Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rcrh6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-27T15:41:18.795Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Effect of a structured educational intervention on explanatory models of relatives of patients with schizophrenia

Randomised controlled trial

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

S. Das
Affiliation:
College of Nursing
B. Saravanan
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry
K. P. Karunakaran
Affiliation:
College of Nursing
S. Manoranjitham
Affiliation:
College of Nursing
P. Ezhilarasu
Affiliation:
College of Nursing
K. S. Jacob*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry Christian Medical College, Vellore, India
*
Professor K. S. Jacob, Department of Psychiatry, Christian Medical College, Vellore 632 002, India. Fax: +91 416 2262268; e-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Summary

We examined the effect of a structured educational programme on explanatory models of illness among the relatives of people with schizophrenia, in a randomised controlled trial. Participants were assessed at baseline (n=100) and after 2 weeks (n=75) using a vignette from the Short Explanatory Model Interview. There was a reduction in non-biomedical causal explanatory models at follow-up among those who had completed the structured educational programme compared with the control group. There was no significant difference in non-biomedical treatment explanatory models between the two groups.

Type
Short Reports
Copyright
Copyright © 2006 The Royal College of Psychiatrists 

By eliciting the explanatory models of patients and their relatives in routine clinical practice, mental health professionals can better understand the subjective experience of illness (Reference KleinmanKleinman, 1980). The past few years have witnessed an increase in the literature on beliefs about causes of schizophrenia and its treatment (Reference Angermeyer and MatschingerAngermeyer & Matschinger, 1996; Reference Banerjee and RoyBanerjee & Roy, 1998; Reference Kulhara, Avasthi and SharmaKulhara et al, 2000), but few studies have evaluated the efficacy of interventions to change explanatory models. We used a randomised controlled design to examine the effect of a structured educational programme on explanatory models of illness among the relatives of people with schizophrenia.

METHOD

Out-patients who attended the Department of Psychiatry, Christian Medical College, Vellore, India for the first time and who satisfied ICD–10 (World Health Organization, 1993) diagnostic criteria for a research diagnosis of schizophrenia were invited to participate in the study with their relatives (Fig. 1). Patients continued to be managed by their psychiatrists. Informed consent was obtained from the relatives, who were then randomised in blocks of four, using a computer program, to a group that would receive the structured educational intervention or to a control group. Progress through the trial is shown in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1 CONSORT diagram.

The Short Explanatory Model Interview (SEMI; Reference Lloyd, Jacob and PatelLloyd et al, 1998) formed the basis of the evaluation. A vignette describing a typical patient with chronic psychosis was presented (Reference Joel, Sathyaseelan and JayakaranJoel et al, 2003). This was followed by open-ended questions to elicit the relatives’ beliefs about the perceived causes and consequences of the condition, and help-seeking behaviour. A verbatim record of the responses was made, and these were later grouped into categories using the procedure recommended by the SEMI. The Tamil version of the instrument was employed (Reference Joel, Sathyaseelan and JayakaranJoel et al, 2003). Basic demographic and clinical information was also recorded. The assessments were conducted on day 1 (baseline) and after 2 weeks of a structured educational programme in the intervention group. The research nurse who performed the evaluations was masked to the intervention status of the participants.

We developed a structured educational programme which discussed the different explanatory causal and treatment models prevalent in the region, and we also presented the biomedical perspective without dismissing or directly challenging local beliefs. This intervention had three components: exploring explanatory models; psychoeducation, aimed at teaching the relatives about the illness, symptoms, treatment and prognosis; and strategies to reduce the risk of relapse. The education package covered the following topics in two sessions: symptoms, beliefs about causation, psychosocial influences, prevalence, biomedical model, diagnosis, treatments (including medication and adherence), and coping strategies for families.

Sample size was calculated assuming that half of those who receive education and one-fifth of those who do not would consider the illness to have a medical cause by the end of the trial. For a power of 80% and 95% confidence the minimum sample required was 72 (36 in each arm). To compensate for possible loss to follow-up we recruited 100 participants.

RESULTS

A total of 100 patients and 100 relatives (one first-degree relative per patient) were contacted, all of whom agreed to take part in the study. The majority of the participants were male (56%), married (91%) and literate (80%), with a mean age of 45.3 years (s.d.=15.7). Most of the participants (60%) regarded the condition of the patient described in the vignette as a disease. Nearly all of the relatives who were interviewed (94%) felt that help should be sought from a doctor or hospital. However, a significant proportion of the relatives also attributed the patient's condition to previous deeds (43%) or to punishment by God (43%), and felt that visiting temples and places of worship could solve the problem (33%). A minority of participants attributed the patient's condition to black magic (33%) or evil spirits (10%), and felt that help should be sought from a traditional healer (5%) or a shaman (9%). The majority of participants held more than one explanatory model of illness, and many believed that there was at least one non-biomedical explanation for the patient's psychosis. The differences between the baseline socio-demographic variables and the explanatory models of the two groups were not statistically significant.

The 25 participants lost to follow-up (see Fig. 1) did not differ significantly from those assessed at 2 weeks with regard to socio-demographic variables, explanatory models at baseline, or treatment arm.

Intent-to-treat analysis of all participants (with the last observation carried forward for those who were lost to follow-up) showed that the intervention group had a statistically significant reduction in the total number of non-biomedical causal models of psychosis compared with the group of relatives who did not receive additional education (mean=0.88 (s.d.=0.96) v. 1.32 (1.15); P=0.08). The two groups also showed a significant difference with regard to change in non-medical causal models from baseline after adjusting for age, gender and literacy (mean=70.58 (s.d.=1.21) v. 0.14 (1.16); P=0.003). There were significant differences between the two groups at follow-up in the number attributing the condition to black magic (8 v. 16; P=0.08) and the number believing that visiting a place of worship would effect a cure (7 v. 14; P=0.04). However, there were no differences between the two groups with regard to non-medical treatment models of illness (see data supplement to online version of this paper). Similar results were obtained when the data for participants who completed the trial were analysed.

DISCUSSION

This study evaluated an educational intervention for psychosis and assessed its effect in the immediate follow-up period. Limitations included the drop-out rate of 25% and the lack of long-term assessment. However, the participants who were lost to follow-up did not differ significantly in terms of baseline variables from those who remained in the trial. Also, the results of an intent-to-treat analysis and analysis of those who completed the trial were similar.

The baseline data suggest that the relatives of patients with psychosis have multiple, diverse and contradictory explanatory models of illness. Participants held simultaneous beliefs in naturalistic explanations (e.g. disease) and personalistic explanations (e.g. supernatural causation, sin and punishment, karma). They also suggested that help could be sought from a range of different sources (e.g. doctor, temple or place of worship, traditional healer). Other studies of explanatory models of psychosis have reported similar findings (Reference Joel, Sathyaseelan and JayakaranJoel et al, 2003), and the issues surrounding the simultaneous holding of multiple beliefs have been discussed in the literature (Saravanan et al, Reference Saravanan, Jacob and Prince2004, Reference Saravanan, David and Bhugra2005).

The educational intervention programme used in this study discussed the local explanations for psychosis, and presented the biomedical explanatory model as an alternative. The indigenous beliefs of the participants were not challenged. The programme did not claim exclusivity or superiority of biomedical beliefs, but discussed issues relating to symptoms, disease models, medication and regular treatment. Although such issues are often raised in routine clinical practice (and the control group may also have received such information), psychoeducation does not follow a structured format, and psychiatrists tend to dismiss local explanations and to favour biomedical concepts.

The relatives who received the educational intervention showed some change in their explanatory models in the immediate follow-up period. However, many of the indigenous explanatory models persisted, especially those related to treatment. The results of this study suggest that although some explanatory models can be changed, others may be more resistant to modification. In the developing world, people with mental disorders often visit places of worship, traditional healers and psychiatric hospitals in search of both relief from symptoms and cure (Reference JacobJacob, 1999). However, it is acknowledged that holding non-biomedical beliefs about psychosis can delay the recognition of disease, prevent early institution of treatment with medication, and interfere with adherence to treatment, resulting in a poor outcome. Health education packages should discuss the advantages of medication, but should not dismiss alternative explanations of illness, as these may also help to restore mental health. Further research is needed to identify the components of the ideal health education package and maximise its effectiveness in changing explanatory models and thereby preventing relapse.

Data supplement 1 Comparison of explanatory models of psychosis after 2 weeks: intent-to-treat analysis1

Explanatory models Intervention group (n=50) n (%) Control group (n=50) n (%) Univariate analysis Multivariate analysis3
Odds ratio (95% CI) P 2 Adjusted odds ratio (95% CI) P
Causal models
    Condition caused by previous deeds/karma 19 (38.0) 23 (46.0) 0.72 (0.32-1.60) 0.41 0.79 (0.34 to 1.86) 0.59
    Condition caused by black magic 8 (16.0) 16 (32.0) 0.41 (0.16-1.10) 0.06 0.40 (0.15 to 1.10) 0.08
    Condition caused by evil spirits 4 (8.0) 6 (12.0) 0.64 (0.17-2.40) 0.50 0.42 (0.10 to 1.84) 0.25
    Condition resulting from punishment by God 13 (26.0) 21 (42.0) 0.46 (0.21-1.10) 0.09 0.61 (0.25 to 1.50) 0.28
    Condition suggests disease 34 (68.0) 38 (76.0) 0.67 (0.28-1.62) 0.34 1.57 (0.57 to 4.31) 0.38
Treatment models
    Shaman can help patient 1 (2.0) 4 (8.0) 0.24 (0.03-2.18) 0.17 0.21 (0.02 to 2.31) 0.20
    Traditional healers can be visited for treatment 3 (6.0) 0 (0.0) 0.08 0.92
    Can pray and visit temple/place of worship for cure 7 (14.0) 14 (28.0) 0.42 (0.15-1.15) 0.09 0.32 (0.11 to 0.95) 0.04
    Visiting a doctor is useful 45 (90.0) 48 (96.0) 0.38 (0.07-2.03) 0.24 0.35 (0.06 to 2.0) 0.23
Intervention group Mean (s.d.) Control group Mean (s.d.) t (d.f.) P Linear regression4
B (95% CI) P
Total number of non-medical causal models5 0.88 (0.96) 1.32 (1.15) -2.1 (94.9) 0.04 -0.38 (-0.80 to 0.05) 0.08
Total number of non-medical treatment models5 0.22 (0.62) 0.36 (0.60) 1.55 (98) 0.25 -0.15 (-0.43 to 0.06) 0.14
Change in number of non-medical causal models5 -0.58 (1.21) 0.14 (1.16) -3.03 (98) 0.003 -0.73 (-1.21 to -0.25) 0.003
Change in number of non-medical treatment models5 -0.18 (0.71) -0.22 (0.62) 0.30 (98) 0.77 0.03 (-0.25 to 0.30) 0.84

Footnotes

Declaration of interest

None.

References

Angermeyer, M. C. & Matschinger, H. (1996) Relatives’ beliefs about causes of schizophrenia. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 93, 199204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Banerjee, G. & Roy, S. (1998) Determinants of help-seeking behaviour offamiies of schizophrenic patients attending a teaching hospital in India: an indigenous explanatory model. International Journal of Social Psychiatry, 44, 199214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacob, K. S. (1999) Mental disorders across cultures: the common issues. International Review of Psychiatry, 11, 111115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joel, D., Sathyaseelan, M., Jayakaran, R., et al (2003) Explanatory models of psychosis among community health workers in South India. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 108, 6669.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kleinman, A. (1980) Patients and Healers in the Context of Culture: An Exploration of the Borderland Between Anthropology, Medicine and Psychiatry. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kulhara, P., Avasthi, A. & Sharma, A. (2000) Magico-religious beliefs in schizophrenia: a study from North India. Psychopathology, 33, 6368.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lloyd, K. R., Jacob, K. S., Patel, V., et al (1998) The development of the Short Explanatory Model Interview (SEMI) and its use among primary care attenders with common mental disorders: apreliminary report. Psychological Medicine, 28, 12311237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saravanan, B., Jacob, K. S., Prince, M., et al (2004) Culture and insight revisited. British Journal of Psychiatry, 184, 107109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saravanan, B., David, A., Bhugra, D., et al (2005) Insight in people with psychosis: the influence of culture. International Review of Psychiatry, 17, 8387.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
World Health Organization (1993) The ICD–10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Diagnostic Criteria for Research. Geneva: WHO.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1 CONSORT diagram.

Figure 1

Data supplement 1 Comparison of explanatory models of psychosis after 2 weeks: intent-to-treat analysis1

Supplementary material: PDF

Das et al. supplementary material

Supplementary Table S1

Download Das et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 30.7 KB
Supplementary material: File

Das et al. supplementary material

Supplementary Material

Download Das et al. supplementary material(File)
File 508 Bytes
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.