Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-r5fsc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-27T11:11:16.182Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Impact of musical training in specialised centres on learning strategies, auditory discrimination and working memory in adolescents

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 July 2023

Carmen María Sepúlveda-Durán
Affiliation:
Department of Education, Universidad de Córdoba, C/ San Alberto Magno, s/n, 14071 Córdoba, Spain
Pilar Martín-Lobo
Affiliation:
Department of Educational Psychology, Universidad Internacional de La Rioja, Avenida de la Paz, nº 137, 26006, Logroño, La Rioja, Spain
Sandra Santiago-Ramajo*
Affiliation:
Department of Educational Psychology, Universidad Internacional de La Rioja, Avenida de la Paz, nº 137, 26006 Logroño, La Rioja, Spain
*
Corresponding author: Sandra Santiago-Ramajo; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The aim of this study was to analyse whether specialised musical training influences auditory discrimination, working memory, learning strategies and academic performance. Sixty students (30 with at least four years of musical training and 30 without) of the same socioeconomic level were compared. Significant differences were found between students with and without musical training in terms of learning strategies, working memory and academic performance (p= <.05). This study shows the benefits of musical training offered at specialised centres, the development of students’ cognitive skills and the contributions of neuroscience to improving professional practice.

Type
Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Music is the art of combining and organising sounds to achieve a harmonious combination of frequencies and, therefore, a pleasant melody, which positively influences physiological and behavioural states (Panteleeva et al., Reference PANTELEEVA2017). Research in this field shows that the neuropsychological processes of music also affect the cognitive field (Bermell, Reference BERMELL2004), increase attention and concentration for learning, facilitate neurorehabilitation (Jauset-Berrocal & Soria-Urios, Reference JAUSET-BERROCAL and SORIA-URIOS2018) and verbal, spatial and general cognitive abilities (Hannon & Trainor, Reference HANNON and TRAINOR2007; Patel & Iversen, Reference PATEL and IVERSEN2007). In this area, neuroscientific and educational psychology studies demonstrate the repercussions of musical activity on musicians in terms of improving brain plasticity by comparing musicians’ functional and structural brain plasticity to that of non-musicians (Moreno et al., Reference MORENO, MARQUES, SANTOS, SANTOS, CASTRO and BESSON2009; Schlaug et al., Reference SCHLAUG, NORTON, OVERY and WINNER2005); child and youth musicians also exhibit more advanced information processing (Stoesz et al., Reference STOESZ, JAKOBSON, KILGOUR and LEWYCKY2007) and intellectual, social and emotional development (Campayo-Muñoz & Cabedo-Mas, Reference CAMPAYO-MUÑOZ and CABEDO-MAS2017; Hallam, Reference HALLAM2010). In addition, recent studies have shown the extent to which neuroscience contributes to music development by providing strong arguments in favour of musical training (Collins, Reference COLLINS2013; Curtis & Fallin, Reference CURTIS and FALLIN2014; Peñalba, Reference PEÑALBA2017; Peterson, Reference PETERSON2011).

These findings, and others described in the following sections, served as a starting point for the present study on the analysis of musical activity, its brain processes and relationships to variables of auditory discrimination, working memory, learning strategies and academic performance.

Auditory discrimination

Neuroimaging studies show that listening to music involves different cortical and subcortical brain regions showing that music can facilitate the development of the neuropsychological and cognitive skills required for student learning (Gray et al., Reference GRAY, CRITCHLEY and KOELSCH2014; Kay et al., Reference KAY2012; Koelsch & Skouras, Reference KOELSCH and SKOURAS2014).

One of these skills is auditory discrimination, which is used in the reception of sound, in understanding music and in the reproduction and creation of music, which requires activation of the auditory cortex of the temporal lobe of the brain, the supplementary motor area, the frontal gyrus and the brain areas related to memory for its recognition (Peretz et al., Reference PERETZ2009). The cerebral cortical organisation of music is analogous to the process illustrated by the speech processing model of Hickok and Poeppel (Reference HICKOK and POEPPEL2007); on the one hand, there is a network (the ventral network) that links phonological sensory networks to conceptual representations while another network (the dorsal network) links phonological sensory networks to the motor areas responsible for the articulation of speech. These findings illustrate the relationship between auditory discrimination and language. In addition, phonological skills are necessary for decoding during reading (Rasinski et al., Reference RASINSKI, BLACHOWICZ and LEMS2012; Resnick & Weaver, Reference RESNICK and WEAVER2013). To this end, grapheme-phoneme coding is performed by transforming graphic signs into sounds. Visual word recognition skills, sound identification, auditory perception, phonetic awareness and language are involved in these processes (Cuetos, Reference CUETOS2012; Martin-Lobo, Reference MARTIN-LOBO2010).

On the other hand, auditory discrimination is necessary to recognising voices and listening to and processing oral information. Furthermore, this feature improves the understanding of speech sounds and their symbolic representation. In fact, strong connections between music and language have been identified, especially in relation to evolutionary background (Perlovsky, Reference PERLOVSKY2012), brain connectivity (Koelsch et al., Reference KOELSCH, GUNTER, WITTFOTH and SAMMLER2005) and skill transfer (Besson et al., Reference BESSON, CHOBERT and MARIE2011). Some studies have highlighted the connection between music and language for communicative uses (Hallam et al., Reference HALLAM, CROSS and THAUT2009).

Along the same lines, neuroimaging studies have shown similarities and overlaps in syntactic processing between music and language (Jentschke & Koelsch, Reference JENTSCHKE and KOELSCH2009; Jentschke et al., Reference JENTSCHKE, KOELSCH, SALLAT and FRIEDERICI2008). Children learn the syntactic rules of language in their mother tongue from an early age and develop a detailed understanding of musical syntax (Jentschke et al., Reference JENTSCHKE, KOELSCH, SALLAT and FRIEDERICI2008; Jentschke & Koelsch, Reference JENTSCHKE and KOELSCH2009; Koelsch, Reference KOELSCH2013; Lamont, Reference LAMONT, Hallam, Cross and Taut2016; Patel, Reference PATEL2008).

Music and language capacities share the same brain processing mechanisms (Fiveash & Pammer, Reference FIVEASH and PAMMER2014). Listening to a song activates the auditory cortex for acoustic analysis and lyric recognition, activating areas related to the music lexicon, where we store information (Peretz et al., Reference PERETZ2009). Primary, secondary and frontal auditory areas are also involved in pitch (Peretz et al., Reference PERETZ2009); the cerebellum, basal ganglia, premotor cortex and supplementary area are involved in rhythm development (Drayna et al., Reference DRAYNA2001; Zatorre, Reference ZATORRE2001). These brain areas are also involved in the processes of reading and writing, language learning and academic learning (Schellenberg et al., Reference SCHELLENBERG, NAKATA, HUNTER and TAMOTO2007).

Other studies demonstrate the benefits of practising music for learning other languages. For example, better pronunciation, speech production and language levels in a second language have been found in students with musical aptitude (Kraus & Chandrasekaran, Reference KRAUS and CHANDRASEKARAN2010; Milovanov et al., Reference MILOVANOV, PIETILÄ, TERVANIEMI and ESQUEF2010; Schellenberg et al., Reference SCHELLENBERG, NAKATA, HUNTER and TAMOTO2007; Stegemööller et al., Reference STEGEMÖÖLLER2008; Toscano-Fuentes, Reference TOSCANO-FUENTES2011). Recently, there have also been consistent findings of a relationship between musicality and second language phonological competence according to the accent faking paradigm (Coumel et al., Reference COUMEL, CHRISTINER and REITERER2019).

In short, the findings of the above studies and of neuropsychologically based research suggest that music can promote the development of not only musical skills but also the hearing, auditory discrimination, language and cognitive skills necessary for learning.

Working memory

One definition of working memory defines it as the cognitive system that allows access to a limited amount of information to be retained in the service of processing complex information (Shipstead et al., Reference SHIPSTEAD, LINDSEY, MARSHALL and ENGLE2014). Working memory enables relevant goals and memories to be addressed and is associated with performance in different aspects of cognition, such as multitasking (Hambrick et al., Reference HAMBRICK, OSWALD, DAROWSKI, RENCH and BROU2010). In the case of music, scientific literature has found a relationship between musical training and working memory, for example, from differences between musicians and non-musicians in how they perform tasks corresponding to the phonological loop (Fiveash & Pammer, Reference FIVEASH and PAMMER2014).

Regarding the musical activity of singing, neuroimaging studies have found that different brain areas are involved in the recognition of familiar melodies such as the upper right and left temporal sulcus, planum temporale, the supplementary motor area and left infero-frontal gyrus (Peretz et al., Reference PERETZ2009); moreover, bilateral motor areas are involved in the musical interpretation of rhythm coordination and organisation, spatial organisation and movement sequencing, together with auditory and premotor areas of the right hemisphere (Zatorre et al., Reference ZATORRE, CHEN and PENHUNE2007).

Learning strategies and academic performance

Bermell (Reference BERMELL2004) found that the neuropsychological processes of music learning affect attention and concentration through their didactics; such processes also have an impact on general cognitive skills and on social and emotional skills (Hannon & Trainor, Reference HANNON and TRAINOR2007; Patel & Iversen, Reference PATEL and IVERSEN2007). For example, musical training enhances language processing due to the overlapping of the brain regions that process language and music (Patel, Reference PATEL2010, Reference PATEL2012). Furthermore, it has been proven that learning strategies of acquisition, codification, recovery and support are related to performance and have an impact on academic performance (Román & Gallego, Reference ROMÁN and GALLEGO1994). In line with these studies, the present study focuses on analysing a potential relationship between musical training and the development of these learning strategies and academic performance.

In Spain, musical education can be studied in two ways:

  • In primary and secondary school: students receive one hour weekly of music education, covering general music concepts for cultural literacy. In some centres, they go deeper into learning the recorder, Orff percussion instruments and other newer ones such as the ukulele or boomwhackers.

  • In conservatory or specialised music centres: students receive specific musical training as an extracurricular activity. In both, musical language and the study of a specific musical instrument (piano, guitar, violin, etc.) are studied. The Conservatory has a regulated educational structure, with qualified teachers. The Conservatory studies in Spain have a total duration of fourteen years divided into three stages: four years of basic training, six years of professional training and four years of higher education. Higher education is equivalent to a University Degree in the European Union. All these stages are independent of school education, not being compulsory studies.

From the results of the studies discussed above, we ask the following: Do children with musical training in specialised centres have better auditory discrimination, working memory, learning strategies and academic performance than children without musical training? The present study expands the literature in this area of research, enriches it with contributions from neuroscience and proposes improvements for professional practice and academic performance

This study will evaluate different neuropsychological variables, including auditory discrimination, working memory and learning strategies, as well as academic performance in adolescence. Our aim is to analyse whether musical training provided through specialised centres can improve auditory discrimination, working memory and strategies related to academic performance. Establishing the relevance of musical training to brain development could help support the use of music to maximise its benefits for cognitive skills and improve teaching.

To this end, we compared two groups of participants (12 to 14 years old), a group with musical training from a specialised centre and a group with no formal musical training. Our specific objectives were as follows:

  • Analyse possible differences between the two groups in terms of learning strategies.

  • Analyse possible differences between the two groups in terms of auditory discrimination.

  • Analyse possible differences between the two groups in terms of working memory.

  • Analyse possible differences between the two groups in terms of academic performance.

  • Analyse correlations between academic performance, auditory discrimination, learning strategies and working memory.

Methodology

Participants

A comparative post hoc design was used for this study. The sample included 60 Spanish students of both genders aged between 12 and 14 in the first and second years of compulsory secondary education. Participants with specific educational support needs (an intellectual disability, autism spectrum disorder, attention deficit disorder or others) were excluded.

The sample was composed of two groups (not random): a group with no musical training consisting of 30 students who had never received formal musical training at school and a group of 30 students who had received formal musical training for four years outside of the school system (all attending the same music centre) and for at least 2 hours per week to learn to play a musical instrument.

The four schools where the participants attended as the specialised music centres are located in Pozoblanco (province of Cordoba, Spain). Regarding the sociocultural context (Valle de los Pedroches), the schools are attended by students from various surrounding villages are of the same socioeconomic level, since on average, the economic profiles of the participants’ families are based in the agricultural sector rural. The participants in the two groups come from the same social context.

Descriptive data for the two groups are shown in Table 1. The two groups are comparable in terms of age and sex.

Table 1. Descriptive Data for the Sample

Instruments

(a) Learning strategies

The ACRA Learning Strategies Scale (Román & Gallego, Reference ROMÁN and GALLEGO1994) was used to measure students’ learning strategies. The ACRA scale is a questionnaire that refers to one of the following learning strategies:

  • Information acquisition scale (20 items): evaluates processing strategies that favour attention control and those that optimise repetition processes.

  • Information coding scale (46 items): assesses three groups of strategies (mnemonic, development and organisation of information).

  • Information retrieval scale (35 items): evaluates search and response generation strategies.

  • Processing support scale (35 items): assesses metacognitive, affective and social strategies

After obtaining raw scores, they were transformed into percentile scores. The indicators of validity and reliability obtained are acceptable for the secondary education students with which they have been validated.

(b) Working memory

Digit Span subtest (WISC-IV) (Wechsler, Reference WECHSLER2005) was used to evaluate working memory, which dictates a series of numbers to the participant and then asks the participant to repeat them. The numbers are grouped into sets that become increasingly longer and, therefore, more complex to remember. This procedure is repeated in the same (direct) order and in reverse order. Each correct response is scored with 1 point. The highest score that can be obtained from this scale is 16.

(c) Auditory discrimination

To measure auditory discrimination, the Phoneme Articulation subtest of the Dyslalia Assessment Test was used (Vallés Arándiga, Reference VALLÉS ARÁNDIGA1990). This test involves dictating 28 pairs of acoustically similar syllables and words and then asking the participant to repeat them. The score reflects the number of failures made in 28 elements.

Finally, we asked the participating schools to provide the average grades of the participants for the academic year (for the first and second semesters). This average was valued on a scale of 0 to 10 points. The subjects taken by the students in these two academic years are as follows:

  1. 1. Natural Sciences

  2. 2. Social Sciences, Geography and History

  3. 3. Spanish Language and Literature

  4. 4. Mathematics

  5. 5. Foreign Languages

  6. 6. Plastics

  7. 7. Music

  8. 8. Technology

  9. 9. Religion or Alternative Subjects

  10. 10. Second Foreign Language or Mathematics and Language Reinforcement

Procedure

For this study, permission was sought from the involved centres, and informed consent was given by the Director or the Director of Studies as appropriate. The ACRA scale was also provided, and the purposes of all tests and scales were explained. Once we had obtained signed copies of the consent form from the students’ legal guardians, the schools each booked one classroom to conduct all of the tests over one day.

The participants completed a questionnaire asking for basic information such as their age, gender, study centre and student number (to be matched to the list of academic results) and whether they had completed the last year of basic musical training. The students had unlimited time to complete ACRA scale. To avoid errors in the answers, we replaced scale options A, B, C and D with options never or hardly ever, sometimes, several times and always.

As the students completed the ACRA scale, they were individually taken to a different classroom to evaluate working memory and auditory discrimination.

Finally, to collect the students’ academic results, each centre provided a list with student numbers and average grades to maintain the students’ anonymity. The evaluation of all the tests took 70 minutes, and all of the participants took the tests in the same order.

Statistical analysis

Descriptive statistics were performed by obtaining mean, standard deviation, N and percentage. To compare both groups of participants, a t-test for independent samples (age) and a chi-square test (gender) were applied.

For objectives 1 to 4, mean comparison analyses (t-tests of independent samples) were carried out. The musical training of the subjects was selected as an independent variable: a group of subjects with no musical training and a group of subjects with specific musical training. The dependent variables of the study were learning strategies (the four scales), auditory discrimination (for which errors were counted), working memory (measured through the direct and inverse digit test) and academic performance. Cohen’s d test was used to calculate the effect size.

For objective 5, Pearson’s correlation coefficient was used. A 95% significance level was adopted (p = <.05). For all statistical analyses, the SPSS statistical package (version 21) was used.

Results

Objective 1: To study differences between the two groups in terms of learning strategies

The results of the analyses show significant differences between the groups in acquisition, coding, retrieval and support (p < .05). Table 2 shows that the averages for the group with musical training from a specialised music centre are higher across all of the measured variables of learning strategies than those of the group with no musical training. Cohen’s d test result indicates a moderate to large effect size.

Table 2. Differences between the Two Groups in Terms of Learning Strategies

Note: *significance p < .05

Objective 2: To study differences between the two groups in terms of auditory discrimination

The results show no significant differences between the groups for the auditory discrimination task, although the observed differences are close to reaching significance (p = .086) (Table 3). Cohen’s d result indicates a moderate effect size.

Table 3. Differences between the Two Groups in Terms of Auditory Discrimination

Note: *significance p < .05.

Objective 3: To study differences between the two groups in terms of working memory

The results obtained from the analysis show significant differences for the part of the memory involved in the processing of the central executive (inverse digits) but not for the part of the memory that uses the phonological loop (direct digits). As shown in Table 4, the average for the group with musical training is higher in inverse digits than that for the group with no musical training. Cohen’s d result indicates a moderate effect size for the variable inverse digits.

Table 4. Differences between the Two Groups in Terms of Working Memory

Note: *significance p < .05.

Objective 4: To study differences between the two groups in terms of academic performance

The results show significant differences between the two groups in terms of academic performance (p < .05) (Table 5) with higher grades found for the group with musical training. Cohen’s d result indicates a large effect size.

Table 5. Differences between the Two Groups in Terms of Academic Results

Note: *significance p < .05.

Objective 5: To study the relationship between academic performance and auditory discrimination, learning strategies and working memory

The results show a significant relationship between academic performance and auditory discrimination (Table 6). This correlation is negative: a higher score in academic performance corresponds to fewer errors in auditory discrimination. Significant correlations were also found for three of the four scales of learning strategies (p < .05) (except for coding). No significant correlations were found for working memory.

Table 6. Correlations between Academic Results, Auditory Discrimination and Working Memory

Note: *significance p < .05; r = Pearson’s correlation.

Discussion

Our results show significant differences between participants who have received formal musical training and those who have not. It is worth mentioning that all of the participants’ scores are within the normal range for their age. Participants who had received formal musical training in specialised centres in rural areas used more learning strategies and achieved higher scores on working memory (central executive) tests and in general academic performance. Academic performance was related to auditory discrimination and the use of learning strategies.

Our first objective was to analyse potential differences between the two groups in terms of learning strategies (students with musical training vs. students with no musical training). Students who have received musical training use more developed learning strategies in acquisition, coding and metacognitive strategies (supporting variables) (Virkkula & Nissilä, Reference VIRKKULA and NISSILÄ2017). Our results are in line with other research such as that of Bermell (Reference BERMELL2004), which shows that the neuropsychological processes of music affect the cognitive field by improving attention and concentration during learning, cognitive skills and other specific intellectual skills of the verbal, spatial and general fields (Hannon & Trainor, Reference HANNON and TRAINOR2007; Patel & Iversen, Reference PATEL and IVERSEN2007) that influences academic performance.

In further studies, music students exhibit improved academic performance because they show flexibility in problem-solving and perceptual speed (Helmbold et al., Reference HELMBOLD, RAMMSAYER and ALTENMÜLLER2005). In addition, music students show improved long-term visual-spatial, verbal and mathematical performance (Moreno et al., Reference MORENO, MARQUES, SANTOS, SANTOS, CASTRO and BESSON2009). González-Castro et al. (Reference GONZÁLEZ-CASTRO, RODRÍGUEZ, CUELI, CABEZA and ÁLVAREZ2014) also highlight the relationship between music and cognitive aspects such as vigilance and attention, which are essential to performance in school tasks. In the emotional sphere, music also facilitates support strategies for emotional learning (Gray et al., Reference GRAY, CRITCHLEY and KOELSCH2014; Koelsch & Skouras, Reference KOELSCH and SKOURAS2014).

In a study by Nielsen (Reference NIELSEN1999), the learning strategies of two organ students were identified as they prepared a piece of music. The students used selection and organisation of information strategies and thus used a systematic procedure to learn the material. Later, Nielsen (Reference NIELSEN2001) studied the learning strategies of advanced conservatory students during practical sessions. The results indicate that the students used more self-regulatory skills in addition to having specific objectives, participating in strategic planning, using self-instruction and task strategies and being selectively supervised at a detailed level during practice. The students also exhibited metacognitive skills such as the ability to assess their own performance. In a later study, the same researcher found beliefs about knowledge acquisition control and knowledge simplicity to be significantly related to strategies (Nielsen, Reference NIELSEN2012). In addition, the advantages and disadvantages of using formal and informal musical training to acquire strategies and channel creativity have been recently studied (Hess, Reference HESS2020; Kastner, Reference KASTNER2020).

Learning strategies are cognitive skills that help improve academic performance, as learning is more effective. These findings are in line with the findings of Martín-Lobo et al. (Reference MARTÍN-LOBO2018) and Martín et al. (Reference MARTÍN, GARCÍA, TORBAY and RODRÍGUEZ2008), who suggested that academic performance is related to the use of strategies that facilitate meaningful and self-regulated learning as well as deeper cognitive processing that seeks to find applications for the content studied.

Our second objective was to analyse auditory discrimination differences between the groups. Our hypothesis predicted that students with specialised musical training would make fewer errors on the provided test due to high levels of auditory stimulation. Nevertheless, we could not find such differences, although the margin of error (5%) might be having an influence on the results. Previous studies, such as Kraus and Chandrasekaran (Reference KRAUS and CHANDRASEKARAN2010), suggested that musicians develop the auditory system differently and have better auditory skills. This also fits in with the work of Marques et al. (Reference MARQUÉS, MORENO, CASTRO and BESSON2007), who found that musicians are more competent when required to quickly detect tonal changes in a foreign language.

Another objective was to study differences between the groups in terms of working memory. We expected to obtain higher scores for the group with specialised musical training than for that with no musical training. Our results partially support this hypothesis. The adolescents with musical training showed a greater mastery of the central executive than the participants with no musical training. Other studies corroborate these results but for other populations. Pallesen et al. (Reference PALLESEN2010) conducted a study of two groups of adult musicians (a small sample) analysing memory stress and found that adults with musical training have better auditory working memory than those with only elementary training. Killough et al. (Reference KILLOUGH, THOMPSON and MORGAN2015) demonstrated the same result for a population of musicians, Hansen et al. (Reference HANSEN, WALLENTIN and VUUST2013) found this result for expert musicians, and Lu and Grenwald (Reference LU and GRENWALD2016) identified this result for adults, confirming the benefits of musical training for the expansion of working memory. Our study contributes to this work by showing that with the expansion of musical training in primary education, the benefits applicable from the start of secondary education can improve academic performance in later educational stages. Similar results are given by Roden et al. (Reference RODEN2014), who evaluated working memory in 7- to 8-year-old children following an 18-month music programme and found that the students showed an increase in auditory information processing. Musical training favours the development of working memory from early childhood because music learning involves cognitive functions of coding, storage and retrieval associated with memory (Zuk et al., Reference ZUK, BENJAMIN, KENYON and GAAB2015). For example, phonological memory has been found to be a predictor of pronunciation ability along with perception and auditory discrimination in children who begin learning music at an early age (Hu et al., Reference HU, ACKERMANN, MARTIN, ERB, WINKLER and REITERER2012).

Along the same lines, it was found that musical training favours musical skills developing the mechanisms of verbal long-term memory in musicians (Franklin et al., Reference FRANKLIN2008). Studies of individuals learning to play a musical instrument suggest that short- and long-term memory are needed for comprehension, repetition, and the exercise of rhythm and sequences, which implies the activation of these processes at a conscious level until learning is automated (Soria-Urios et al., Reference SORIA-URIOS, DUQUE and GARCÍA-MORENO2011). The perception of musical tone has been shown to be a predictor of pronunciation when using a second language (Posedelet et al., 2011).

Finally, the effects of school musical training programmes on verbal and visual memory skills in primary school children have been examined, and significant improvements in verbal memory have been identified (Roden et al., Reference RODEN, KREUTZ and BONGARD2012). From a study on instrumental training programmes, Roden et al. (Reference RODEN2014) concluded that students with musical training show significant differences in working memory in terms of central executive, phonological loop and visuospatial agenda components; such individuals also exhibit improved cognitive functioning and overall performance.

For objective 4, our hypothesis predicted higher levels of academic performance for the group with musical training, and our results are in line with this prediction. Engaña (Reference ENGAÑA2008) observed stronger performance in subjects such as language and mathematics in children who were part of a junior orchestra with similar musical training using the SIMCE 2001 and PSU 2003 tests. Our findings are also in line with those reported by Reyes (Reference REYES2011), who studied 4000 primary school students from the community of Valencia (Spain). In this study, the author found children with artistic musical training to obtain better academic results than others while analysing the impact of musical training in subjects such as mathematics, language, sports, science and the arts. Such results also coincide with other recent studies carried out in other countries, such as Switzerland (Wetter et al., Reference WETTER, KOERNER and SCHWANINGER2009), Canada (Cabanac et al., Reference CABANAC, PERLOVSKY, BONNIOT-CABANAC and CABANAC2013) and the United States (Kinney, Reference KINNEY2008; Southgate & Roscigno, Reference SOUTHGATE and ROSCIGNO2009).

Finally, for objective 5, the results show a significant correlation between academic performance and auditory discrimination and learning strategies. Regarding the correlation between academic performance and auditory discrimination, the relationship seems obvious, as Defior (Reference DEFIOR2008) already demonstrated, showing that auditory discrimination is fundamental to the development of reading and writing skills. Rodríguez and Remesal (Reference RODRÍGUEZ and REMESAL2007) also found a correlation between academic performance and learning strategies in university students. The authors observed higher academic performance in students with higher scores on learning strategies.

The main limitation of this study relates to the difficulty of finding participants with musical training from specialised centres. For this reason, we had to recruit participants from several schools to obtain a sample of the same socioeconomic level. According to a study carried out in the United States (Elpus, Reference ELPUS2013), it is important to control demographic variables used in comparative studies such as the one we carried out (studying a single music centre, socioeconomic level, gender and ethnicity). We tried, as much as possible, to ensure that the variables for our two study groups were homogeneous. Therefore, it may be advisable to carry out a similar study in a larger city (or several) to recruit participants from different music schools. This approach would help increase sample variability and geographical representativeness. Another limitation of this study concerns the academic performance variable used. This variable was calculated from the scores of students in different subjects and from different schools (although all of the schools studied are located in the same rural area and are of the same socioeconomic level). Variations in the qualification criteria of the different schools or teachers might have affected our results. As a final limitation, we must note our inability to control certain variables, such as the IQ scores of the participants and their prior cognitive skills, even though all of the involved students participated in schooling without difficulty and within the normal range. For this reason, we must be cautious in interpreting differences in the academic performance of the two groups studied even though participants from different schools were indiscriminately assigned to either group. It should be noted that the conclusions presented were already known in the scientific field, but it is always helpful to review the findings in different environments and at different times, to find new findings or errors from the past.

Finally, we believe that this study highlights the value of formal musical training and the relevance of including this subject in educational and academic contexts within the educational field and adopting neuroscience to improve learning.

The results of this study show significant correlations between musical training and learning strategies, working memory and academic results in adolescence. More specifically, we found that students with specialised musical training achieve higher scores on learning strategies and their subscales: acquisition, coding, recovery and support. We observed higher levels of auditory discrimination in the group with specialised musical training. However, these differences are not significant, possibly due to the influence of the margin of error (5%) on results. In addition, students with specialist musical training scored better on the inverse digit subtest of the working memory scale, suggesting further development of the central executive. We observed better academic results for students in the group with specialised musical training. We found correlations between academic performance, auditory discrimination and learning strategies. To conclude, we highlight the relevance of neuroscience to music training from specialised centres. It would be desirable to promote musical training for students in all educational contexts given the data obtained and the scientific literature.

References

BERMELL, M. A. (2004). Bases de la investigación musical. [Bases of musical research]. Música y Educación, 17(60), 109124.Google Scholar
BESSON, M., CHOBERT, J. & MARIE, C. (2011). Transfer of training between music and speech: Common processing, attention, and memory. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 112. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpysg.2011.00094.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
CABANAC, A., PERLOVSKY, L., BONNIOT-CABANAC, M. C. & CABANAC, M. (2013). Music and academic performance. Behavioural Brain Research, 256, 257260. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2013.08.023.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
CAMPAYO-MUÑOZ, E. & CABEDO-MAS, A. (2017). The role of emotional skills in music education. British Journal of Music Education, 34(3), 243258. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0265051717000067.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
COLLINS, A. (2013). Neuroscience meets music education: Exploring the implications of neural processing models on music education practice. International Journal of Music Education, 31(2), 217231. https://doi.org/10.1177/0255761413483081 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
COUMEL, M., CHRISTINER, M. & REITERER, S. M. (2019). Second language accent faking ability depends on musical abilities, not on working memory. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 257. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00257 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
CUETOS, F. (2012). Language Neuroscience. Neurological Bases and Clinical Implications. Buenos Aires: Editorial Panamericana.Google Scholar
CURTIS, L. & FALLIN, J. (2014). Neuroeducation and music: Collaboration for student success. Music Educators Journal, 101(2), 5256. https://doi.org/10.1177/0027432114553637 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DALLA BELLA, S. (2015). Music and brain plasticity. In Hallam, S., Cross, I., and Thaut, M., The Oxford Handbook of Music Psychology (2nd ed., pp. 116). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
DEFIOR, S. (2008). ¿Cómo facilitar el aprendizaje inicial de la lectoescritura? Papel de las habilidades fonológicas. [How to facilitate initial literacy learning? Role of phonological skills]. Infancia y Aprendizaje, 31(3), 333345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DRAYNA, D., et al. (2001). Genetic correlates of musical pitch recognition in humans. Science, 291, 19691972.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
ELPUS, K. (2013). Is it the music or is it selection bias? A nationwide analysis of music and non-music students’ SAT scores. Journal of Research in Music Education, 61(2), 175194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
ENGAÑA, P. E. (2008). Relevancia e Impacto de las Actividades Artísticas sobre los Resultados Escolares: el Caso de la Orquesta de Curanilahue. [Relevance and Impact of Artistic Activities on Academic Results: The Case of the Curanilahue Orchestra]. Tesis Doctoral, Universidad de Chile.Google Scholar
FIVEASH, A. & PAMMER, K. (2014). Music and language: Do they draw on similar syntactic working memory resources? Psychology of Music, 42(2) 190209. https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735612463949.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
FRANKLIN, M. S., et al. (2008). The effects of musical training on verbal memory. Psychology of Music, 36(3), 353365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
GONZÁLEZ-CASTRO, P., RODRÍGUEZ, C., CUELI, M., CABEZA, L. & ÁLVAREZ, L. (2014). Mathematical competencies and executive control in students with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder and Learning Difficulties in Mathematics. Revista de Psicodidáctica, 19, 125143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
GRAY, M., CRITCHLEY, H. & KOELSCH, S. (2014). Superficial amygdala and hippocampal activity during affective music listening observed at 3 T but not 1.5 T fMRI. Neuroimage, 101, 364369. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.07.007.Google Scholar
HALLAM, S. (2010). The power of music: Its impact on the intellectual, social and personal development of children and young people. International Journal of Music Education, 28(3), 269289. https://doi.org/10.1177/0255761410370658.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
HALLAM, S., CROSS, I. & THAUT, M. (2009). Oxford Handbook of Music Psychology. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
HAMBRICK, D. Z., OSWALD, F. L., DAROWSKI, E. S., RENCH, T. A. & BROU, R. (2010). Predictors of multitasking performance in a synthetic work paradigm. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24(8), 11491167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
HANNON, E. E. & TRAINOR, L. J. (2007). Music acquisition: Effects of enculturation and formal training on development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 466472.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
HANSEN, M., WALLENTIN, M. & VUUST, P. (2013). Working memory and musical competence of musicians and non-musicians. Psychology of Music, 41(6), 779793. https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735612452186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
HELMBOLD, N., RAMMSAYER, T. & ALTENMÜLLER, E. (2005). Differences in primary mental abilities between musicians and nonmusicians. Journal of Individual Differences, 26(2), 7485.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
HESS, J. (2020). Finding the “both/and”: Balancing informal and formal music learning. International Journal of Music Education, 38(3), 441455. https://doi.org/10.1177/0255761420917226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
HETLAND, L. & WINNER, E. (2001). The arts and academic achievement: What the evidence shows. Arts Education Policy Review, 102(5), 36. https://doi.org/10.1080/10632910109600008.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
HICKOK, G. & POEPPEL, D. (2007). The cortical organization of speech processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8, 393402.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
HU, X., ACKERMANN, H., MARTIN, J. A., ERB, M., WINKLER, S. & REITERER, S. (2012). Language aptitude for pronunciation in advanced second language (L2) learners: behavioural predictors and neural substrates. Brain Lang, 127, 366376. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2012.11.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
JAUSET-BERROCAL, J. A. & SORIA-URIOS, G. (2018). Neurorrehabilitación cognitiva: fundamentos y aplicaciones de la musicoterapia neurológica. [Cognitive neurorehabilitation foundation and application of neurological music therapy]. Revue Neurologique, 67, 303310. https://doi.org/10.33588/rn.6708.201802.Google Scholar
JENTSCHKE, S. & KOELSCH, S. (2009). Musical training modulates the development of syntax processing in children. NeuroImage, 47, 735744.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
JENTSCHKE, S., KOELSCH, S., SALLAT, S. & FRIEDERICI, A. D. (2008). Children with specific language impairment also show impairment in music syntactic processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 19401951.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
JUSLIN, P. N. & LAUKKA, P. (2003). Communication of emotions in vocal expression and music performance: Different channels, same code? Psychological Bulletin, 129, 770814.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
KASTNER, J.D. (2020). Healing bruises: Identity tensions in a beginning teacher’s use of formal and informal music learning. Research Studies in Music Education, 42(1), 318. https://doi.org/10.1177/1321103X18774374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
KAY, B. P., et al. (2012). Moderating effects of music on resting state networks. Brain Research, 1447, 5364. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2012.01.064.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
KILLOUGH, C.M., THOMPSON, L. A. & MORGAN, G. (2015). Self-regulation and working memory in musical performers. Psychology of Music, 43(1), 86102. https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735613498917.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
KINNEY, D. W. (2008). Selected demographic variables, school music participation, and achievement test scores of urban middle school students. Journal of Research in Music Education, 56(2), 145161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
KOELSCH, S. (2013). Brain and Music. Oxford: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
KOELSCH, S. (2015). Music-evoked emotions: Principles, brain correlates, and implications for therapy. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1337, 193201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
KOELSCH, S., GUNTER, T.C., WITTFOTH, M. & SAMMLER, D. (2005). Interaction between syntax processing in language and in music: An ERP study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 15651577. https://doi.org/10.1162/08989290774597290.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
KOELSCH, S. & SKOURAS, S. (2014). Functional centrality of amygdala, striatum and hypothalamus in a “small-world” network underlying joy: An MRI study with music. Human Brain Mapping, 35, 34853498. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
KRAUS, N. & CHANDRASEKARAN, B. (2010). Musical training for the development of auditory skills. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 599605. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2882.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LAMONT, A. (2016). Musical development from the early years onward. In Hallam, S., Cross, I. & Taut, M (eds.), Oxford Handbook of Music Psychology. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
LU, C.-I. & GRENWALD, M. (2016). Reading and working memory in adults with or without formal musical training: Musical and lexical tone. Psychology of Music, 44(3), 369387. https://doi.org/10.1177/030573561456888.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MARQUÉS, C., MORENO, S., CASTRO, S. L. & BESSON, M. (2007). Musicians detect pitch violation in a foreign language better than non-musicians: Behavioural and electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(9), 14531463. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2007.19.9.1453.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MARTÍN, E., GARCÍA, L. A., TORBAY, A. & RODRÍGUEZ, T. (2008). Estrategias de aprendizaje y rendimiento académico en estudiantes universitarios. [Learning strategies and academic performance in university students]. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 8(3), 401412.Google Scholar
MARTIN-LOBO, P. (2010). The Reading. Neuropsychological Learning Processes, Difficulties, Intervention Programs and Case Studies. Barcelona: Lebón.Google Scholar
MARTÍN-LOBO, P., et al. (2018). A study of 16-year-old student learning strategies from a neuropsychological perspective: An intervention proposal. Trends in Neuroscience and Education, 11, 18. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tine.2018.03.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MILOVANOV, R., PIETILÄ, P., TERVANIEMI, M. & ESQUEF, P. A. (2010). Foreign language pronunciation skills and musical aptitude: a study of Finnish adults with higher education. Learning and Individual Differences, 20, 5660. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2009.11.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MORENO, S., MARQUES, C., SANTOS, A., SANTOS, M., CASTRO, S. L. & BESSON, M. (2009). Musical training influences linguistic abilities in 8-year-old children: more evidence for brain plasticity. Cerebral Cortex, 19(3), 712723. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhn120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
NIELSEN, S. G. (1999). Regulation of learning strategies during practice. Psychology of Music, 27(2), 218229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
NIELSEN, S. G. (2001). Self-regulating learning strategies in instrumental music practice. Music Education Research, 3(2), 155167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
NIELSEN, SG. (2012). Epistemic beliefs and self-regulated learning in music students. Psychology of Music, 40(3), 324338. https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735610385509.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
PALLESEN, K. J., et al. (2010). Cognitive control in auditory working memory is enhanced in musicians. PloS One, 5(6). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0011120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
PANTELEEVA, Y., et al. (2017). Music for anxiety? Meta-analysis of anxiety reduction in non-clinical samples. Psychology of Music, 0305735617712424. https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735617712424.Google Scholar
PATEL, A. D. (2008). Music, Language, and the Brain. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press Google Scholar
PATEL, A. D. (2010). Music, Language, and the Brain. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
PATEL, A. D. (2012). The OPERA hypothesis: assumptions and clarifications. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1252, 124128. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06426.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
PATEL, A. D. & IVERSEN, J. R. (2007). The linguistic benefits of musical abilities. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 369372. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.08.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
PEÑALBA, A. (2017). La defensa de la educación musical desde las neurociencias. [Defence of music education from neuroscience]. Revista Electrónica Complutense de Investigación en Educación Musical, 14, 109127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
PERETZ, I., et al. (2009). Music lexical networks. The cortical organization of music recognition. The neurosciences and music III –disorders and plasticity. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1169, 256265. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04557.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
PERLOVSKY, L. (2012). Cognitive function, origin, and evolution of musical emotions. Misicae Scientiae, 16(2), 185199. https://doi.org/10.1177/102986491244832 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
PETERSON, A. D. (2011). The impact of neuroscience on music education advocacy and philosophy. Arts Education Policy Review, 112(4), 206213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
POSEDELET, J., EMERY, L., SOUZA, B. & FOUNTAIN, C. (2012). Pitch perception, working memory, and second language phonological production. Psychology of Music, 40, 508517. https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735611415145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
RASINSKI, T. V., BLACHOWICZ, C. L. & LEMS, K. (2012). Fluency Instruction: Research-Based Best Practices. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
RESNICK, L. B. & WEAVER, P. A. (2013). Theory and Practice of Early Reading. Volume 1. Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
REYES, M. C. (2011). El rendimiento académico de los alumnos de primaria que cursan estudios artístico-musicales en la Comunidad Valenciana. [The Academic Performance of Primary School Pupils Who Study Art and Music in the Valencian Community]. Tesis Doctoral. Departamento de Filosofía. Universidad de Valencia.Google Scholar
ROBISON, M. K., MILLER, A. L. & UNSWORTH, N. (2018). Individual differences in working memory capacity and filtering. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 44(7), 10381053. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2014.01.004 Google ScholarPubMed
RODEN, I., et al. (2014). Effects of musical training on attention, processing speed and cognitive music abilities—Findings from a longitudinal study. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28, 545557. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3034.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
RODEN, I., GRUBE, D., BONGARD, S. & KREUTZ, G. (2013). Does musical training enhance working memory performance? Findings from a quasi-experimental longitudinal study. Psychology of Music, 42(2), 284298. https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735612471239 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
RODEN, I., KREUTZ, G., & BONGARD, S. (2012). Effects of a school-based instrumental music program on verbal and visual memory in primary school children: A longitudinal study. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 19. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00572.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
RODRÍGUEZ, J. S. & REMESAL, A. F. (2007). Estrategias de aprendizaje y rendimiento académico en estudiantes universitarios. [Learning strategies and academic performance in university students]. Revista de Investigación Educativa 25(2), 421441.Google Scholar
ROMÁN, J. M. & GALLEGO, S. (1994). Escala de Estrategias de Aprendizaje, ACRA. [ACRA, Learning Strategies Scale]. Madrid: TEA Ediciones.Google Scholar
SCHELLENBERG, E. G. (2005). Music and cognitive abilities. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 322325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
SCHELLENBERG, E. G. (2009). Musical training and non-musical abilities: Commentary on Stoesz, Jakobson, Kilgour, and Lewycky (2007) and Jakobson, Lewycky, Kilgour, and Stoesz (2008). Music Perception, 27, 139143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
SCHELLENBERG, E. G. & HALLAM, S. (2005). Music listening and cognitive abilities in 10- and 11-year-olds: The Blur effect. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1060, 202209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
SCHELLENBERG, E. G., NAKATA, T., HUNTER, P. G. & TAMOTO, S. (2007). Exposure to music and cognitive performance: Tests of children and adults. Psychology of Music, 35, 519.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
SCHELLENBERG, E. G. & PERETZ, I. (2008). Music, language, and cognition: Unresolved issues. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 4546.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
SCHELLENBERG, E.G. (2006). Exposure to music: The truth about the consequences. In McPherson, G. E. (ed.), The Child as Musician: A Handbook of Musical Development (pp. 111134). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
SCHLAUG, G., NORTON, A., OVERY, K. & WINNER, E. (2005). Effects of musical training on the child’s brain and cognitive development. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1060(1), 219230.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
SHIPSTEAD, Z., LINDSEY, D. R. B., MARSHALL, R. L., & ENGLE, R. W. (2014). The mechanisms of working memory capacity: primary memory, secondary memory, and attention control. Journal of Memory and Language, 72, 116141. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2014.01.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
SORIA-URIOS, G., DUQUE, P. & GARCÍA-MORENO, J.M. (2011). Música y cerebro: fundamentos neurocientíficos y trastornos musicales. [Music and the brain: neuroscientific foundations and musical disorders]. Revue Neurologique, 52, 4555.Google Scholar
SOUTHGATE, D. E. & ROSCIGNO, V. J. (2009). The impact of music on childhood and adolescent achievement. Social Science Quarterly, 90, 421. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-6237.2009.00598.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
STEGEMÖÖLLER, E. L., et al. (2008). Musical training and vocal production of speech and song. Music Percept, 25, 419428. https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2008.25.5.419 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
STOESZ, B. M., JAKOBSON, L. S., KILGOUR, A. R. & LEWYCKY, S. T. (2007). Local processing advantage in musicians: Evidence from disembedding and constructional tasks. Music Perception, 25, 153165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
SWISS INSTITUTE of EDUCATION and CULTURE (2014). Swiss Education Report: Pre-School and Primary Levels. Recuperated from: http://www.skbf-csre.ch/fileadmin/files/pdf/bildungsmonitoring/epaper-bildungsbericht2014fr/#/0 Google Scholar
TOSCANO-FUENTES, C. (2011). Estudio Empírico de la Relación existente entre el Nivel de Adquisición de una Segunda Lengua, la Capacidad Auditiva y la Inteligencia Musical del alumnado. [Empirical study and the relationship between the level of acquisition of a second language, the auditory capacity and the musical intelligence of students] . Huelva: Universidad de Huelva.Google Scholar
TRIMBLE, M. & HESDORFFER, D. (2017). Music and the brain: The neuroscience of music and musical appreciation. British Journal of Psychiatry International, 14(2), 2831. https://doi.org/10.1192/S2056474000001720.Google ScholarPubMed
VALLÉS ARÁNDIGA, A. (1990). P.A.F. Evaluación de la dislalia. (P.A.F. Assessment of dislalia) . Madrid: Ciencias de la Educación Preescolar y Especial.Google Scholar
VIRKKULA, E. & NISSILÄ, S. P. (2017). Towards professionalism in music: self-assessed learning strategies of conservatory music students. Center for Educational Policy Studies Journal, 7(3), 113135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
WECHSLER, D. (2005). WISC IV: Escala de Inteligencia Wechsler para Niños IV. [Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children IV] . Madrid: TEA.Google Scholar
WETTER, O. E., KOERNER, F. & SCHWANINGER, A. (2009). Does musical training improve school performance? Instructional Science, 37, 365374. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-008-9052-y CrossRefGoogle Scholar
ZATORRE, R. J. (2001). Neural specializations for tonal processing. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 930, 193210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
ZATORRE, R. J., CHEN, J. L. & PENHUNE, V. B. (2007). When the brain plays music: auditory motor interactions in music perception and production. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8, 547558.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
ZUK, J., BENJAMIN, C., KENYON, A. & GAAB, N. (2015). Correction: Behavioural and neural correlates of executive functioning in musicians and non-musicians. PLoS One, 10(9), e0137930. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0137930.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptive Data for the Sample

Figure 1

Table 2. Differences between the Two Groups in Terms of Learning Strategies

Figure 2

Table 3. Differences between the Two Groups in Terms of Auditory Discrimination

Figure 3

Table 4. Differences between the Two Groups in Terms of Working Memory

Figure 4

Table 5. Differences between the Two Groups in Terms of Academic Results

Figure 5

Table 6. Correlations between Academic Results, Auditory Discrimination and Working Memory