We use cookies to distinguish you from other users and to provide you with a better experience on our websites. Close this message to accept cookies or find out how to manage your cookie settings.
To save content items to your account,
please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies.
If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account.
Find out more about saving content to .
To save content items to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected]
is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings
on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part
of your Kindle email address below.
Find out more about saving to your Kindle.
Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations.
‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi.
‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.
Experimental analysis of drinking in humans has yielded inconsistent results, ranging from no deficits relative to young groups, to substantial deficits. It is possible that cognitive or other factors can account for some of these differences between studies. Age-related dehydration in at-risk human populations may be minimized by ensuring adequate food intake and meal-associated drinking and/or provision of foods with high fluid content. With the exception of hypotension-related stimuli, age-related declines in drinking by rats after about 25% of the life span are small. Increased drinking at younger ages (3–4 mo) seems to be the unexplained anomaly; a generic explanation would be maturation in late adolescence of an as-yet unrecognized inhibitory mechanism.
Recommend this
Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this to your organisation's collection.