Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-g7gxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-08T08:08:32.316Z Has data issue: true hasContentIssue false

The policy basis of group sentiments

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 September 2024

Scott Clifford*
Affiliation:
Department of Political Science, Texas A&M University, College Station, TX, USA
Elizabeth Simas
Affiliation:
Department of Political Science, University of Houston, Houston, TX, USA
JeongKyu Suh
Affiliation:
Department of Political Science, University of Houston, Houston, TX, USA
*
Corresponding author: Scott Clifford; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Although influential models of public opinion hold that group sentiments play an important role in shaping political beliefs, they often assume that group attitudes stem from socialization and are thus exogenous to politics. We challenge this assumption, arguing that group attitudes may themselves be the consequence of political views. Across three survey experiments that each uses a unique social group–issue pair, we consistently demonstrate that attitudes toward groups are influenced by information about the groups' policy views. These findings persist even when accounting for potential partisan signaling. Altogether, these results show that group sentiments should not be regarded as wholly exogenous to policy concerns and suggest that the use of group-based heuristics can be consistent with instrumental models of public opinion.

Type
Research Note
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of EPS Academic Ltd

Group attitudes have long been described as central objects in political belief systems that shape and constrain political attitudes. Scholars have proposed a variety of group-centric models of public opinion (Miller et al., Reference Miller, Wlezien and Hildreth1991; Wlezien and Miller, Reference Wlezien and Miller1997; Green et al., Reference Green, Palmquist and Schickler2002; Kane et al., Reference Kane, Mason and Wronski2021) and offered a wealth of evidence that group attitudes shape issue attitudes. For example, there is evidence that views of Muslims influenced support for the War on Terror (Sides and Gross, Reference Sides and Gross2013), feelings toward African Americans affect support for welfare (Gilens, Reference Gilens1996), and that attitudes toward Latinos affect support for immigration (Ramirez and Peterson, Reference Ramirez and Peterson2020). Common in this literature is a tendency to assume, either explicitly or implicitly, that group attitudes “are acquired early in life and represent long-standing predispositions that are then capable of shifting political attitudes” (Elder and O'Brian, Reference Elder and O'Brian2022, 1409). Thus, in many common theoretical and statistical models, group attitudes are assumed to be exogenous to policy attitudes and other instrumental concerns.

We, however, challenge this notion. We argue that just as policy preferences shape opinions about individuals (Rogowski and Sutherland, Reference Rogowski and Sutherland2016; Webster and Abramowitz, Reference Webster and Abramowitz2017; Clifford, Reference Clifford2020; Orr and Huber, Reference Orr and Huber2020; Lelkes, Reference Lelkes2021; Goren, Reference Goren2022; Simas, Reference Simas2023), they should also drive perceptions of social groups. Across three experimental studies using three different groups and issues, we consistently show that information about a group's support for an issue significantly affects the favorability of that group. These results hold even when accounting for potential partisan signaling. So while group attitudes can play an important role in shaping opinions and behaviors, our work shows that the causal arrow also runs the other way. Thus, our findings challenge the idea that contemporary American politics is mere tribalism and suggest that emphasizing areas of issue agreement may be a fruitful path for ameliorating inter-group conflict and animosity.

1. Theory

Our basic argument is that people evaluate social groups, in part, based on the perception of shared political interests. For this to happen, citizens must (1) know where social groups tend to stand on issues; and (2) hold meaningful attitudes on those same issues. The first claim is crucial to the group-centrality literature itself, as that knowledge is necessary for group attitudes to affect political attitudes. For example, Elder and O'Brian (Reference Elder and O'Brian2022, 1422) state that “[m]any people are knowledgeable about the types of social groups that support or oppose policies; this knowledge has historically exceeded knowledge of where parties or ideological groups stand on those same issues.” And indeed, there is growing evidence of individuals' capabilities to accurately connect various groups to parties, policy preferences, and vote choices (Rothschild et al., Reference Rothschild, Howat, Shafranek and Busby2019; Orr and Huber, Reference Orr and Huber2020; Kane et al., Reference Kane, Mason and Wronski2021; Titelman and Lauderdale, Reference Titelman and Lauderdale2021). Thus, people do seem to hold the knowledge required for the reverse causal process.

The second premise, that people hold meaningful issue attitudes, is more contentious. Although many citizens lack ideologically constrained belief systems (Converse, Reference Converse and Apter1964; Kinder and Kalmoe, Reference Kinder and Kalmoe2017), they often hold strong and meaningful attitudes on specific issues. A wide range of evidence supports the existence of “issue publics,” or groups of respondents who care deeply about a particular issue (Krosnick, Reference Krosnick1990). Citizens may come to hold particularly strong views on an issue due to perceived self-interest (Boninger et al., Reference Boninger, Krosnick and Berent1995), or their moral beliefs and values (Ryan, Reference Ryan2014; Skitka and Morgan, Reference Skitka and Morgan2014). Moreover, some types of issues are particularly “easy” in that they require little knowledge or awareness to make a connection between an issue position and core values (Carmines and Stimson, Reference Carmines and Stimson1980; Johnston and Wronski, Reference Johnston and Wronski2015; Ryan and Ehlinger, Reference Ryan and Ehlinger2023). Thus, most people seem to hold some meaningful policy attitudes, even if they are not ideologically constrained.

Taken together, there is ample reason to expect that group attitudes may, in part, be shaped by the perception of shared political interests. To be sure, we are not the first to advance such a hypothesis. A number of recent studies have challenged the presumed causal role of group attitudes, primarily through the use of panel data. For example, partisanship may cause racial attitudes as much or more than the reverse process (Engelhardt, Reference Engelhardt2021). And although issue attitudes are often seen as a consequence of partisan identity (Barber and Pope, Reference Barber and Pope2019; Freeder et al., Reference Freeder, Lenz and Turney2019), views on culture war issues can cause partisan identity (Goren and Chapp, Reference Goren and Chapp2017). Finally, turning a classic finding on its head, Goren (Reference Goren2022) finds that views on welfare influence attitudes toward African Americans, a group that is strongly associated with the policy.

Extending this line of literature, we are the first to explicitly test how the favorability of a social group is influenced by whether an individual shares the policy views of that group. We do so across multiple social groups and political issues, demonstrating the broad applicability of our theory. Moreover, instead of using panel data, we use pre-registered experimental designs that allow us to isolate the causal effects of shared policy attitudes and show that they operate even when accounting for partisanship. This aspect of our work is especially important given debates about whether individuals actually care about policy attitudes or just the partisan identities they signal (Dias and Lelkes, Reference Dias and Lelkes2021; Orr and Huber Reference Orr and Huber2021; Orr et al., Reference Orr, Fowler and Huber2023).

2. Evidence from three studies

2.1 Design and measures

Between February 2022 and March 2023, we conducted three experimental studies among three different samples.Footnote 1 All three studies were approved by the institutional review board at the University of Houston. Studies 2 and 3 follow a pre-registration plan.Footnote 2 Each study featured a different social group and a different issue. Although it is extremely difficult to separately estimate the effects of identity and policy preferences (Fowler, Reference Fowler2020; Rogers, Reference Rogers2020; Orr and Huber Reference Orr and Huber2021; Orr et al., Reference Orr, Fowler and Huber2023), all our experimental groups are explicitly shown the partisanship of the group, while only the treatment groups receive the additional information that the group holds an opinion that runs counter to the party. Since the policy information conflicts with what should be inferred from the party label, this setup reduces problems with pre-treatment and addresses concerns that any effects of issue information are simply due to the fact that it is signaling partisan identity.

To avoid providing misinformation, we thus selected three groups (Vietnamese Americans, Catholics, and Mormons) for whom public opinion data show to have a policy preference (support for gun control, opposition to transgender rights, or support for environmental regulation) that conflicts with the party's stance. While chosen for this more pragmatic reason, these groups offer variation in the strength of their stereotypical partisan association, as Kane et al. (Reference Kane, Mason and Wronski2021) show that Mormons are clearly perceived as Republican, perceptions of Catholics are more mixed, and Vietnamese Americans actually counter the stereotypical association of Asians with the Democratic Party. Having this variation will allow us to speak to the generalizability of our results, and findings of consistent effects across all three groups should help allay potential concerns that the effects of issue information are contingent on the strength of the association between a social group and a party.

Each study follows the same basic structure. First, respondents reported their position on the focal issue used in the treatments (see Table 1 for wordings), followed by measures of attitude strength (studies 2 and 3 only), which make up the moderating variable. Respondents also reported feelings toward several social groups, including the target group,Footnote 3 and their partisan identification. These measures are used as pre-treatment control variables to increase the precision of our estimates (Clifford et al., Reference Clifford, Sheagley and Piston2021). In all three studies, respondents then completed unrelated content prior to the experiment.

Table 1. Summary of experimental designs

To introduce the experiment, respondents were told that researchers are interested in their opinions on a social group that plays an important role in politics. As noted above, respondents in both conditions were told about the partisan distribution of the focal group, while only those in the treatment condition received the additional information about the group's party-inconsistent position on the featured issue. Following the treatment, respondents completed several questions capturing their attitudes toward the target group, as well as two items serving as manipulation checks.

Even though our experiments are designed to minimize the potential for issue information to only operate by signaling partisan identity, this is again a concern we cannot completely eliminate (Dias and Lelkes, Reference Dias and Lelkes2021). Likewise, the issues themselves may evoke separate group sentiments (Conover, Reference Conover1988), and the partisan stereotypes of those groups may also influence responses. We further address these concerns by assessing manipulation checks (Dafoe et al., Reference Dafoe, Zhang and Caughey2018), and by estimating alternative models that account for partisan inferences.

2.2 Measures

In study 1, respondents reported their position on gun control on a five-point scale. In studies 2 and 3, respondents reported their issue position on a seven-point scale, then how important the issue is to them personally and their level of moral conviction on the issue (Skitka, Reference Skitka2010; Ryan Reference Ryan2014).Footnote 4 For these studies, following our pre-registration plan for study 2, we rescale the attitude position variable to range from −1 to 1, average the two measures of attitude strength, then multiply the attitude position measure by the attitude strength measure to construct a single measure of issue attitude (for a similar approach, see Taber and Lodge Reference Taber and Lodge2006).

To measure group attitudes, respondents were asked how well the social group shares their values (five-point scale), how close they feel to the group (Mason and Wronski, Reference Mason and Wronski2018) and their favorability of the group (seven-point scale). For our primary outcome, we follow our pre-registered plan and recode these three variables to range from 0 to 1, then averaged them (αs > 0.71). Finally, as a manipulation check, respondents were asked to estimate the percentage of the focal social group favoring the target policy and the breakdown of partisan identification among that group.

3. Results

Following the study 2 analysis plan, we predict the group attitudes index as a function of treatment assignment, issue attitudes, and an interaction between the two. Additionally, we control for pre-treatment measures of partisan identity and pre-treatment feelings toward the featured group. Figure 1 shows the marginal effects of the treatment as moderated by the issue attitude (full model results are available in the Appendix). As expected, in all three studies there is a significant interaction between the treatment and the respondent's issue attitude (p's < 0.004).

Figure 1. Marginal effects of group issue information by respondent issue attitudes. Main plots show the effects of the group issue stance treatments and the 95 percent confidence intervals for those estimates. Minor plots show the distribution of issue attitudes in our samples. See the Appendix for full model results.

Starting with the left-hand panel, among respondents who strongly favor stricter gun control laws, the treatment increases the favorability of Vietnamese Americans by 0.07 (p < 0.001), or about 0.35 standard deviations. Among those who strongly oppose stricter gun control laws, the treatment decreases the favorability of Vietnamese Americans by 0.08 (p < 0.001), or about 0.42 standard deviations. Moving to the middle panel, among respondents who oppose a transgender bathroom bill, the treatment decreases the favorability of Catholics by 0.08 (p = 0.001), or about 0.36 standard deviations. Among those who favor the bathroom bill, the treatment increased favorability by 0.05, or about 0.23 standard deviations (p = 0.026). Turning to the right-hand panel, effects are similarly strong at either end of the attitude scale. Among those who strongly favor (oppose) environmental regulation, the treatment increases (decreases) the favorability of Mormons by 0.12 (p < 0.003), or about 0.28 standard deviations. We find substantively similar results, though weaker in magnitude, when we examine only the favorability outcome (see the Appendix for details). Notably, all of these effects are substantially larger when accounting for non-compliance (Harden et al., Reference Harden, Sokhey and Runge2019; see the Appendix for details), which likely arises due to pre-treatment and satisficing.

An alternative explanation is that the treatments are affecting group attitudes largely because they are sending signals about the group's partisanship (Dias and Lelkes, Reference Dias and Lelkes2021).Footnote 5 We minimized this concern by design by providing information about group partisanship in all conditions. Manipulation checks available in the Appendix show that in studies 2 and 3, there was no evidence that the policy treatment moved perceptions of group partisanship (study 2: p = 0.72; study 3: p = 0.37). In study 1, however, the treatment shifted perceived support for the Republican Party by about four percentage points (p = 0.002).Footnote 6 So to address this possible confound, we re-estimate the initial models while also including an interaction between respondent partisanship and the treatment. If the treatment works primarily by affecting perceptions of group partisanship, then we should see a strong interaction between the treatment and partisanship, which eliminates the interaction between the treatment and issue attitudes.

The results of these models (see the Appendix) reveal some evidence of partisan signaling, though our core findings are unchanged. In both studies 1 and 2, the interaction between the treatment and partisanship is significant (p's < 0.029). In study 3, however, the interaction between the treatment and partisanship is both substantively and statistically indistinguishable from zero (b = 0.00, p = 0.849). Most importantly, in all three studies, the interactions between the treatment and issue attitudes remain statistically significant (p < 0.01) and show little change in magnitude. Our experimental design does not allow us to estimate the relative importance of party and policy cues, but the persistence of the effects of policy agreement support our claim that partisan signaling cannot explain away sizable effects of issue attitudes on group attitudes.

4. Discussion

Scholars have long acknowledged the role that group attachments and sentiments play in shaping political attitudes and identifications. We contribute to this line of research not by disputing the role of those sentiments, but by probing deeper into their roots. Across three samples and three different group–issue pairs, we consistently show that feelings about social groups are shaped by agreement with the policies those groups endorse. These findings show that the common assumption that group sentiments are exogenous to policy concerns needs to be reconsidered and suggest the implications of some prior studies should be revisited. For example, recent work showing a relationship between group affect and partisan identification argues that this connection “suggests that political decisions can often be made on the basis of liking or disliking groups, rather than purely rational self-interest” (Kane et al., Reference Kane, Mason and Wronski2021, 1784). But since our evidence suggests that like or dislike of those groups is at least partially due to shared political interests, it appears that the public may in fact be more rational than previously assumed.

We have focused on the USA, as group theory plays a prominent role in explaining shifts in party coalitions over time (Achen and Bartels, Reference Achen and Bartels2016), but we expect our findings to generalize beyond the USA. But, of course, our experiments are somewhat limited. Although we find consistent results across multiple groups and issues, future work should expand the design and further test (1) the conditions under which policy information may be more or less informative, (2) the relative importance of policy and non-policy factors, and (3) the generalizability across groups and issues. Still, our work makes an important contribution by highlighting the need to better explore the more instrumental component of group sentiments. Group attitudes surely play a causal role in politics, but as works on partisan intoxication show (Fowler, Reference Fowler2020; Rogers, Reference Rogers2020), identity and policy explanations should not be treated as mutually exclusive. As such, failure to adequately acknowledge the potential role of policy agreement can lead to mischaracterization of the nature of contemporary partisan politics in the USA. While any attempts to address affective polarization will undoubtedly be confronted with elements of pure “teamsmanship,” the possibility of appealing to common group interests does open up broader avenues for dealing with the negative consequences of the growing divide.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/psrm.2024.30. To obtain replication material for this article, https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/FYRSGM

Competing interests

None.

Footnotes

1 See the Appendix for sample demographics and information on efforts taken to ensure data quality.

2 The pre-registration for study 2 can be found in the Appendix and the link below. Due to an oversight, we did not pre-register study 3, but we follow the same pre-registered analysis plan from study 2. https://osf.io/ude28/?view_only=a9f3e5ea580040cfaad3f91d1fcd40da.

3 In study 1, respondents evaluated “Asian Americans” rather than “Vietnamese Americans.”

4 While there are often concerns that measuring moderators prior to an experiment can bias the results, a systematic test of this concern finds no evidence to support it (Clifford et al., Reference Clifford, Sheagley and Piston2021).

5 Likewise, there may be concerns that effects are driven by reactions to the stereotypical partisanship of the groups cued by the issues featured (gun owners, transgender individuals, or environmentalists).

6 As suggested by an anonymous reviewer, this effect may be linked to the fact that this was perhaps a more counter-stereotypical group, the inclusion of the word “however” in the treatment, or some combination of the two. But it may also be something unique to either Vietnamese Americans or the issue of the environment. And thus, we acknowledge this as an interesting finding, but leave it to future work to speak more directly to its potential causes.

References

Achen, C and Bartels, L (2016) Democracy for Realists: Why Elections Do Not Produce Responsive Government. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Barber, M and Pope, JC (2019) Does Party Trump ideology? Disentangling party and ideology in America. American Political Science Review 113(1), 3854.Google Scholar
Boninger, DS, Krosnick, JA and Berent, MK (1995) Origins of attitude importance: self-interest, social identification, and value relevance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 68(1), 6180.Google Scholar
Carmines, EG and Stimson, JA (1980) The two faces of issue voting. American Political Science Review 74(1), 7891.Google Scholar
Clifford, S (2020) Compassionate democrats and tough republicans: how ideology shapes partisan stereotypes. Political Behavior 42(4), 12691293.Google Scholar
Clifford, S, Sheagley, G and Piston, S (2021) Increasing precision without altering treatment effects: repeated measures designs in survey experiments. American Political Science Review 115(3), 10481065.Google Scholar
Conover, PJ (1988) The role of social groups in political thinking. British Journal of Political Science 18(1), 5176.Google Scholar
Converse, P (1964) The nature of belief systems in mass publics. In Apter, D (ed.), Ideology and Discontent. Gelncoe, IL: The Free Press, pp. 206261.Google Scholar
Dafoe, A, Zhang, B and Caughey, D (2018) Information equivalence in survey experiments. Political Analysis 26(4), 399416.Google Scholar
Dias, N and Lelkes, Y (2021) The nature of affective polarization: disentangling policy disagreement from partisan identity. American Journal of Political Science 66(3), 775790.Google Scholar
Elder, EM and O'Brian, NA (2022) Social groups as the source of political belief systems: fresh evidence on an old theory. American Political Science Review 116(4), 14071424.Google Scholar
Engelhardt, AM (2021) Observational equivalence in explaining attitude change: have white racial attitudes genuinely changed? American Journal of Political Science 67(2), 411425.Google Scholar
Fowler, A (2020) Partisan intoxication or policy voting? Quarterly Journal of Political Science 15(2), 141179.Google Scholar
Freeder, S, Lenz, GS and Turney, S (2019) The importance of knowing “what goes with what”: reinterpreting the evidence on policy attitude stability. Journal of Politics 81(1), 274290.Google Scholar
Gilens, M (1996) “Race coding” and white opposition to welfare. American Political Science Review 90(3), 593604.Google Scholar
Goren, P (2022) Pliable prejudice: the case of welfare. American Journal of Political Science 66(4), 961976.Google Scholar
Goren, P and Chapp, C (2017) Moral power: how public opinion on culture war issues shapes partisan predispositions and religious orientations. American Political Science Review 111(1), 110128.Google Scholar
Green, D, Palmquist, B and Schickler, E (2002) Partisan Hearts and Minds: Political Parties and Social Identities of Voters. New Haven: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Harden, JJ, Sokhey, AE and Runge, KL (2019) Accounting for noncompliance in survey experiments. Journal of Experimental Political Science 6(3), 199202.Google Scholar
Johnston, CD and Wronski, J (2015) Personality dispositions and political preferences across hard and easy issues. Political Psychology 36(1), 3553.Google Scholar
Kane, JV, Mason, L and Wronski, J (2021) Who's at the party? Group sentiments, knowledge, and partisan identity. Journal of Politics 83(4), 17831799.Google Scholar
Kinder, DR and Kalmoe, NP (2017) Neither Liberal or Conservative: Ideological Innocence in the American Public. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Krosnick, JA (1990) Government policy and citizen passion: a study of issue publics in contemporary America. Political Behavior 12(1), 5992.Google Scholar
Lelkes, Y (2021) Policy over party: comparing the effects of candidate ideology and party on affective polarization. Political Science Research and Methods 9(1), 189196.Google Scholar
Mason, L and Wronski, J (2018) One tribe to bind them all: how our social group attachments strengthen partisanship. Political Psychology 39(S1), 257277.Google Scholar
Miller, AH, Wlezien, C and Hildreth, A (1991) A reference group theory of partisan coalitions. The Journal of Politics 53(4), 11341149.Google Scholar
Orr, LV and Huber, GA (2020) The policy basis of measured partisan animosity in the United States. American Journal of Political Science 64(3), 569586.Google Scholar
Orr, LV and Huber, GA (2021) Measuring misperceptions: limits of party-specific stereotype reports. Public Opinion Quarterly 85(4), 10761091.Google Scholar
Orr, LV, Fowler, A and Huber, GA (2023) Is affective polarization driven by identity, loyalty, or substance? American Journal of Political Science 67(4), 948962.Google Scholar
Ramirez, MD and Peterson, DAM (2020) Ignored Racism: White Animus Toward Latinos. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Rogers, S (2020) Sobering up after “partisan intoxication or policy voting?”. Quarterly Journal of Political Science 15(2), 181212.Google Scholar
Rogowski, JC and Sutherland, JL (2016) How ideology fuels affective polarization. Political Behavior 38(2), 485508.Google Scholar
Rothschild, JE, Howat, AJ, Shafranek, RM and Busby, EC (2019) Pigeonholing partisans: stereotypes of party supporters and partisan polarization. Political Behavior 41(2), 423443.Google Scholar
Ryan, TJ (2014) Reconsidering moral issues in politics. The Journal of Politics 76(2), 118.Google Scholar
Ryan, TJ and Ehlinger, JA 2023. Issue Publics: How Electoral Constituencies Hide in Plain Sight. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sides, J and Gross, K (2013) Stereotypes of Muslims and support for the war on terror. Journal of Politics 75(3), 583598.Google Scholar
Simas, EN (2023) In Defense of Ideology: Reexaming the Role of Ideology in the American Electorate. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Skitka, LJ (2010) The psychology of moral conviction. Social and Personality Psychology Compass 4(4), 267281.Google Scholar
Skitka, LJ and Morgan, GS (2014) The social and political implications of moral conviction. Political Psychology 35(1), 95110.Google Scholar
Taber, CS and Lodge, M (2006) Motivated skepticism in the evaluation of political beliefs. American Journal of Political Science 50(3), 755769.Google Scholar
Titelman, N and Lauderdale, BE (2021) Can citizens guess how other citizens voted based on demographic characteristics? Political Science Research and Methods 11(2), 254274.Google Scholar
Webster, SW and Abramowitz, AI (2017) The ideological foundations of affective polarization in the U.S. electorate. American Politics Research 45(4), 621647.Google Scholar
Wlezien, C and Miller, AH (1997) Social groups and political judgments. Social Science Quarterly 78(3), 625640.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Summary of experimental designs

Figure 1

Figure 1. Marginal effects of group issue information by respondent issue attitudes. Main plots show the effects of the group issue stance treatments and the 95 percent confidence intervals for those estimates. Minor plots show the distribution of issue attitudes in our samples. See the Appendix for full model results.

Supplementary material: File

Clifford et al. supplementary material

Clifford et al. supplementary material
Download Clifford et al. supplementary material(File)
File 45.2 KB
Supplementary material: Link

Clifford et al. Dataset

Link