-based functional brain connectivity in developmental dyslexia. Cerebral Cortex, 25, 35023514. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhu184.Google Scholar
Shankweiler, D., Crain, S., Katz, L. et al. (1995). Cognitive profiles of reading-disabled children: Comparison of language skills in phonology, morphology, and syntax. Psychological Science, 6, 149156.Google Scholar
Shankweiler, D., Mencl, W. E., Braze, D. et al. (2008). Reading differences and brain: Cortical integration of speech and print in sentence processing varies with reader skill. Developmental Neuropsychology, 33, 745775.Google Scholar
Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (2003). The science of reading and dyslexia. Journal of American Association for Pediatric Ophthalmology and Strabismus, 7, 158166. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s1091-8531(03)00002-8.Google Scholar
Shaywitz, B. A., Shaywitz, S. E., Blachman, B. A. et al. (2004). Development of left occipitotemporal systems for skilled reading in children after a phonologically-based intervention. Biological Psychiatry, 55, 926933.Google Scholar
Shaywitz, S. E., Shaywitz, B. A., & Pugh, K. R. et al. (1998). Functional disruption in the organization of the brain for reading in dyslexia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 95, 26362641.Google Scholar
Shaywitz, B. A., Shaywitz, S. E., Pugh, K. R. et al. (2002). Disruption of posterior brain systems for reading in children with developmental dyslexia. Biological Psychiatry, 52, 101110.Google Scholar
Shu, H., Anderson, R. C., & Wu, N. N. (2000). Phonetic awareness: Knowledge of orthography-phonology relationships in the character acquisition of Chinese children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 5662.Google Scholar
Shu, H., McBride-Chang, C., Wu, S., & Liu, H. Y. (2006). Understanding Chinese developmental dyslexia: Morphological awareness as a core cognitive construct. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 122133.Google Scholar
Shu, H., Meng, X., Chen, X., Luan, H., & Cao, F. (2005). The subtypes of developmental dyslexia in Chinese: Evidence from three cases. Dyslexia, 11, 311329.Google Scholar
Shu, H., Peng, H., & McBride-Chang, C. (2008). Phonological awareness in young Chinese children. Developmental Science, 11, 171181. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00654.x.Google Scholar
Simos, P. G., Breier, J., Fletcher, J., Bergman, E., & Papanicolaou, A. (2000). Cerebral mechanisms involved in word reading in dyslexic children: A magnetic source imaging approach. Cerebral Cortex, 10, 809816.Google Scholar
Siok, W. T., Niu, Z., Jin, Z., Perfetti, C. A., & Tan, L. H. (2008). A structural–functional basis for dyslexia in the cortex of Chinese readers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105, 55615566. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0801750105.Google Scholar
Siok, W. T., Perfetti, C. A., Jin, Z., & Tan, L. H. (2004). Biological abnormality of impaired reading is constrained by culture. Nature, 431, 7176.Google Scholar
Siok, W. T., Spinks, J. A., Jin, Z., & Tan, L. H. (2009). Developmental dyslexia is characterized by the co-existence of visuospatial and phonological disorders in Chinese children. Current Biology, 19, R890R892.Google Scholar
Snowling, M. J., & Hulme, C. (2008). The science of reading: A handbook (Vol. 9). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/9780470757642.Google Scholar
Soltész, F., Szűcs, D., Leong, V., White, S., & Goswami, U. (2013). Differential entrainment of neuroelectric delta oscillations in developmental dyslexia. PLoS One, 8, e76608. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0076608.Google Scholar
Spitsyna, G., Warren, J. E., Scott, S. K., Turkheimer, F. E., & Wise, R. J. (2006). Converging language streams in the human temporal lobe. The Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 73287336.Google Scholar
Tallal, P. (1984). Temporal or phonetic processing deficit in dyslexia? That is the question. Applied Psycholinguistics, 5, 167169.Google Scholar
Tan, L. H., Hoosain, R., & Peng, D.-L. (1995). Role of early presemantic phonological code in Chinese character identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 4354.Google Scholar
Temple, E., Deutsch, G. K., Poldrack, R. A. et al. (2003). Neural deficits in children with dyslexia ameliorated by behavioral remediation: Evidence from functional MRI. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100, 28602865.Google Scholar
Temple, E., Poldrack, R. A., Salidis, J. et al. (2001). Disrupted neural responses to phonological and orthographic processing in dyslexic children: An fMRI study. Neuroreport, 12, 299307.Google Scholar
Turkeltaub, P. E., Gareau, L., Flowers, D. L., Zeffiro, T. A., & Eden, G. F. (2003). Development of neural mechanisms for reading. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 767773.Google Scholar
Verhoeven, L., & Perfetti, C. (2003). Introduction to this special issue: The role of morphology in learning to read. Scientific Studies of Reading, 7, 209217.Google Scholar
Viana, A. R., Razuk, M., de Freitas, P. B., & Barela, J. A. (2013). Sensorimotor integration in dyslexic children under different sensory stimulations. PLoS One, 8, e72719. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0072719.Google Scholar
Von Karolyi, C., Winner, E., Gray, W., & Sherman, G. F. (2003). Dyslexia linked to talent: Global visual-spatial ability. Brain and Language, 85, 427431.Google Scholar
Wang, J.-J., Bi, H.-Y., Gao, L.-Q., & Wydell, T. N. (2010). The visual magnocellular pathway in Chinese-speaking children with developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 48, 36273633.Google Scholar
Wang, W. S., & Tsai, Y. (2011). The alphabet and the sinogram. Dyslexia across cultures. Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publishing.Google Scholar
Wang, X., Yang, J., Yang, J. et al. (2015). Language differences in the brain network for reading in naturalistic story reading and lexical decision. PLoS One, 10, e0124388. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0124388.Google Scholar
Wheat, K. L., Cornelissen, P. L., Frost, S. J., & Hansen, P. C. (2010). During visual word recognition, phonology is accessed within 100 ms and may be mediated by a speech production code: Evidence from magnetoencephalography. The Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 52295233.Google Scholar
Wimmer, H., Mayringer, H., & Landerl, K. (2000). The double-deficit hypothesis and difficulties in learning to read a regular orthography. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 668680. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.92.4.668.Google Scholar
Wimmer, H., Schurz, M., Sturm, D. et al. (2010). A dual-route perspective on poor reading in a regular orthography: An fMRI study. Cortex, 46, 12841298.Google Scholar
Wu, C.-Y., Ho, M.-H. R., & Chen, S.-H. A. (2012). A meta-analysis of fMRI studies on Chinese orthographic, phonological, and semantic processing. Neuroimage, 63, 381391.Google Scholar
Wu, N. N., Zhou, X. L., & Shu, H. (1999). Sublexical processing in reading Chinese: A development study. Language and Cognitive Processes, 14, 503524.Google Scholar
Yang, J., Wang, X., Shu, H., & Zevin, J. D. (2011). Brain networks associated with sublexical properties of Chinese characters. Brain and Language, 119, 6879.Google Scholar
Yoncheva, Y. N., Zevin, J. D., Maurer, U., & McCandliss, B. D. (2010). Auditory selective attention to speech modulates activity in the visual word form area. Cerebral Cortex, 20, 622632.Google Scholar
Zhao, J. [Jing], Qian, Y., Bi, H.-Y., & Coltheart, M. (2014). The visual magnocellular-dorsal dysfunction in Chinese children with developmental dyslexia impedes Chinese character recognition. Scientific Reports, 4, 7068. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/srep07068.Google Scholar
Zhao, J. [Jingjing], Wang, X., Frost, S. J. et al. (2014). Neural division of labor in reading is constrained by culture: A training study of reading Chinese characters. Cortex, 53, 90106.Google Scholar
Zhou, X., & Marslen-Wilson, W. (1999). Phonology, orthography, and semantic activation in reading Chinese. Journal of Memory and Language, 41, 579606.Google Scholar
Zhou, X., & Marslen-Wilson, W. (2000). The relative time course of semantic and phonological activation in reading Chinese. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 12451265.Google Scholar
Ziegler, J. C., Bertrand, D., Tóth, D. et al. (2010). Orthographic depth and its impact on universal predictors of reading: A cross-language investigation. Psychological Science, 21, 551559. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797610363406.Google Scholar
Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 329.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×