learning how to mark tense, aspect, and mood
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 18 December 2014
Introduction
Previous research (e.g., Lipski 1993; Montrul 2009; Silva-Corvalán 1994; Zentella 1997) has shown that in situations of societal bilingualism a functionally restricted language evidences grammatical simplification, among other phenomena. The question that arises is whether this stage of grammatical simplification is due to imperfect or incomplete acquisition in the early years of a bilingual’s life, or is a result of processes of attrition or loss of acquired knowledge of the underused language (Cuza 2010; Montrul 2002, 2005). I explore this question in this chapter, in addition to the issue that underlies the various chapters of the book, namely, the effect that differing levels of proficiency have on bilingual development.
The empirical study is conducted on data from the two developing bilingual siblings (Nico and Brennan), from the very first production of what in adult language would be considered to be “a verb” until age 5;11, when the siblings began to attend kindergarten in the Spanish-immersion track of a dual-language school (see Chapter 2). In this case, the central question of what effect reduced exposure to a language may have is explored in the development of tense, mood, and aspect markers (TMA) in English and Spanish. To this end, the chapter presents a description of the path of development of verb inflections in the two languages. It also gives information about the siblings’ verb lexicon in the early age stages. Although the study is centered on the siblings’ productive abilities, it is assumed that their comprehension abilities are at least equally developed. There is no evidence of misunderstandings in the early stages, but some instances of misunderstood conditionals in Spanish are attested in the later years, when communication includes reference to hypothetical situations and the language addressed to the children is more complex linguistically and in terms of content.
To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.
Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.
Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.
To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.
To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.