Hostname: page-component-5cf477f64f-54txb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-04-05T22:33:12.368Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Understanding drivers of loneliness: machine learning insights from the HILDA survey

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 February 2025

A response to the following question: Does declining social connection, and increased reported loneliness, explain the apparent increase in depressive and other mood disorders, particularly among younger people?

Isabel Li
Affiliation:
Brain and Mind Centre, Faculty of Medicine and Health, University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia
Adam Skinner
Affiliation:
Brain and Mind Centre, Faculty of Medicine and Health, University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia
Mathew Varidel
Affiliation:
Brain and Mind Centre, Faculty of Medicine and Health, University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia
Ian B. Hickie
Affiliation:
Brain and Mind Centre, Faculty of Medicine and Health, University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia
Jo-An Occhipinti*
Affiliation:
Brain and Mind Centre, Faculty of Medicine and Health, University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia Computer Simulation & Advanced Research Technologies, Sydney, NSW, Australia
*
Corresponding author: Jo-An Occhipinti; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background:

Loneliness has emerged as a pervasive public health challenge. Understanding loneliness and its associated risk factors is crucial for developing interventions to address this issue effectively. This study aimed to investigate loneliness among adults living in Australia, comparing different age cohorts.

Method:

This study used 10,815, 11,234, 14,670 and 15,049 records with loneliness measurements taken at 2006, 2010, 2014 and 2018, respectively, from the Household, Income and Labour Dynamics in Australia (HILDA) survey. A supervised machine learning algorithm, CatBoost, was employed to predict loneliness. Model predictions were explained using SHapley Additive exPlanations (SHAP) and partial dependence plots across five age-based subgroups to capture life stage variations.

Results:

Mental well-being, having a life partner, social connectedness and social fulfilment were the most important predictors of loneliness at the whole-population level. Among young adults, the level of friendship fulfilment, financial satisfaction and health status were relatively strong predictors of loneliness, while loneliness in older adults was more strongly associated with spare time fulfilment, community satisfaction and the loss of loved ones. Youth who reported that they did not have a lot of friends were predicted to have a 46.5% (95% CI: 45.9%–47.2%) chance of experiencing loneliness. Seniors have a 44.9% (95% CI: 43.9%–45.8%) chance of experiencing loneliness if they were almost always not fulfilled in their spare time.

Implications:

This study underscores the need to recognise the heterogeneity of loneliness across the lifecourse and the importance of both targeted strategies and efforts to improve broader social cohesion.

Type
Results
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Despite decades of peace and prosperity in industrialised nations post-World War II, a concerning paradox has emerged: loneliness is increasingly recognised as a significant public health issue. Economic prosperity often masks the profound social and emotional challenges individuals face, with economic policies contributing to those challenges – particularly by increasing inequality, disrupting social connections and exacerbating isolation (Occhipinti et al., Reference Occhipinti, Skinner and Doraiswamy2024). Loneliness, a facet of mental well-being, has reached epidemic levels, according to former US Surgeon General Vivek Murthy (Reference Murthy2017). In developed countries, approximately a third of the population contends with loneliness, with 1 in 12 severely affected (Cacioppo and Cacioppo, Reference Cacioppo and Cacioppo2018). This trend is prevalent in Australia, where loneliness was a substantial concern even before the COVID-19 pandemic (Lim et al., Reference Lim, Badcock and Smith2020), affecting approximately one in five Australians (Welfare, 2023). Loneliness, defined as distress due to inadequate social relationships (Hawkley and Cacioppo, Reference Hawkley and Cacioppo2010; Peplau and Perlman, Reference Peplau and Perlman1982; Badcock et al., Reference Badcock, Holt-Lunstad, Garcia, Bombaci and Lim2022), has far-reaching consequences beyond subjective feelings. It is associated with deteriorating mental well-being (Wang et al., Reference Wang, Mann, Lloyd-Evans, Ma and Johnson2018) and suicidal ideation (McClelland et al., Reference McClelland, Evans, Nowland, Ferguson and O’Connor2020; Stravynski and Boyer, Reference Stravynski and Boyer2001), as well as increased risks of dementia, Alzheimer’s disease (Wilson et al., Reference Wilson, Krueger and Arnold2007; Salinas et al., Reference Salinas, Beiser and Samra2022), cardiovascular diseases (Hawkley and Cacioppo, Reference Hawkley and Cacioppo2003) and stroke (Valtorta et al., Reference Valtorta, Kanaan, Gilbody, Ronzi and Hanratty2016). Long-term loneliness is linked to a 26% higher risk of mortality (Holt-Lunstad et al., Reference Holt-Lunstad, Smith, Baker, Harris and Stephenson2015). Additionally, loneliness is a well-established social determinant of depression throughout the lifecourse, with a sense of social sufficiency and the need for belongingness proposed as factors that modify the strength of this relationship, though the mechanism is likely to be complex (Erzen and Çikrikci, Reference Erzen and Çikrikci2018). Furthermore, evidence suggests a bidirectional relationship, with depression acting as a risk factor for the development of loneliness, while concurrently, loneliness functions as a precursor to the onset of depressive symptoms (Sbarra et al., Reference Sbarra, Ramadan and Choi2023). Scholarly attention has increasingly focused on other key influences on loneliness, particularly social connection. While some studies highlight social connection’s mitigating role (Victor and Yang, Reference Victor and Yang2012; Guthmuller, Reference Guthmuller2022; Franssen et al., Reference Franssen, Stijnen, Hamers and Schneider2020; Ejlskov et al., Reference Ejlskov, Wulff, Bøggild and Stafford2018; von et al., Reference von Soest, Luhmann, Hansen and Gerstorf2020; Hawkley and Kocherginsky, Reference Hawkley and Kocherginsky2018), others find no evidence of association (Dahlberg et al., Reference Dahlberg, McKee, Frank and Naseer2022; Nyqvist et al., Reference Nyqvist, Näsman, Hemberg and Nygård2023). However, research predominantly concentrates on specific demographics, particularly the elderly and populations from North America and Europe. Loneliness research in Australia is an emerging field, often concentrating on older age groups (Ogrin et al., Reference Ogrin, Cyarto and Harrington2021; Steed et al., Reference Steed, Boldy, Grenade and Iredell2007; Engel et al., Reference Engel, Lee, Le, Lal and Mihalopoulos2021), specific cohorts such as those with disability, cardiovascular disease or dementia (Freak-Poli et al., Reference Freak-Poli, Phyo, Hu and Barker2022; Moyle et al., Reference Moyle, Kellett, Ballantyne and Gracia2011; Bishop et al., Reference Bishop, Llewellyn and Kavanagh2024), and typically employing more traditional analytic methods. Responding to the global urgency highlighted by the World Health Organization commission to address loneliness, our study employs advanced machine learning techniques to predict loneliness among adults living in Australia. By investigating complex, non-linear relationships across different age groups, our approach examines whether new insights can be derived to inform strategies for addressing the challenge of loneliness across the lifecourse.

Methods

Data

The data for our study came from the Household, Income and Labour Dynamics in Australia (HILDA) survey – a nationally representative longitudinal study tracking the demographic, economic, social well-being and health of Australian households over time (Watson and Wooden, Reference Watson and Wooden2021). Data were collected via a combination of in-person interviews and self-completion questionnaires. This analysis focused on adults living in Australia (aged 18 and above) and extracted data from four waves spanning 2006, 2010, 2014 and 2018, with sample sizes of 10,815, 11,234, 14,670 and 15,049 individuals (Department of Social Services Melbourne Institute: Applied Economic & Social Research, 2023), respectively. These time points were specifically chosen due to the availability of social connection and participation measurements, aligning with the key interests of our study. Merging these datasets resulted in a final dataset comprising 51,768 entries. There were 9,643 entries excluded from the original data due to missing loneliness measurements. To model loneliness across life stages, the data were divided into five age-based subgroups: youth (18–30, n = 12,277), young adults (31–40, n = 8,774), middle adults (41–60, n = 18,187), late adults (61–75, n = 9,064) and seniors (76+, n = 3,466).

Outcome measure and features

The primary outcome measure, loneliness, was originally assessed using a 7-point scale to gauge agreement with the statement: “I often feel very lonely,” with responses ranging from 1 (completely disagree) to 7 (completely agree). To simplify interpretation, this study re-coded the loneliness variable into two categories: “not lonely” for responses lower than 4 and “lonely” for 4 and above. The considered features associated with loneliness encompass two main perspectives: fundamental and social integration, with the latter comprising both micro and macro levels. Fundamental features included age, gender, highest education achieved, perceived health status, mental and emotional well-being, frequency of physical activity, current labour force status, job satisfaction and satisfaction with financial situation.

The micro level of social integration considered individual-level social relationships and fulfilment. This included having a life partner, social connection and experiencing recent significant personal losses. Social fulfilment measures, including friendship fulfilment and spare time fulfilment. The macro aspect measures broader community connection levels, including community satisfaction and community participation. Community participation involved attending events, volunteering and club membership. Additionally, the study wave of data (four time points) was incorporated as a feature to explore potential associations with loneliness. See Supplementary Materials Appendix A.1 Table A.1 for data summary and Appendix A.2 for the methods used to construct derived features.

Categorical boosting

To predict loneliness, we employed the supervised machine learning algorithm CatBoost, which utilises gradient boosting to combine multiple weak learners, like decision trees, to address residuals from previous trees. Unlike other boosting algorithms, CatBoost efficiently manages categorical features, automatically encoding them during training (Dorogush et al., Reference Dorogush, Ershov and Gulin2018). Its ordered boosting technique effectively tackles data leakage and overfitting issues (Prokhorenkova et al., Reference Prokhorenkova, Gusev, Vorobev, Dorogush and Gulin2018). Recent studies have demonstrated CatBoost’s superiority over other gradient boosting methods like XGBoost and LightGBM (Sahin, Reference Sahin2022; Bentéjac et al., Reference Bentéjac, Csörgő and Martínez-Muñoz2021). Additionally, our non-parametric tree-based model offers greater flexibility in handling complex data and relationships compared to linear models. An accessible, health-focused introduction to gradient boosting methods is provided elsewhere (Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Zhao, Canes, Steinberg and Lyashevska2019).

Interpretation tools

To enhance the interpretability of our machine learning models, we employed the SHAP (SHapley Additive exPlanations) framework (Lundberg and Lee, Reference Lundberg and Lee2017), a model-agnostic approach that provides explanations for model outputs by assigning SHAP values to features, elucidating their contributions to predictions. Additionally, to visualise complex relationships captured by our models, we utilised partial dependence plots (PDP) (Hastie et al., Reference Hastie, Tibshirani, Friedman and Friedman2009), which illustrate how predicted probabilities of loneliness vary with changes in specific features. See Supplementary Materials Appendix A.3 and Appendix A.4 for detailed explanations and methodologies regarding the SHAP and PDP.

Modelling strategies

Figure A.1 (see Supplementary Materials Appendix A.5) illustrates the modelling workflow for analysing the full population. To construct models with good generalisation, we limited the potential for overfitting by performing cross-validation. We split the data into training and testing sets (70/30 ratio) using stratification to mirror the original dataset’s loneliness distribution. A recursive feature elimination based on SHAP values was employed for feature selection, retaining 10 features per model. Hyperparameters, including learning rate and L2 regularisation coefficient, were tuned using grid search and 5-fold cross-validation. The CatBoost built-in early stopping mechanism determined the optimal number of trees. Statistical modelling was performed in Python version 3.8.

Results

Exploratory data analyses

There was a monotonic increase in loneliness prevalence from 2006 (29.7% [95% CI: 28.8%–30.5%]) to 2018 (31.0% [95% CI: 30.3%–31.7%]) (Table A.2, Supplementary Materials Appendix A.6). Across age groups, prevalence formed a “W” shape, with individuals aged 31–40 and 61–75 experiencing less loneliness than those aged 18–30 and 76 and above. Seniors had the highest prevalence, with a noticeable decline since 2006, while other groups generally showed an increasing trend (Figure A.2, Supplementary Materials Appendix A.6). Loneliness rates among females consistently exceeded those of males, but the gender gap has gradually narrowed over time (2018: 29.7% [95% CI: 28.7%–30.8%], female 32.1% [95% CI: 31.1%–33.1%]; 2006: male 27.8% [95% CI: 26.5%–29.0%], female 31.3% [95% CI: 30.1%–32.5%]) (Figure A.3, Supplementary Materials Appendix A.6).

A data summary, stratified by loneliness status, is presented in Table A.1 in Supplementary Materials Appendix A.1. New composite features, “cp” and “connect,” were derived from original features related to community participation and social connectedness, respectively (Supplementary Materials Appendix A.2). Wilcoxon rank-sum tests and Pearson’s χ2 tests were used to assess differences in numerical and categorical features between individuals experiencing loneliness and those who were not. Results indicated significant associations between loneliness and all features except for the time points of data collection (“Wave”).

Statistical modelling results

Figure A.4 (Supplementary Materials Appendix A.6) shows how each feature contributes to the predicted probability of loneliness for a given observation based on SHAP values. Figure A.6 (see Supplementary Materials A.4) presents PDP (in the first column) and SHAP scatter plots (in the second columns) for the top five most important features. To summarise the contribution of these features across all samples, we present beeswarm plots of SHAP values in Figure A.5 (see Supplementary Materials Appendix A.6) for the entire population and each age cohort. These features were ordered based on descending feature importance, calculated as the mean absolute value of SHAP. Each dot for a specific feature on the plot represents an observation from the data, and its corresponding value on the x-axis indicates the magnitude and direction of its contribution to the predicted probability. The top five most influential features contributing to loneliness for the entire population were mental well-being (“mental”), having a life partner (“ptnr”), friendship fulfilment (“sful1”), social connectedness (“connect”) and spare time fulfilment (“sful2”).

Mental well-being emerged as the most predictive feature of loneliness for both the overall population and age cohorts. It contributed to a maximum increase of over 40% and up to a 20% reduction in the probability of loneliness (see Figure A.5a, Supplementary Materials Appendix A.6). A non-linear relationship was revealed by the PDP in Figure A.6a (see Supplementary Materials Appendix A.6), where a sharp reduction in loneliness was evident as the mental well-being score increased from 50, levelling off at 90. The average chance of experiencing loneliness was estimated be 14.7% [95% CI: 14.6%–14.8%] if the population’s mental well-being score were improved to 90, representing a halving compared to the prevalence. Adults with poor mental well-being and less social connection were at a particularly high risk, with average predicted probabilities of loneliness reaching over 69% (Figure A.7, Supplementary Materials Appendix A.6).

Except for the youth age group, social connectedness was consistently a crucial predictor of loneliness, with a stronger emphasis as life stages progress (see Figure A.5, Supplementary Materials Appendix A.6). A distinct downward trajectory is evident in the SHAP and PDP plots (see Figure A.6c and Figure A.6d, Supplementary Materials Appendix A.6), where a higher social connectedness index was associated with lower predicted probabilities of loneliness. More specifically, the chance of loneliness was predicted to be 37% [95% CI: 36.6%–37.3%] on average if the population’s social connectedness was low as 2 on the index. Assuming other features remain constant, the interaction between age and social connectedness in Figure A.8 (see Supplementary Materials Appendix A.6) reveals that among Australians who were less socially connected, individuals aged 45–75 generally experienced greater loneliness than others. Figure A.9 (see Supplementary Materials Appendix A.6) illustrates that if social connectedness and friendship fulfilment were improved jointly, loneliness could more effectively be reduced. Furthermore, adults with a social connectedness index in the highest 2.5th percentile were estimated to experience greater loneliness compared to some who were relatively less connected. This may be because those who were coping with loneliness were in the process of actively seeking social connections. Interestingly, we found that this group consists of older, single females who experienced the death of someone important in the last 12 months.

As shown in Figure A.5 (see Supplementary Materials A.4), distinct separation and clusters of SHAP values were observed between individuals with a life partner and those without, indicating a strong influence on loneliness. Notably, adults living in Australia without a life partner are more likely to experience loneliness, with the average probability as high as (37.7% 95% CI: 37.4%–38.0%), marking an 11-percentage-point increase compared to those with a life partner (26.6% [95% CI: 26.4%–27.1%]) (see Figure A.6e, Supplementary Materials Appendix A.6). Interestingly, across different life stages, having a life partner was more important among adults aged 31–40 and 76+ compared to others. Social fulfilment emerged as one of the top risk factors. Interestingly, friendship fulfilment played a more pronounced role among younger cohorts, while spare time fulfilment showed a stronger influence among older cohorts (see Figure A.5, Supplementary Materials Appendix A.6). Specifically, youth (aged 18–30) who strongly disagreed with the statement ‘I seem to have a lot of friends’ were predicted to be over two times lonelier compared to those who strongly agreed (46.5% [95% CI: 45.9%–47.2%]; 22.0% [95% CI: 21.4%–22.5%]). Youth who were less mentally well and not fulfilled in their friendships were at a high risk of loneliness, with predicted loneliness reaching as high as 78%. Conversely, this probability could be reduced to below 15% through the joint improvement of mental well-being and social fulfilment (see Figure A.10). For the elderly (aged 76+), those who were almost always not fulfilled in their spare time were predicted to have a (44.9% 95% CI: 43.9%–45.8%) chance of experiencing loneliness, reflecting a 13-percentage-point increase compared to the cohort prevalence. This prediction increased to more than 51% if they also did not feel part of the community (see Figure A.11, Supplementary Materials Appendix A.6).

In contrast to the micro level of social integration, community participation was not found to be as influential in predicting loneliness compared to other factors and hence was not selected in the final models. Results for other features, including experiencing the death of someone important, sex, employment and age, are provided in Supplementary Materials Appendix A.6. Table A.3 in Supplementary Materials Appendix A.7 presents key performance metrics of the fitted models, including accuracy, precision, recall, F1-score and the area under the curve (AUC) of the receiver operating characteristic. The performance of our model was acceptable in predicting loneliness with an out-of-sample AUC of 0.80 in the full data set and 0.77–0.84 across the different age groups.

Discussion

Our study employed predictive machine learning models to examine loneliness risk factors among adults living in Australia, highlighting the importance of social integration and mental health. Insights were provided for both the entire population and specific age groups. For the overall population, key factors were identified: mental well-being, having a life partner, social connectedness and social fulfilment – encompassing friendship and spare time fulfilment. Moreover, heterogeneity in loneliness was observed across different life stages, suggesting tailored approaches may be necessary. Specifically, among young adults, loneliness correlated more strongly with friendship fulfilment, satisfaction with financial situation and health, whereas among older adults, spare time fulfilment, community satisfaction and experiencing loss of loved ones were more influential. Recognising both the homogeneity and heterogeneity of loneliness is crucial for effective interventions. This finding of heterogeneity in drivers of loneliness reflects distinct social and emotional needs at different stages of life and is consistent with international studies. Loneliness in youth is often linked to identity formation and peer group dynamics. A longitudinal study in the Southeastern United States found that adolescents who perceived higher levels of cumulative support from family, peer and teacher relationships exhibit greater socioemotional functioning, sense of belonging and decreased feelings of loneliness (Cavanaugh and Buehler, Reference Cavanaugh and Buehler2016). Similarly, a study involving 14,077 adolescents from 156 schools in England from 2006 to 2014 found that loneliness in youth is associated with peer relationships and social inequality, with authors suggesting that comparison in terms of living conditions contributes to loneliness among young people (Qualter et al., Reference Qualter, Hennessey, Yang, Chester, Klemera and Brooks2021). They also found that loneliness becomes more intense among older adolescents, suggesting that loneliness emerging during adolescence is likely to be carried into early adulthood (Qualter et al., Reference Qualter, Hennessey, Yang, Chester, Klemera and Brooks2021). Our findings are also consistent with the literature on drivers of loneliness in seniors, which is primarily associated with adjustment to life transitions such as retirement and bereavement. A 28-year prospective study in Finland found that loss of a partner, reduced social engagement, increased physical disabilities and increased feelings of low mood were related to enhanced feelings of loneliness (Aartsen and Jylhä, Reference Aartsen and Jylhä2011).

Mental well-being consistently emerged as the most influential feature in our models, highlighting its significant relationship with loneliness across all populations. This finding aligns with existing research, which has shown a strong association between loneliness and mental health (Beutel et al., Reference Beutel, Klein and Brähler2017; Mansour et al., Reference Mansour, Greenwood, Biden, Francis, Olsson and Macdonald2021; Richard et al., Reference Richard, Rohrmann, Vandeleur, Schmid, Barth and Eichholzer2017). Despite its protective effect against loneliness, the effect of improvement in mental well-being does not follow a linear pattern, with slower progress observed in the lower range (i.e. less than 40). Mendelian randomisation analysis (Sbarra et al., Reference Sbarra, Ramadan and Choi2023) and prospective cohort studies (Nuyen et al., Reference Nuyen, Tuithof, de Graaf, Van Dorsselaer, Kleinjan and Have2020; Mann et al., Reference Mann, Wang and Pearce2022) indicate that the relationship between loneliness and mental health is bidirectional. Poor mental well-being may contribute to loneliness through social withdrawal and an unmet need for social support, while, conversely, loneliness may exacerbate existing mental health issues; however, the precise mechanism underlying observed associations between loneliness and mental health is likely to be complex and requires urgent clarification (largely via longitudinal studies and utilisation of appropriate statistical techniques).

Our findings regarding social integration are generally consistent with current research. For example, our models showed that people without a life partner experienced significantly greater loneliness (Steed et al., Reference Steed, Boldy, Grenade and Iredell2007; Beutel et al., Reference Beutel, Klein and Brähler2017; Botha and Bower, Reference Botha and Bower2023; Dahlberg et al., Reference Dahlberg, Andersson, McKee and Lennartsson2015). We further found that this companionship with a life partner is more pivotal among middle-aged adults and the elderly compared to others. Some studies have highlighted that individuals with frequent social connections and more friendships are less lonely (Botha and Bower, Reference Botha and Bower2023). Our model results also underscore the significant impact of social connection and social fulfilment on loneliness. Greater social connections with family, relatives, friends and neighbours, as well as fulfilling friendships and spare time activities, were found to be protective against loneliness. Furthermore, there would be more effective protection against loneliness when social connectedness and social fulfilment are improved jointly. While our analyses showed weaker associations between community participation and loneliness, macro factors may play an important indirect role in loneliness. By fostering civil society, investing in social infrastructure and ensuring robust social protections, the vulnerability to loneliness may be mitigated. Such measures also create fertile grounds for enhancing social integration through opportunities to establish, expand and nurture personal relationships and mental well-being (Swader and Moraru, Reference Swader and Moraru2022). Given the distinct life stage-related challenges of loneliness, interventions must be designed to address specific needs: enhancing social skills to enable successful reconnection to peers, family and school community for young people; promoting community engagement, social support and physical and financial accessibility for seniors; and providing mental health support across the lifecourse (Eccles and Qualter, Reference Eccles and Qualter2021; Katulis et al., Reference Katulis, Kaniušonytė and Laursen2023; Gustafsson et al., Reference Gustafsson, Berglund, Faronbi, Barenfeld and Ottenvall Hammar2017).

Efforts are already underway globally to address the loneliness epidemic. For instance, the US Surgeon General has outlined a framework emphasising the strengthening of social infrastructure and reducing disparities in social connection, with the aim of mitigating loneliness (Department of Health, Human Services, 2023; Murthy, Reference Murthy2020). In the UK, the Loneliness Commission was established to ensure that reducing loneliness remains an enduring parliamentary priority. They have also published the world’s first loneliness reduction strategy and created a Know Your Neighbourhood Fund to invest in empowering communities to alleviate chronic loneliness in disadvantaged areas and other initiatives (McDaid et al., Reference McDaid, Qualter and Arsenault2022; Department for Culture, Media and Sport, 2023). Intervention research is being undertaken in Australia demonstrating the promising effects of targeting the development and maintenance of social group memberships in improving mental health, well-being and social connectedness and reducing loneliness (Haslam et al., Reference Haslam, Cruwys, Haslam, Dingle and Chang2016; Haslam et al., Reference Haslam, Cruwys and Chang2019). However, despite research and advocacy highlighting the need for a systemic response, the Australian Government has yet to establish a national strategy (Gregory, Reference Gregory2024; Suicide Prevention Australia, 2022; Ending Loneliness Together, 2022).

Our findings underscore calls in the Australian context to develop targeted interventions to address loneliness. Assuming that mental ill-health is a cause of loneliness, population-based and health services interventions should focus on improving national mental health and social community connections while considering the heterogeneity across life stages. Population-based mental health initiatives could focus on delivering an appropriate balance of universal and indicated interventions (Skinner et al., Reference Skinner, Occhipinti, Song and Hickie2023). National mental health services initiatives could focus on increasing equitable and early access for young people to quality mental health care and enhancing technology-based coordination of care (Crosland et al., Reference Crosland, Ho and Hosseini2024). Creating supportive environments in workplaces and communities that prioritise mental well-being and promote community-based support networks and peer support groups is essential. To achieve these goals, collaboration between governments, businesses and community groups is important to ensure a coordinated and comprehensive approach to fostering social connections and mental health support.

Modelling has also shown good impacts from interventions focused on fostering social connectedness, from which people can build quality friendships, facilitate employment opportunities and provide mental guidance and counselling for those experiencing health issues (Occhipinti et al., Reference Occhipinti, Skinner and Iorfino2021; Occhipinti et al., Reference Occhipinti, Skinner and Carter2021). By implementing such initiatives, younger individuals may benefit from increased social support networks and enhanced well-being. On the other hand, interventions for loneliness among older adults could focus on facilitating regular social groups and events in the community, encouraging participation in community building and providing care and counselling services aimed at supporting those experiencing grief. However, these interventions require the allocation of resources to foster a more connected community. This includes building a supportive infrastructure that encourages investments in community facilities, mental health services, employment programmes and social support networks tailored to the diverse needs of different age groups. Investing in social capital infrastructure to foster social production (unpaid activities that contribute to civil society and strengthen the social fabric of communities) (Occhipinti et al., Reference Occhipinti, Buchanan and Hynes2023) could be a strategic approach to combating loneliness, particularly among older adults. This approach underlines the importance of social integration and could guide policy interventions that prioritise social cohesion and the creation of supportive environments conducive to mental health and interpersonal relationships. Additionally, initiatives aimed at reducing loneliness should be integrated into broader public health strategies to ensure sustained support and impact. By prioritising these efforts and investing in the necessary infrastructure, policymakers and communities can work together to create environments that promote social cohesion, mental well-being and overall resilience against loneliness among adults living in Australia.

A key limitation of this study is the simplification of the 7-point loneliness scale into a binary feature, focusing on the likelihood of “often feeling very lonely” rather than assessing the severity of loneliness. Furthermore, the measure of loneliness used in the HILDA survey (and our analyses) is direct, asking participants specifically about loneliness, and is therefore open to potentially significant social-desirability bias (Mund et al., Reference Mund, Maes, Drewke, Gutzeit, Jaki and Qualter2023). Another limitation is that our model predicts loneliness using data for the included features collected in the same study wave only so that our analyses are effectively cross-sectional, restricting our ability to infer causality and precluding the establishment of definitive cause-effect relationships.

Conclusion

In conclusion, our study highlights the complex interplay of various factors potentially contributing to loneliness among adults living in Australia across different age groups. Through the utilisation of predictive machine learning models, we identified common risk factors, including mental well-being, social connectedness, social fulfilment and having a life partner. Our findings contribute to the growing literature highlighting the importance of addressing loneliness as a multifaceted issue that requires targeted interventions tailored to the specific needs of different age groups. By recognising the heterogeneity of loneliness and prioritising efforts to foster social cohesion and support networks, policymakers and communities can work towards creating environments that promote mental well-being and resilience against loneliness among adults living in Australia.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/dep.2025.2.

Data availability statement

No original data were collected for this study. The data that support the findings of this study are available from the Department of Social Services (DSS) and Melbourne Institute of Applied Economic and Social Research, 2022, “The Household, Income and Labour Dynamics in Australia (HILDA) Survey, GENERAL RELEASE 21 (Waves 1–21),” https://doi.org/10.26193/KXNEBO. Analysis output data are available for non-commercial purposes upon request to the corresponding author.

Acknowledgements

This paper uses unit record data from the HILDA survey conducted by the Australian Government DSS. The findings and views reported in this paper, however, are those of the authors and should not be attributed to the Australian Government, DSS or any of DSS’s contractors or partners (DOI: 10.26193/KXNEBO).

Author contributions

Conceptualisation: IL, JO, AS. Formal analysis: IL, AS, MV. Writing – original draft: IL. Writing – review, editing and intellectual contributions: all authors.

Financial support

This research was supported by philanthropic funding from the Grace Fellowship, the Johnston Fellowship and other donor(s) that are families affected by mental illness who wish to remain anonymous.

Competing interests

IL, AS, MV and JZ declare they have no conflicts of interest relevant to this work. JO is both Head of Systems Modelling and Simulation and Co-Director of the Mental Wealth Initiative at the University of Sydney’s Brain and Mind Centre. She is also the Managing Director of Computer Simulation & Advanced Research Technologies and acts as advisor to the Brain Capital Alliance. IBH is the co-director of Health and Policy at the Brain and Mind Centre (BMC), University of Sydney. The BMC operates an early-intervention youth service at Camperdown under contract to Headspace. He is the chief scientific advisor to, and a 3.2% equity shareholder in, InnoWell Pty Ltd, which aims to transform mental health services through the use of innovative technologies.

Ethical standards

This paper uses unit record data from the HILDA survey conducted by the Melbourne Institute and funded by the Australian Government DSS. The HILDA survey data collection protocols and survey instruments have been approved by the University of Melbourne Human Ethics Committee. This study did not require ethical approval as the analysis used only secondary data from the HILDA survey.

References

Connections references

Hickie, IB, Occhipinti, J, Skinner, A, Ospina-Pinillos, L. (2024). Does declining social connection, and increased reported loneliness, explain the apparent increase in depressive and other mood disorders, particularly among younger people? Research Directions: Depression, 1, e6, https://doi.org/10.1017/dep.2023.17 Google Scholar

References

Occhipinti, J, Skinner, A, Doraiswamy, PM et al. (2024) The influence of economic policies on social environments and mental health. Bulletin of the World Health Organization 102, 323.Google Scholar
Murthy, V (2017) Work and the loneliness epidemic. Harvard Business Review 9, 37.Google Scholar
Cacioppo, JT and Cacioppo, S (2018) The growing problem of loneliness. The Lancet 391, 426.Google Scholar
Lim, MH, Badcock, J, Smith, B et al. (2020) Ending loneliness together in Australia. New South Wales, Australia: Ending Loneliness Together.Google Scholar
Australian Institute of Health, Welfare (2023) Social isolation and loneliness. https://www.aihw.gov.au/reports/australias-welfare/social-isolation-and-loneliness Google Scholar
Hawkley, LC and Cacioppo, JT (2010) Loneliness matters: a theoretical and empirical review of consequences and mechanisms. Annals of Behavioral Medicine 40, 218227.Google Scholar
Peplau, LA and Perlman, D (1982) Loneliness: a sourcebook of current theory, research, and therapy. (No Title).Google Scholar
Badcock, JC, Holt-Lunstad, J, Garcia, E, Bombaci, P and Lim, MH (2022) Position statement: Addressing social isolation and loneliness and the power of human connection. Global Initiative on Loneliness and Connection (GILC):1–43.Google Scholar
Wang, J, Mann, F, Lloyd-Evans, B, Ma, R and Johnson, S (2018) Associations between loneliness and perceived social support and outcomes of mental health problems: a systematic review. BMC Psychiatry 18, 116.Google Scholar
McClelland, H, Evans, JJ, Nowland, R, Ferguson, E and O’Connor, RC (2020) Loneliness as a predictor of suicidal ideation and behaviour: a systematic review and meta-analysis of prospective studies. J Affect Disord 274, 880896.Google Scholar
Stravynski, A and Boyer, R (2001) Loneliness in relation to suicide ideation and parasuicide: a population-wide study. Suicide Life Threat Behav 31, 3240.Google Scholar
Wilson, RS, Krueger, KR, Arnold, SE et al. (2007) Loneliness and risk of Alzheimer disease. Archives of General Psychiatry 64, 234240.Google Scholar
Salinas, J, Beiser, AS, Samra, JK et al. (2022) Association of loneliness with 10-year dementia risk and early markers of vulnerability for neurocognitive decline. Neurology 98, e1337e1348.Google Scholar
Hawkley, LC and Cacioppo, JT (2003) Loneliness and pathways to disease. Brain Behav Immun 17, 98105.Google Scholar
Valtorta, NK, Kanaan, M, Gilbody, S, Ronzi, S and Hanratty, B (2016) Loneliness and social isolation as risk factors for coronary heart disease and stroke: systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal observational studies. Heart 102, 10091016.Google Scholar
Holt-Lunstad, J, Smith, TB, Baker, M, Harris, T and Stephenson, D (2015) Loneliness and social isolation as risk factors for mortality: a meta-analytic review. Perspectives on Psychological Science 10, 227237.Google Scholar
Erzen, E and Çikrikci, Ö (2018) The effect of loneliness on depression: a meta-analysis. International Journal of Social Psychiatry 64, 427435.Google Scholar
Sbarra, DA, Ramadan, FA, Choi, KW et al. (2023) Loneliness and depression: bidirectional Mendelian randomization analyses using data from three large genome-wide association studies. Molecular Psychiatry 28, 45944601.Google Scholar
Victor, CR and Yang, K (2012) The prevalence of loneliness among adults: a case study of the United Kingdom. J Psychol 146, 85104.Google Scholar
Guthmuller, S (2022) Loneliness among older adults in Europe: the relative importance of early and later life conditions. PLoS One 17, e0267562.Google Scholar
Franssen, T, Stijnen, M, Hamers, F and Schneider, F (2020) Age differences in demographic, social and health-related factors associated with loneliness across the adult life span (19–65 years): a cross-sectional study in the Netherlands. BMC Public Health 20, 112.Google Scholar
Ejlskov, L, Wulff, J, Bøggild, H and Stafford, M (2018) Assessing the relative importance of correlates of loneliness in later life. Gaining insight using recursive partitioning. Aging Ment Health 22, 14861493.Google Scholar
von Soest, T, Luhmann, M, Hansen, T and Gerstorf, D (2020) Development of loneliness in midlife and old age: its nature and correlates. J Pers Soc Psychol 118, 388.Google Scholar
Hawkley, LC and Kocherginsky, M (2018) Transitions in loneliness among older adults: a 5-year follow-up in the National Social Life, Health, and Aging Project. Res Aging 40, 365387.Google Scholar
Dahlberg, L, McKee, KJ, Frank, A and Naseer, M (2022) A systematic review of longitudinal risk factors for loneliness in older adults. Aging Ment Health 26, 225249.Google Scholar
Nyqvist, F, Näsman, M, Hemberg, J and Nygård, M (2023) Risk factors for loneliness among older people in a Nordic regional context–a longitudinal study. Ageing Soc 43, 23722393.Google Scholar
Ogrin, R, Cyarto, E V, Harrington, KD et al. (2021) Loneliness in older age: what is it, why is it happening and what should we do about it in Australia?. Australas J Ageing 40, 202207.Google Scholar
Steed, L, Boldy, D, Grenade, L and Iredell, H (2007) The demographics of loneliness among older people in Perth, Western Australia. Australas J Ageing 26, 8186.Google Scholar
Engel, L, Lee, YY, Le, LKD, Lal, A and Mihalopoulos, C (2021) Reducing loneliness to prevent depression in older adults in Australia: A modelled cost-effectiveness analysis. Ment Health Prev 24, 200212.Google Scholar
Freak-Poli, R, Phyo, AZZ, Hu, J and Barker, SF (2022) Are social isolation, lack of social support or loneliness risk factors for cardiovascular disease in Australia and New Zealand? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Health Promotion Journal of Australia 33, 278315.Google Scholar
Moyle, W, Kellett, U, Ballantyne, A and Gracia, N (2011) Dementia and loneliness: an Australian perspective. J Clin Nurs 20, 14451453.Google Scholar
Bishop, GM, Llewellyn, G, Kavanagh, AM et al. (2024) Disability-related inequalities in the prevalence of loneliness across the lifespan: trends from Australia, 2003 to 2020. BMC Public Health 24, 621.Google Scholar
Watson, N and Wooden, M (2021) The household, income and labour dynamics in Australia (HILDA) survey. Jahrb Natl Okon Stat 241, 131141.Google Scholar
Department of Social Services Melbourne Institute: Applied Economic & Social Research (2023) The Household, Income and Labour Dynamics in Australia (HILDA) Survey, GENERAL RELEASE 21 (Waves 1–21). Carlton VIC: Melbourne Institute: Applied Economic & Social Research.Google Scholar
Dorogush, AV, Ershov, V and Gulin, A (2018) CatBoost: Gradient boosting with categorical features support. arXiv. arXiv preprint arXiv:181011363.Google Scholar
Prokhorenkova, L, Gusev, G, Vorobev, A, Dorogush, AV and Gulin, A (2018) CatBoost: unbiased boosting with categorical features. Adv Neural Inf Process Syst 31, 111.Google Scholar
Sahin, EK (2022) Comparative analysis of gradient boosting algorithms for landslide susceptibility mapping. Geocarto International 37, 24412465.Google Scholar
Bentéjac, C, Csörgő, A and Martínez-Muñoz, G (2021) A comparative analysis of gradient boosting algorithms. Artificial Intelligence Review 54, 19371967.Google Scholar
Zhang, Z, Zhao, Y, Canes, A, Steinberg, D and Lyashevska, O (2019) Predictive analytics with gradient boosting in clinical medicine. Annals of Translational Medicine 7, 152.Google Scholar
Lundberg, SM and Lee, SI (2017) A unified approach to interpreting model predictions. Adv Neural Inf Process Syst 30, 47684777.Google Scholar
Hastie, T, Tibshirani, R, Friedman, JH and Friedman, JH (2009) The Elements of Statistical Learning: Data Mining, Inference, and Prediction. New York City: Springer.Google Scholar
Cavanaugh, AM and Buehler, C (2016) Adolescent loneliness and social anxiety: the role of multiple sources of support. J Soc Pers Relat 33, 149170.Google Scholar
Qualter, P, Hennessey, A, Yang, K, Chester, KL, Klemera, E and Brooks, F (2021) Prevalence and social inequality in youth loneliness in the UK. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 18, 10420.Google Scholar
Aartsen, M and Jylhä, M (2011) Onset of loneliness in older adults: results of a 28 year prospective study. European Journal of Ageing 8, 3138.Google Scholar
Beutel, ME, Klein, EM, Brähler, E et al. (2017) Loneliness in the general population: prevalence, determinants and relations to mental health. BMC Psychiatry 17, 17.Google Scholar
Mansour, KA, Greenwood, CJ, Biden, EJ, Francis, LM, Olsson, CA and Macdonald, JA (2021) Pre-pandemic predictors of loneliness in adult men during COVID-19. Frontiers in Psychiatry 12, 775588.Google Scholar
Richard, A, Rohrmann, S, Vandeleur, CL, Schmid, M, Barth, J and Eichholzer, M (2017) Loneliness is adversely associated with physical and mental health and lifestyle factors: results from a Swiss national survey. PLoS One 12, e0181442.Google Scholar
Nuyen, J, Tuithof, M, de Graaf, R, Van Dorsselaer, S, Kleinjan, M and Have, M (2020) The bidirectional relationship between loneliness and common mental disorders in adults: findings from a longitudinal population-based cohort study. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 55, 12971310.Google Scholar
Mann, F, Wang, J, Pearce, E et al. (2022) Loneliness and the onset of new mental health problems in the general population. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 57, 21612178.Google Scholar
Botha, F and Bower, M (2023) Predictors of male loneliness across the lifespan: a longitudinal Australian study. PREPRINT. https://doi.org/10.21203/rs.3.rs-3511228/v1.Google Scholar
Dahlberg, L, Andersson, L, McKee, KJ and Lennartsson, C (2015) Predictors of loneliness among older women and men in Sweden: a national longitudinal study. Aging Ment Health 19, 409417.Google Scholar
Swader, CS and Moraru, AV (2022) Social infrastructure and the alleviation of loneliness in Europe. KZfSS Kölner Zeitschrift für Soziologie und Sozialpsychologie 75, 387414.Google Scholar
Eccles, AM and Qualter, P (2021) Alleviating loneliness in young people–a meta-analysis of interventions. Child Adolesc Ment Health 26, 1733.Google Scholar
Katulis, G, Kaniušonytė, G and Laursen, B (2023) Positive classroom climate buffers against increases in loneliness arising from shyness, rejection sensitivity and emotional reactivity. Frontiers in Psychiatry 14, 1081989.Google Scholar
Gustafsson, S, Berglund, H, Faronbi, J, Barenfeld, E and Ottenvall Hammar, I (2017) Minor positive effects of health-promoting senior meetings for older community-dwelling persons on loneliness, social network, and social support. Clinical Interventions in Aging 12, 18671877.Google Scholar
US Department of Health, Human Services et al. (2023) New Surgeon General Advisory Raises Alarm about the Devastating Impact of the Epidemic of Loneliness, Isolation in the United States.Google Scholar
Murthy, VH (2020) Together. New York, NY, USA: Harper Collins Publishers.Google Scholar
McDaid, D, Qualter, P, Arsenault, L et al. (2022) Tackling loneliness evidence review.Google Scholar
Department for Culture, Media and Sport (2023) Major Fund to tackle loneliness and boost volunteering in disadvantaged areas launched. https://www.gov.uk/government/news/major-fund-to-tackle-loneliness-and-boost-volunteering-in-disadvantaged-areas-launched Google Scholar
Haslam, C, Cruwys, T, Haslam, SA, Dingle, G and Chang, MXL (2016) Groups 4 health: evidence that a social-identity intervention that builds and strengthens social group membership improves mental health. J Affect Disord 194, 188195.Google Scholar
Haslam, C, Cruwys, T, Chang, MXL et al. (2019) GROUPS 4 HEALTH reduces loneliness and social anxiety in adults with psychological distress: Findings from a randomized controlled trial. J Consult Clin Psychol 87, 787.Google Scholar
Gregory, X (2024) Australians are tackling “huge” issue of loneliness amid calls for government to do more. https://www.abc.net.au/news/2024-02-12/loneliness-australia-federal-government-urged-to-do-more/103441076 Google Scholar
Ending Loneliness Together (2022) Social Recovery beyond COVID-19: A National Strategy to Alleviate Loneliness and Social Isolation. New South Wales, Australia: Ending Loneliness Together.Google Scholar
Skinner, A, Occhipinti, JA, Song, YJC and Hickie, IB (2023) Population-level effectiveness of alternative approaches to preventing mental disorders in adolescents and young adults. Scientific Reports 13, 19982.Google Scholar
Crosland, P, Ho, N, Hosseini, SH et al. (2024) Cost-effectiveness of system-level mental health strategies for young people in the Australian Capital Territory: a dynamic simulation modelling study. Lancet Psychiatry 11, 123133.Google Scholar
Occhipinti, J, Skinner, A, Iorfino, F et al. (2021) Reducing youth suicide: systems modelling and simulation to guide targeted investments across the determinants. BMC Medicine 19, 113.Google Scholar
Occhipinti, J, Skinner, A, Carter, S et al. (2021) Federal and state cooperation necessary but not sufficient for effective regional mental health systems: insights from systems modelling and simulation. Scientific Reports 11, 11209.Google Scholar
Occhipinti, J, Buchanan, J, Hynes, W et al. (2023) Estimating the mental wealth of nations: valuing social production and investment. Nature Mental Health 1, 247253.Google Scholar
Mund, M, Maes, M, Drewke, PM, Gutzeit, A, Jaki, I and Qualter, P (2023) Would the real loneliness please stand up? The validity of loneliness scores and the reliability of single-item scores. Assessment 30, 12261248.Google Scholar
Supplementary material: File

Li et al. supplementary material

Li et al. supplementary material
Download Li et al. supplementary material(File)
File 1.7 MB

Author comment: Understanding drivers of loneliness: Machine learning insights from the HILDA Survey — R0/PR1

Comments

No accompanying comment.

Review: Understanding drivers of loneliness: Machine learning insights from the HILDA Survey — R0/PR2

Comments

Thank you for the opportunity to review this manuscript, "Understanding drivers of loneliness: Machine learning insights from the HILDA Survey." The topic of loneliness is highly significant, given its rising prominence as a public health challenge and its associations with various mental and physical health outcomes. This study provides valuable insights into loneliness among adults living in Australia, leveraging machine learning to explore predictors and life-stage variations. The use of a large, longitudinal dataset and advanced analytical techniques is a strength of the paper, offering important contributions to the understanding of this complex issue.

I have a few suggestions that could further strengthen the manuscript, as detailed below.

1. Alignment with the Journal’s Scope

While this study focuses on loneliness, it does not explicitly address depression, which I understand to be a key focus of Research Directions: Depression. To align more closely with the journal's scope, the authors could discuss the relationship between loneliness and depression in greater detail. For example, integrating findings from the literature on how loneliness contributes to depression or vice versa would enhance relevance. Additionally, implications for interventions targeting depression through the lens of loneliness could be explored.

2. Methodological clarity

  • HILDA dataset selection: The manuscript uses HILDA data up to 2018 due to the availability of relevant measurements. However, it would be helpful to clarify whether more recent data were unavailable or excluded for other reasons (as my understanding is that some if not all of these factors were included in later datasets). Additionally, as HILDA is a longitudinal survey, please address whether merging datasets over multiple years could result in repeated entries for the same individuals and whether this could affect the analysis.

  • CatBoost predictions: The process by which the CatBoost model predicts loneliness could be more clearly explained. For example, how were the predictors selected, and how does the model account for the complexity of the relationships between variables?

3. Conceptual issues

  • Causality vs. correlation: The manuscript presents factors such as mental wellbeing and social connectedness as predictors of loneliness but does not sufficiently address the possibility of reverse causality. For instance, loneliness may impair mental wellbeing and hinder social connection. These dynamics should be acknowledged and explored in both the introduction and discussion sections. While the discussion suggests longitudinal analyses to clarify causality, it would be worth noting that HILDA is a longitudinal survey and why it was not used to support such analyses.

4. Implications and Discussion

  • Heterogeneity in loneliness: The manuscript emphasises heterogeneity across life stages but does not explore these differences in depth. For example, what distinguishes loneliness in youth from seniors beyond individual predictors? How might interventions address these specific challenges?

  • Policy and practice: The discussion section includes recommendations such as "increasing equitable access to mental health care," but it is not always clear how these suggestions stem from the study’s findings. Strengthening the link between results and recommendations would improve coherence and impact.

  • Novelty: While the paper confirms known predictors of loneliness (e.g., mental wellbeing, social connectedness), the authors could focus more on novel insights or practical applications. For instance, factors like geographic location, cultural diversity, and financial constraints could be examined to uncover more nuanced patterns.

6. Writing and presentation

  • Abstract: Consider rephrasing "Australian adults" to "adults living in Australia" to ensure inclusivity (particularly as people who are culturally diverse are often disproportionately represented in cohorts of people experiencing loneliness). Additionally, briefly mention comparative results, such as differences between those experiencing and not experiencing individual factors, and corresponding associations with loneliness across age groups.

  • Vagueness in language: Some statements, particularly in the Discussion section, could be more specific. For example, instead of "considering the heterogeneity of each cohort," describe the distinct needs or characteristics of young people and seniors.

  • Implications section: The section beginning with "The implications of our findings..." would benefit from tighter writing and a clearer structure. Linking statements directly to findings would aid in demonstrating how important each implication might be.

  • Incomplete sentences: Sentences such as "Create supportive environments in workplaces..." need to be completed for clarity.

  • Please provide a reference for the statement: "Economic prosperity often masks the profound social and emotional challenges individuals face."

Decision: Understanding drivers of loneliness: Machine learning insights from the HILDA Survey — R0/PR3

Comments

No accompanying comment.

Presentation

Overall score 4 out of 5
Is the article written in clear and proper English? (30%)
5 out of 5
Is the data presented in the most useful manner? (40%)
4 out of 5
Does the paper cite relevant and related articles appropriately? (30%)
4 out of 5

Context

Overall score 3 out of 5
Does the title suitably represent the article? (25%)
5 out of 5
Does the abstract correctly embody the content of the article? (25%)
4 out of 5
Does the introduction give appropriate context and indicate the relevance of the results to the question or hypothesis under consideration? (25%)
3 out of 5
Is the objective of the experiment clearly defined? (25%)
3 out of 5

Results

Overall score 4.6 out of 5
Is sufficient detail provided to allow replication of the study? (50%)
4 out of 5
Are the limitations of the experiment as well as the contributions of the results clearly outlined? (50%)
4 out of 5

Author comment: Understanding drivers of loneliness: Machine learning insights from the HILDA Survey — R1/PR4

Comments

No accompanying comment.

Decision: Understanding drivers of loneliness: Machine learning insights from the HILDA Survey — R1/PR5

Comments

No accompanying comment.