Introduction
Lewy body dementias (LBDs) include dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) and Parkinson's disease with dementia (PDD). Individuals with DLB and PDD share a common underlying alpha-synuclein neuropathology and display similar neuropsychological impairments, including attentional and visuoperceptual deficits (McKeith et al., Reference McKeith2005; Emre et al., Reference Emre2007; Lippa et al., Reference Lippa2007).
The treatment of attentional and cognitive impairments in LBD is typically limited to the use of cholinesterase inhibitors (McKeith et al., Reference McKeith2000; Emre et al., Reference Emre2004); however, due to the limited efficacy of these agents and their associated side effects, alternative treatments are urgently needed. One potentially promising non-pharmacological intervention is transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS), a simple, non-invasive, and inexpensive technique in which a weak electrical current is delivered to the brain through two electrodes soaked in saline or conductive gel. This approach has a modulatory effect upon cortical excitability, where these effects are polarity-dependent: anodal stimulation typically increases and cathodal stimulation typically decreases the underlying neural membrane potential (Stagg and Nitsche, Reference Stagg and Nitsche2011).
The application of tDCS to the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) has been shown to enhance cognitive functions including working memory, visuomotor co-ordination and decision-making in healthy individuals (Antal et al., Reference Antal, Nitsche, Kruse, Kincses, Hoffmann and Paulus2004; Fregni et al., Reference Fregni2005; Fecteau et al., Reference Fecteau, Knoch, Fregni, Sultani, Boggio and Pascual-Leone2007; Zaehle et al., Reference Zaehle, Sandmann, Thorne, Jancke and Herrmann2011). Importantly, beneficial cognitive effects following DLPFC stimulation have also been demonstrated in relevant clinical populations, including patients with Alzheimer's disease and Parkinson's disease (Elder and Taylor, Reference Elder and Taylor2014). Additionally, these positive effects may persist beyond the immediate period of stimulation. For example, Doruk and colleagues (Reference Doruk, Gray, Bravo, Pascual-Leone and Fregni2014) recently demonstrated that in patients with Parkinson's disease, ten consecutive sessions of anodal tDCS delivered to the left or right DLPFC led to short-term and longer-term (measured one month post-stimulation) improvements in executive functioning, compared with placebo stimulation.
As attentional and executive function deficits are observed within the LBDs, this is suggestive of frontal dysfunction (Johns et al., Reference Johns2009). This is supported by neuropsychological studies which have observed impairments in frontally dependent attentional and executive tasks in LBD (Ballard et al., Reference Ballard2001; Calderon et al., Reference Calderon, Perry, Erzinclioglu, Berrios, Dening and Hodges2001; Aarsland et al., Reference Aarsland, Litvan, Salmon, Galasko, Wentzel-Larsen and Larsen2003); pathologically, Lewy bodies are commonly found in the anterior frontal and temporal regions (Yoshimura, Reference Yoshimura1983; Kuzuhara and Yoshimura, Reference Kuzuhara and Yoshimura1993), and in addition, neuroimaging studies have shown that frontal atrophy is a feature of LBD (Burton et al., Reference Burton2002, Reference Burton, McKeith, Burn, Williams and O’Brien2004). Thus, there is a broad context to suggest that there may be a reduced top-down influence of the frontal cortex in LBD. Previous studies have observed beneficial effects of DLPFC stimulation upon attention and executive functioning (Fecteau et al., Reference Fecteau, Knoch, Fregni, Sultani, Boggio and Pascual-Leone2007; Gladwin et al., Reference Gladwin, den Uyl, Fregni and Wiers2012), and the left DLPFC has been implicated in top-down cognitive control (MacDonald et al., Reference MacDonald, Cohen, Stenger and Carter2000). Given this empirical and theoretical basis, we therefore hypothesized that the application of anodal tDCS to the DLPFC might benefit attentional and executive function in individuals with LBD. However, to date, the feasibility and tolerability, or the potential benefits of tDCS upon attentional and executive function, have not been examined in an LBD patient group.
Therefore, the primary aim of this preliminary study was to assess the feasibility of using tDCS in individuals with LBD, and to investigate the effects of tDCS upon attentional function in LBD.
Methods
Participants
Sixteen participants who met diagnostic criteria for either probable DLB or PDD (McKeith et al., Reference McKeith2005; Emre et al., Reference Emre2007), as assessed by two experienced clinicians (J.P.T. and H.K.), were recruited from the movement disorders service at the Institute of Neurosciences, Kolkata, India. The diagnosis was verified by detailed physical, neurological, and neuropsychiatric examinations, including hematology and biochemistry screening, thyroid function, syphilis serology, vitamin B12 and folate level tests, and chest X-rays. Structural imaging (magnetic resonance imaging or computed tomography scans) were performed on six of the 16 participants, when clinically suggested, in order to exclude other neurodegenerative etiologies.
Participants were included if they were ≥60 years of age; had a Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE; Folstein et al., Reference Folstein, Folstein and McHugh1975) score of ≥16; and were stable on anti-Parkinsonian medication or cholinesterase inhibitors for a period of one month prior to participation. Exclusion criteria included skin allergies; a history of excess alcohol intake; concurrent major psychiatric illness; significant/severe physical illness or comorbidities; other neurological disorders; current/previous visual impairment due to glaucoma, cataract, or macular degeneration; metallic or electronic implants; and significant motor fluctuations. All participants provided written informed consent and the study was approved by the local ethics committee.
Measures
Participants completed several clinical measures of cognitive and motor function prior to stimulation in order to provide information regarding disease severity. These included the MMSE (Folstein et al., Reference Folstein, Folstein and McHugh1975), the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA; Nasreddine et al., Reference Nasreddine2005) and Part III of the Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS-III; Goetz et al., Reference Goetz2008).
Medication use
Information regarding the use of cholinesterase inhibitors, anti-depressant and anti-Parkinsonian medication was also obtained. The anti-Parkinsonian medication equivalent levodopa dose was calculated, using levodopa conversion formulae reported elsewhere (Tomlinson et al., Reference Tomlinson, Stowe, Patel, Rick, Gray and Clarke2010), and is expressed in milligrams (mg).
Procedure
Participants completed one brief practice session of the attentional and visuoperceptual tasks, consisting of five practice trials per task. Participants then completed one full run of the computer tasks (lasting for approximately 10 min) immediately prior to stimulation, and repeated the tasks 10 min post-stimulation. Participants were tested in their “on” state with regard to their Parkinsonism; this was verified by ensuring that dopaminergic medication had been administered in the previous 2 h and by assessment of motor function immediately prior to stimulation.
Three attentional (simple reaction time (SRT); choice reaction time (CRT); digit vigilance (DV)); and two forced-choice visuoperceptual (angle and motion perception) tasks were used; these tasks have previously been demonstrated to be differentially affected in LBD patients compared with controls and other dementia groups (Wesnes et al., Reference Wesnes2002; Mosimann et al., Reference Mosimann, Mather, Wesnes, O’Brien, Burn and McKeith2004). These visuoperceptual tasks are not dependent upon prefrontal regions (Wood et al., Reference Wood, Firbank, Mosimann, Taylor and O’Brien2011) and performance was therefore hypothesized to be unlikely to be affected by DLPFC stimulation. Full details of the tasks are described elsewhere (Mosimann et al., Reference Mosimann, Mather, Wesnes, O’Brien, Burn and McKeith2004; Wood et al., Reference Wood, Firbank, Mosimann, Taylor and O’Brien2011; Reference Wood2013) and in Figure 1. Tasks were presented on a laptop PC and responses were recorded using response button boxes, which were held either in the dominant hand or in both hands, depending on the task.
Transcranial direct current stimulation
A single session of stimulation (2.8 mA) was delivered for 20 min using an Eldith DC Stimulator (Magstim, Whitland, UK), through two 35-cm2 electrodes in 0.9% saline-soaked bags (equivalent current density: 0.08 mA/cm2). This level of current density has been previously used, with no reported adverse effects, in other dementia cohort studies (Huey et al., Reference Huey2007; Cotelli et al., Reference Cotelli2014). The anodal electrode was placed over the left DLPFC (50% between F3 and FP1) using the International 10–20 system (Jasper, Reference Jasper1958). The cathodal electrode was placed on the right deltoid muscle. All participants received active stimulation. After the stimulation session, participants were asked to provide feedback regarding their experience of the stimulation; similarly, patients and their families were contacted 24 h post-stimulation and were asked whether any residual side effects were present.
Statistical analysis
Attentional and visuoperceptual task performances were compared before and after stimulation using paired t-tests. Attentional (SRT, CRT, and DV) task outcome measures included the percentage of correct answers and the mean reaction time (RT) to correct answers. Visuoperceptual outcome measures included the percentage of correct answers and the task difficulty, expressed either as degree values (angle task, where a lower degree value indicates better performance) or relative speed values (cars task, where a lower relative speed indicates a better performance). Outliers (≥2 SD) were removed from each RT task. As the aim of the current preliminary study was to assess the feasibility and tolerability of tDCS in LBD participants, analyses were not adjusted for multiple comparisons. Effect sizes are reported as Cohen's dz .
Results
Three participants were excluded from the study prior to stimulation, due to a poor understanding of task instructions. This resulted in a final sample of 13 participants (8 PDD; 5 DLB; Mean, Mage = 64.77 years, SDage = 7.66 years). Demographic details are included in Table 1. All participants tolerated stimulation and did not report any side effects, other than a brief tingling sensation underneath the electrodes, during or immediately after stimulation. No adverse events were reported.
Notes: DLB: Dementia with Lewy bodies; PDD: Parkinson's disease with dementia; MMSE: Mini-Mental State Examination; MoCA: Montreal Cognitive Assessment; UPDRS-III: Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale Motor Subscale.
aUPDRS-III data missing for one participant.
The percentage of CRT correct answers significantly increased following stimulation (t(12) = −2.98, p = 0.01, dz = 0.83). DV improvements were also shown, where the mean RT to correct answers (t(11) = 2.77, p = 0.02, dz = 0.80) was significantly reduced following stimulation. Eleven participants (84.6%) showed a positive CRT task response, displaying an increased percentage of correct answers following stimulation, and nine participants (69.2%) showed a positive DV task response, with a reduced mean RT to correct answers following stimulation. No other significant post-stimulation differences were shown in the remaining attentional or visuoperceptual tasks (all p-values > 0.05). The results are summarized in Table 2.
Notes: SRT: simple reaction time; CRT: choice reaction time; DVT: digit vigilance; ms: milliseconds; RT: reaction time.
a n = 12.
bLower degree value indicates better performance.
cLower relative speed indicates better performance.
*p < 0.05.
Discussion
This preliminary study indicates that the use of tDCS, with a current density of 0.08 mA/cm2, is both feasible and well-tolerated in a group of individuals with LBD, as no residual post-stimulation side effects were reported by participants or their families. This study also demonstrated that a single 20-min session of tDCS, applied to the left DLPFC, led to post-stimulation improvements to some aspects of attentional, but not visuoperceptual, function in LBD patients. In particular, there were improvements in CRT accuracy and in DV reaction times, although there was also a broader tendency for post-stimulation improvements in all other CRT and DV attentional measures. This differential benefit is likely to be due to the placement of electrode over the DLPFC rather than parietal or visual areas, since previous studies have shown that stimulation delivered to the DLPFC enhances working memory and decision-making (Fregni et al., Reference Fregni2005; Fecteau et al., Reference Fecteau, Knoch, Fregni, Sultani, Boggio and Pascual-Leone2007), and in the case of left DLPFC stimulation, can enhance attentional function (Gladwin et al., Reference Gladwin, den Uyl, Fregni and Wiers2012). Speculatively, our data suggest that as the left DLPFC has a suggested role in top-down cognitive control (MacDonald et al., Reference MacDonald, Cohen, Stenger and Carter2000), anodal stimulation, which can modulate cortical excitability by increasing the membrane potential (Stagg and Nitsche, Reference Stagg and Nitsche2011), might help improve the degree of top-down influence of the left DLPFC in LBD.
Whilst not placebo-controlled, the results of the present study may also support the notion that visuoperceptual performance in LBD is less dependent on fronto-executive function; posterior cortical function may be more relevant for these tasks and this would be consistent with the parieto-occipital deficits apparent in LBD (Colloby et al., Reference Colloby2002). It is likely that task-specific electrode montages will be needed to improve visuoperceptual function in LBD; the application of anodal stimulation to right parietal areas has been shown to result in visuoperceptual improvements within healthy individuals (Bolognini et al., Reference Bolognini, Fregni, Casati, Olgiati and Vallar2010). Therefore, the efficacy of anodal stimulation to parietal areas should be tested within LBD.
Limitations of the current study include the absence of a sham condition and the lack of correction for multiple comparisons in our analyses. However, by its nature, this study is exploratory in terms of highlighting potential benefits, determining tolerability, and helping to inform stimulation parameters; the details of which are currently lacking in LBD and can be determined by studies with small sample sizes and studies that are open-label in design (Elder and Taylor, Reference Elder and Taylor2014). Although the present study assessed a small sample of patients, a current density of 0.08 mA/cm2, delivered for a period of 20 min, was shown to be both acceptable and tolerable in LBD patients. Whilst this level of current density has previously been used in other dementia cohort studies (Huey et al., Reference Huey2007; Cotelli et al., Reference Cotelli2014), this has not been the case in LBD, and importantly, these stimulation parameters could be used in future placebo-controlled studies in LBD.
While post-stimulation performance improvements were not observed across all task measures, the influence of training effects cannot be excluded from the present study. For this reason, placebo-controlled trials are necessary to confirm these results. However, the present study contributes important pilot data in LBD, as it has been recommended that the results of all studies, including those which are negative, are reported in order to advance the clinical utility of tDCS and to allow the inclusion of eligible studies in future meta-analyses (Elder and Taylor, Reference Elder and Taylor2014).
Finally, it was not possible to examine whether stimulation differentially benefits DLB or PDD patients in the present study; certainly whilst there is significant overlap neuropathologically and clinically between both conditions (McKeith et al., Reference McKeith2005; Emre et al., Reference Emre2007; Lippa et al., Reference Lippa2007), differences between the two groups have been suggested (Park et al., Reference Park, Kim, Cheon, Cha, Kim and Kim2011). Nevertheless, these preliminary data suggest that tDCS may benefit both patient populations, thus supporting large-scale trials in both patient groups.
In conclusion, this study indicates that tDCS is feasible and is well tolerated in an LBD population, adding to the current lack of data in this patient group. This study also provides tentative evidence to suggest that the delivery of tDCS to the left DLPFC might benefit attentional function in LBD. However, larger placebo-controlled trials with repeated stimulation sessions are needed, and are now justified, in an LBD population to confirm this; promising data in PD have suggested that tDCS can offer sustained benefits upon executive functioning (Doruk et al., Reference Doruk, Gray, Bravo, Pascual-Leone and Fregni2014) and it would be relevant to explore if these effects are carried over into LBD.
Conflict of interest
None.
Description of authors’ roles
G. J. Elder conceived and designed the study, analyzed and interpreted the data, and drafted the manuscript. M. J. Firbank analyzed and interpreted the data, and revised the manuscript. H. Kumar, P. Chatterjee, T. Chakraborty, and A. Dutt conceived and designed the study, assisted with the recruitment of participants, and revised the manuscript. J-P Taylor conceived and designed the study, analyzed and interpreted the data and revised the manuscript. All authors have approved the final version of the manuscript.
Acknowledgments
The research was supported by an Intermediate Clinical Fellowship to Dr. J-P Taylor (WT088441 MA) and the National Institute of Health Research (NIHR) Newcastle Biomedical Research Unit based at Newcastle upon Tyne Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust and Newcastle University. The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR, or the Department of Health. The funders had no role in the study design, in the collection, analysis, and interpretation of data, in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to submit the manuscript for publication.