Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-7cvxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T15:52:00.622Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

14 - Prosodic Pragmatics in Context

from Part III - Pragmatic Approaches to Context

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 November 2023

Jesús Romero-Trillo
Affiliation:
Universidad Autónoma de Madrid
Get access

Summary

The relationship between context and prosody is undoubtedly one of the most intuitive ones in language. At the same time, it is one of the most difficult to describe because it is based on acoustic cues that only need milliseconds to create an image in our brain. However, speakers of a language can generally understand their interlocutors’ emotional and cognitive status through their prosodic realization. Prosodic pragmatics is the branch of pragmatics that attempts to identify the intentionality of the speaker’s meaning in a real context based on the analysis of the suprasegmental aspects of speech production. If prosody studies how an utterance is pronounced in unison with the perceptual features of pitch, length, and loudness, then prosodic pragmatics studies the acoustic and cognitive contextual parameters in conversation. The chapter will show the relationship between prosody, information, and context in communication. Starting from the essential acoustic parameters of speech, it will revise the most influential theories of intonation through the prosodic pragmatics lens to understand the cognitive adaptation of a message in a specific context.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2023

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bazzanella, C., and Damiano, R. (1999). The interactional handling of misunderstanding in everyday conversations. Journal of Pragmatics, 31, 817836.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolinger, D. (1958). A theory of pitch accent in English. Word, 14, 109149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolinger, D. (1972). Accent is predictable (if you’re a mind reader). Language, 37, 8396.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolinger, D. (1978). Intonation across languages. In Greenberg, J. H., Ferguson, C. A., and Moravcsik, E. A. (eds.), Universals of Human Language, Vol. II: Phonology (pp. 471–524). Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Brazil, D. (1975). Discourse Intonation 1/2. Birmingham: Birmingham University Press.Google Scholar
Brown, P., and Levinson, S. C. (1978). Politeness: Some Universals in Language Usage. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Chafe, W. L. (1974). Language and consciousness. Language, 50, 111133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chafe, W. L. (1987). Cognitive constraints on information flow. In Tomlin, R. S. (ed.), Coherence and Grounding in Discourse (pp. 2151). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, H. H., and Haviland, S. E. (1977). Comprehension and the given-new contract. In R. D. Freedle, (ed.), Discourse Production and Comprehension (pp. 140). Norwood: Ablex Publishing Corporation.Google Scholar
Cooper, W. E., Eady, S. J., and Mueller, P. R. (1985). Acoustical aspects of contrastive stress in question–answer contexts. Journal of the Acoustic Society of America, 77, 21422156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cruttenden, A. (1997). Intonation, 2nd ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cruz-Ferreira, M. (1987). Non-native interpretative strategies for intonational meaning. In Leather, J. A. and Leather, J. (eds.), Sound Patterns in Second Language Acquisition (pp. 103120). Dordrecht: Foris Publications.Google Scholar
Crystal, D. (1986). Prosodic development. In Fletcher, P. J. and Garman, M. (eds.), Studies in First Language Development (pp. 174–197). New York: Cambridge University Press. Google Scholar
De Jong, K., and Zawaydeh, B. (2002). Comparing stress, lexical focus and segmental focus: patterns of variation in Arabic vowel duration. Journal of Phonetics, 30, 5375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeRose, K. (1999). Contextualism: An explanation and defense. In Greco, J. and Sosa, E. (eds.), The Blackwell Guide to Epistemology (pp. 187–206). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Eady, S. J., and Cooper, W. E. (1986). Speech intonation and focus location in matched statements and questions. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 80, 402415.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eefting, W. 1991. The effect of ‘information value’ and ‘accentuation’ on the duration of Dutch words, syllables and segments. Journal of the Acoustic Society of America 89: 412423.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fetzer, A., and Oishi, E., eds. (2011). Context and Contexts: Parts Meet Whole? Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fry, D. B. (1954). Duration and intensity as physical correlates of linguistic stress. Journal of the Acoustic Society of America, 27, 765768.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geluykens, R. (1991). Information flow in English conversation: A new approach to the Given-New distinction. In E. Ventola, (ed.), Functional and Systemic Linguistics. Approaches and Uses (pp. 141–169). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. Google Scholar
Giora, R. (2003). On Our Mind: Salience, Context and Figurative Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glandzberg, M. (2002). Context and discourse. Mind and Language, 17, 333375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodwin, C., and Duranti, A. (1992). Rethinking context: An introduction. In Duranti, A. and Goodwin, C. (eds.), Rethinking Context: Language as an Interactive Phenomenon (pp. 142). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Google Scholar
Gussenhoven, C. (1984). On the Grammar and Semantics of Sentence Accents. Dordrecht: Foris.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halliday, M. A. K. (1967). Intonation and Grammar in British English. The Hague: MoutonCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halliday, M. A. K. (1970). A Course in Spoken English: Intonation. London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Halliday, M. A. K., and Hasan, R. (1989). Language, Context and Text: Aspects of Language in a Social-Semiotic Perspective, 2nd ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Halliday, M. A. K., and Matthiessen, M. I. M. (2014). Halliday’s Introduction to Functional Grammar, 4th ed. Oxford: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haviland, S. E., and Clark, H. (1974). What’s new? Acquiring new information as a process in comprehension. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 13, 512521.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heldner, M., and Strangert, E. (2001). Temporal effects of focus in Swedish. Journal of Phonetics, 29, 329361.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heritage, J. (1984). Garfinkel and ethnomethodology: Developments in Conversation Analysis. Sociolinguistics Newsletter, 1, 116.Google Scholar
Kecskes, I. (2006). On my mind: Thoughts about salience, context and figurative language from a second language perspective. Second Language Research, 22, 219237. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kecskes, I. (2008). Dueling contexts: A dynamic model of meaning. Journal of Pragmatics, 40, 385406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kecskes, I., and Fenghui, Z. (2009). Activating, seeking, and creating common ground: A socio-cognitive approach. Pragmatics & Cognition, 17, 331355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ladd, R. (1980). The Structure of Intonational Meaning: Evidence from English. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
Ladd, R. (1996). Intonational Phonology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Leckie-Tarry, H. (1995). Language and Context: A Functional Linguistic Theory of Register. London/New York: Pinter Publishers.Google Scholar
Lehiste, I. (1970). Suprasegmentals. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Levinson, S. C. (1983). Pragmatics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liberman, P. (1960). Some acoustic correlates of word stress in American English. Journal of the Acoustic Society of America, 32, 451454.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lieberman, P. (1963). Some effects of semantic and grammatical context on the productions and perception of speech. Language and Speech, 6, 172187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Madella, P., and Romero-Trillo, , J. (2019). Prosodic Pointing in inferential comprehension: The application of Relevance Theory to L2 listening instruction. Letronica 4. http://dx.doi.org/10.15448/1984-4301.2019.4.34157.Google Scholar
Malinowski, B. [1923] (1989). The problem of meaning in primitive languages. In Ogden, C. and Richards, I. (eds.), The Meaning of Meaning (pp. 451510). New York: Harcourt, Brace and World.Google Scholar
Nooteboom, S. G., and Kruyt, J. G. (1987). Accents, focus distribution and the perceived distribution of given and new information: an experiment. Journal of the Acoustic Society of America, 82, 15121524.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Connor, J. D., and Gordon, F. A. (1973). Intonation of Colloquial English. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Pierrehumbert, J. (1980). The phonology and phonetics of English intonation. Ph.D. dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.Google Scholar
Pierrehumbert, J., and Hirschberg, J. (1990). The meaning of intonation contours in the interpretation of discourse. In Philip, R. Cohen, J. Morgan, and M. E. Polack, (eds.), Intention in Communication (pp. 371411). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Google Scholar
Pollock, K. E., Brammer, D. M., and Hageman, C. F. (1993). An acoustic analysis of young children’s productions of word stress. Journal of Phonetics, 21, 183203.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pomerantz, A. (1984). Agreeing and disagreeing with assessments: Some features of preferred/dispreferred turn shapes. In Atkinson, J. and Heritage, J. (eds.), Structures of Social Action (pp. 57101). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Google Scholar
Prince, E. F. (1981). Toward a taxonomy of given-new information. In Cole, P. (ed.), Radical Pragmatics (pp. 223255). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Ramírez-Verdugo, M. D. (2021). Intonation in L2 Discourse: Research Insights. London: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Recanati, F. (2002). Does communication rest on inference? Mind & Language, 17, 105126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Romero-Trillo, J. (1993). Reading aloud and the structure of information. Estudios Ingleses de la Universidad Complutense, 1, 133142.Google Scholar
Romero-Trillo, J. (1994). Ahm, ehm … You call it theme? A thematic approach to spoken English. Journal of Pragmatics, 22, 495509.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Romero-Trillo, J. (1997). Your attention please: Pragmatic mechanisms to obtain the addressee’s attention in English and Spanish conversations. Journal of Pragmatics, 28, 205221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Romero Trillo, J. (2001). A mathematical model for the analysis of variation in discourse. Journal of Linguistics, 37, 527550.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Romero-Trillo, J. (2002). The pragmatic fossilization of discourse markers in non-native speakers of English. Journal of Pragmatics, 34, 769784.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Romero-Trillo, J., ed. (2012). Pragmatics and Prosody in English Language Teaching. Dordrecht: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Romero-Trillo, J. (2014). “Pragmatic punting” and prosody: Evidence from corpora. In Gómez González, M. A., Ruíz de Mendoza Ibáñez, F. J., Gonzálvez García, F., and Downing, A. (eds.), The Functional Perspective on Language and Discourse: Applications and Implications (pp. 209–221). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Google Scholar
Romero-Trillo, J. (2018a). Corpus pragmatics and second language pragmatics: A mutualistic entente in theory and practice. Corpus Pragmatics, 2, 113127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Romero-Trillo, J. (2018b). Prosodic modeling and position analysis of pragmatic markers in English conversation. Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory, 14, 169195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Romero-Trillo, J. (2019). Prosodic pragmatics and feedback in intercultural communication. Journal of Pragmatics, 151, 91102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Romero-Trillo, J. (2023). “Life is a journey” (literally): Conflict and emotion in the narratives of migrants and refugees. Talk delivered at King’s College London, January 18, 2023.Google Scholar
Romero-Trillo, J., and Maguire, L. (2011). Adaptive context: The fourth element of meaning. International Review of Pragmatics, 3, 228241.Google Scholar
Romero-Trillo, J. and Newell, J. (2012). Prosody and feedback in native and non-native speakers of English. In Romero-Trillo, J. (ed.), Pragmatics, Prosody and English Language Teaching (pp. 117–132). Dordrecht: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Romero-Trillo, J., Ramírez-Verdugo, D., Jiménez-Vilches, R., Mestre-Mestre, E., Avila, Ledesma, Sadeghi, N. Y., and Madella, P. (2022). Teaching English Prosodic Pragmatics APP (TEPP). Available in Google Play and Apple Store.Google Scholar
Searle, J. (1969). Speech Acts. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snow, D., and Balog, H. L. (2002). Do children produce the melody before the words? A review of developmental intonation research. Lingua, 112, 10251058.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sperber, D., and Wilson, D. (1986/1995). Relevance: Communication and Cognition. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Svartvik, J. (1990). The London–Lund Corpus of Spoken English: Description and Research. Lund: Lund University Press/Bromley: Chartwell-Brat.Google Scholar
Terken, J. (1991). Fundamental frequency and perceived prominence of accented syllables. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 89, 17681776.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vanderslice, R., and Ladefoged, P. (1972). Binary suprasegmental features and transformational word-accentuation rule. Language, 48, 819838.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ward, G., and Birner, B. J. (2001). Discourse and information structure. In Schiffrin, D., Tannen, D., and Hamilton, H. (eds.), Handbook of Discourse Analysis (pp. 119137). Oxford: Basil Blackwell. Google Scholar
Ward, G., and Birner, B. J. (2003). Information structure. In Horn, L. R. and Ward, G. (eds.), Handbook of Pragmatics (pp. 153174). Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Wierzbicka, A. (2010). Cross-cultural communication and miscommunication: The role of cultural keywords. Intercultural Pragmatic, 7, 123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×