Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-7cvxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T21:42:13.052Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Reward functioning from an attentional perspective and obsessive-compulsive symptoms—an eye-tracking study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 November 2022

Dana Basel
Affiliation:
School of Psychological Sciences, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel
Amit Lazarov*
Affiliation:
School of Psychological Sciences, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel
*
*Author for correspondence: Amit Lazarov Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Recently, a novel approach to obsessive-compulsive disorder has emerged, implicating altered reward functioning in the disorder. Yet, no study to date has directly examined the attentional aspect of reward functioning in participants with obsessive-compulsive (OC) symptoms, with past research mostly relying on reaction-time-based tasks.

Methods

A reward-based value-modulated attentional capture task was completed by a sample of nonclinical student participants—44 with high (HOC) and 48 with low (LOC) levels of OC symptoms. We measured the extent to which high and low reward-signaling distractors captured attention and impaired performance on the task, resulting in a lower possibility of obtaining a monetary reward. Attentional capture was indexed via fixation data, and further explored using saccade data.

Results

Both groups performed more poorly when a high-reward signaling distractor was present, compared to when a low-reward signaling distractor was present. Importantly, this difference was significantly greater in the HOC group, and was found to be driven by the specific effects of reward-signaling distractors. Similar results emerged when exploring saccade data, and remained significant after controlling for both addiction-related compulsivity and depressive symptoms.

Conclusions

Current findings suggest that attentional reward-related functioning may be associated with OC symptoms. Different aspects of reward functioning, including attention, should be further explored and incorporated into future research and clinical endeavors.

Type
Original Research
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) is a chronic and debilitating disorder with a lifetime prevalence ranging between 2% and 3%.Reference Ruscio, Stein, Chiu and Kessler 1 It is characterized by two main phenomena: obsessions, defined as recurrent, distressing, and intrusive thoughts or images, and/or compulsions, repetitive behaviors, or mental acts, performed in order to reduce obsession-related aversive emotions and anxiety. Accordingly to the traditional view of the disorder, the stereotypical portrait of an obsessive-compulsive (OC) individual is usually of an excessively self-controlled, risk-aversive person that acts in order to avoid potential loss or harm.Reference Rasmussen and Eisen 2

Although this traditional description fits well with studies showing increased harm avoidance in OCD,Reference Ecker and Gönner 3 - Reference Pietrefesa and Coles 6 more recent research has challenged this perspective, yielding results that conflict with the stereotypical portrait of OCD in three important domains: impulsivity, decision-making, and reward functioning.Reference Benatti, Dell’Osso, Arici, Hollander and Altamura 7 - Reference Grassi, Makris and Pallanti 12 First, research shows patients with OCD to display increased impulsivity,Reference Benatti, Dell’Osso, Arici, Hollander and Altamura 7 , Reference Ettelt, Ruhrmann and Barnow 8 , Reference Grassi, Pallanti and Righi 11 , Reference Grassi, Figee and Ooms 13 specifically highlighting cognitive and attentional impulsiveness in the disorder.Reference Grassi, Makris and Pallanti 12 , Reference Sohn, Kang, Namkoong and Kim 14 Second, impaired decision-making was found to characterize individuals with OCD, with both neurocognitive and clinical studies reporting risky decision-making in patients with OCD, compared with healthy non-OCD participants, namely, preference for an immediate reward despite negative future consequences.Reference Grassi, Pallanti and Righi 11 , Reference Grassi, Makris and Pallanti 12 , Reference Cavedini, Zorzi and Baraldi 15 Finally, recent research has also shown alterations in reward functioning in OCD, including, among others, reward anticipation (eg,Reference Figee, Vink and de Geus 10 , Reference Choi, Shin and Jung 16 ; for a review seeReference Figee, Pattij and Willuhn 9 ), reward valuation (eg, Reference Ferreira, Moreira and Magalhães 17 ), reward generalization (eg,Reference Rouhani, Wimmer, Schneier, Fyer, Shohamy and Simpson 18 ), and most relevant for this study—reward-based learning (eg,Reference Marsh, Tau and Wang 19 , Reference Remijnse, Nielen and van Balkom 20 ).

A major aspect that has been mostly overlooked by most research in the field of reward-based learning in OCD is that of attention. However, learning from an attentional perspective is an integral part of reward processing.Reference Le Pelley, Mitchell, Beesley, George and Wills 21 Moreover, as attention precedes behavior and guides higher thought processes, (working) memory, decision-making, and other higher order cognitive processes,Reference Feldmann-Wüstefeld, Busch and Schubö 22 , Reference Desimone and Duncan 23 it seems vital to better understand it, also in OCD. Theoretically, reward learning from an attentional perspective, termed reward-based Selection History, refers to the effects of prior reward-learning on subsequent attention allocation, or, put differently, to the way in which one’s learning of a (rewarding) value of specific stimuli affects the way attention is later allocated to those stimuli, compared to non or less-rewarded ones.Reference Awh, Belopolsky and Theeuwes 24 Research on reward-based selection history in healthy individuals has consistently shown that stimuli which gained a rewarding value can later guide visuospatial attention, even without conscious awareness.Reference Anderson 25 , Reference Failing and Theeuwes 26 Thus, exploring alterations in reward-based selection history in OCD may potentially shed much-needed light on mechanisms related to reward functioning in the disorder.

One well-established experimental paradigm used to examine the attentional manifestation of reward learning is the value-modulated attentional capture task (the VMAC taskReference Le Pelley, Pearson, Griffiths and Beesley 27 ) designed to assess the attentional manifestation of Sign-trackingthe process by which reward-predictive cues gain precedence in directing the organism’s behavior.Reference Boakes 28 , Reference Hearst and Jenkins 29 Briefly, the VMAC task is a visual-search task, in which participants are presented with a search display, comprised of six shapes arranged evenly around an imaginary circle—five circles (ie, nontarget shapes) and one diamond (ie, the target shape). Within each of the five circles appears a line tilted 45° to the left or right, while within the diamond a line-oriented horizontally or vertically appears. Participants need to report the orientation of the line within the diamond as quickly as possible by pressing a corresponding key. Importantly, one of the nontarget stimuli in each display signals the magnitude of the reward to be won (high versus low reward) on the trial. However, actual reward delivery is not contingent on these reward-signaling stimuli, but rather on correctly responding to the target. Moreover, any attentional capture by reward-signaling distractors impedes performance (slowing it down, increasing erroneous responses), thereby lowering the possibility of obtaining the reward. The VMAC task consistently shows reduced task performance when high-reward signaling cues are present, compared with when low-reward signaling cues are shown, presumably due to the former capturing participants’ attention to a higher extent, thereby inhibiting the search for the target.Reference Le Pelley, Pearson, Griffiths and Beesley 27 , Reference Anderson, Laurent and Yantis 30 , Reference Failing and Theeuwes 31

Most relevant to this study, recent studies using the VMAC task have reported a positive correlation between performance on the task and compulsivity, such that participants with higher compulsivity scores also show greater attentional capture by reward-signaling stimuli, even at the expense of procuring the reward itself.Reference Albertella, Chamberlain and Le Pelley 32 - Reference Albertella, Le Pelley and Chamberlain 34 While considerably advancing the knowledge in the field, this study elaborates on these previous studies in two important ways. First, prior studies used a reaction-time (RT)-based version of the task, and thus could not provide additional information as to the course of attention deployment before the moment of measurement.Reference Lazarov, Suarez-Jimenez and Tamman 35 , Reference Lazarov, Suarez-Jimenez, Zhu, Pine, Bar-Haim and Neria 36 For example, when responses are slow or incorrect, one cannot know the actual deployment of attention that led to this deficient performance—was attention captured by the reward signaling cue itself? was attention deployed to any of the nontarget shapes? Second, previous studies were correlative studies based on convenience samples, with no relevant inclusion/exclusion criteria implemented in a group-comparison design.

Aiming to elaborate on previous findings in the field, this study compared participants with high (HOC) and low (LOC) levels of OC symptoms as they completed an eye-tracking-based version of the VMAC task. On the basis of the above-described previous studies, we expected the HOC group to show a greater reward-related attentional capture as compared with the LOC group.

Method

Participants

Six hundred and sixty-seven undergraduate students were screened using the Obsessive-Compulsive Inventory-Revised (OCI-RReference Foa, Huppert and Leiberg 37 ). Those scoring at the top of the OCI-R distribution comprised the HOC group, contingent on having an OCI-R score > 27, a score that is well above the clinical cutoff score on this scale (OCI-R = 21Reference Foa, Huppert and Leiberg 37 ), denoting severe OCD.Reference Abramovitch, Abramowitz, Riemann and McKay 38 This enabled the enrollment of participants that most closely resemble the clinical population of interest. Importantly, only those scoring above 21 also on the day of their participation, held several weeks following the initial screening, were enrolled in the study. The LOC group consisted of those who scored at the bottom of the sampling pool, contingent on having an OCI-R score < 10 (as 15 is considered the cutoff score reflecting minimal OC symptomsReference Abramovitch, Abramowitz, Riemann and McKay 38 ). The final sample included 92 participants: Forty-four HOC participants (M age = 23.22 years, SD = 1.62, range = 20–28 years; 34 women), and 48 LOC participants (M age = 23.50 years, SD = 1.98, range = 20–31 years; 32 women). The study protocol was approved by the Research Ethics Council of Tel Aviv University. We only invited participants with normal or corrected-to normal vision, excluding usage of multi-focal eyewear to prevent eye-tracking calibration difficulties. All participants provided informed consent and received course credit for participation.

1.1. Measures

Obsessive-compulsive symptoms

OC symptoms were measured using the OCI-R,Reference Foa, Huppert and Leiberg 37 an 18-item self-report questionnaire assessing OC symptoms. Participants indicate their level of distress associated with each symptom on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 0 (not at all) to 4 (very much), resulting in a 0-to-72 total score. The OCI-R has been shown to have good validity, test-retest reliability, and internal consistency in both clinical,Reference Foa, Huppert and Leiberg 37 , Reference Lazarov, Liberman, Hermesh and Dar 39 , Reference Lazarov, Oren, Liberman, Gur, Hermesh and Dar 40 and nonclinical samples.Reference Hajcak, Huppert, Simons and Foa 41 , Reference Lazarov, Friedman, Comay, Liberman and Dar 42 Internal consistency in this study was 0.95.

Depression and anxiety symptoms

Depression and anxiety symptoms were measured using the depression and anxiety subscales of the Depression, Anxiety, and Stress Scales-21 (DASS-21Reference Lovibond and Lovibond 43 )—a 21-item self-report questionnaire assessing dimensional components of depression and anxiety. Each individual item is rated on a 4-point scale ranging from 0 (the item does not apply to me at all) to 3 (the item applies to me very much or most of the time), on which participants indicate how much each statement applied to him/her experience over the past week. The DASS-21 has been shown to have high reliability, validity, and internal consistency in both clinical and nonclinical groups.Reference Lovibond and Lovibond 43 - Reference Henry and Crawford 45 Internal consistency in this study was 0.92 and 0.86, for the depression and anxiety subscales, respectively.

Compulsivity-associated problems

Compulsivity-associated problems were measured using the recently developed Brief Assessment Tool for Compulsivity-Associated Problems (BATCAPReference Albertella, Le Pelley and Chamberlain 33 ). The BATCAP is a two-part questionnaire. In the first part, participants indicate whether they endorse any of a set of compulsivity-associated behaviors (ie, checking, washing, ordering, binge eating, alcohol use, gambling, and excessive internet use), as it pertains to the previous month. For each endorsed behavior, participants are then required to complete the second part of the BATCAP, comprised of six items measuring the severity of the compulsivity-related problem. Each item is rated on a 5-point scale, ranging from 0 (none/not at all) to 4 (extreme/constant), for a 0-to-24 total score. The BATCAP provides two sub-scores—one for OCD compulsivity-associated problems (ie, accumulative score of checking, washing, and ordering), and one for addiction compulsivity-associated problems (ie, binge eating, alcohol use, gambling, and excessive internet use). The two BATCAP subscales were found to be moderately to highly correlated with well-established measures of the corresponding disorders (ie, the Alcohol-Use Disorders Identification TestReference Saunders, Aasland, Babor, De La Fuente and Grant 46 and the OCI-RReference Albertella, Le Pelley and Chamberlain 33 , Reference Foa, Huppert and Leiberg 37 ). Internal consistency in this study was 0.91 and 0.96 for the BATCAP-addiction and BATCAP-OCD sub-scores, respectively.

1.2. The VMAC task

The current task was based on the additional singleton paradigm,Reference Failing, Nissens, Pearson, Le Pelley and Theeuwes 47 , Reference Theeuwes 48 incorporating elements from the version used by Albertella et al.Reference Albertella, Le Pelley and Chamberlain 33 when exploring the VMAC performance-compulsivity association. It was designed and executed using the Experiment Builder software (version 2.1.140; SR Research Ltd., Mississauga, Ontario, Canada).

Each trial comprised a search display (see Figure 1) consisting of six differently colored shapes, 310pxl in diameter—one diamond serving as the to-be-found target and five circles (ie, nontarget stimuli). Of the five nontarget circles, one was always colored in either red or blue, serving as the reward-signaling shape (ie, the distractor). Specifically, one of these two colors signaled a high-reward trial, while the other signaled a low-reward trial, with distractor color counterbalanced across participants. The colors of the other five shapes in each display (the diamond and the remaining four circles) were randomly sampled from a fixed set of colors without replacement (green, yellow, pink, brown, and cyan; seeReference Failing, Nissens, Pearson, Le Pelley and Theeuwes 47 ). The six shapes were presented at equal distances from the center of an imaginary circle (102pxl diameter) on a black background. Shape location was randomly determined per trial.

Figure 1. Trial sequence of the value-modulated attentional capture (VMAC) task. Each trial of the task began with a centrally located fixation-cross mandating a 1-s fixation for the search display to appear. The search display was then presented until a fixation ≥100 ms was registered within any of the six presented shapes, or after 1000 ms have elapsed. Next, the display blanked for 250 ms, followed by the feedback display, presented for 1250 ms. Participants were asked to locate the target “as accurately and quickly as possible” and then to fixate on it. One of the nontarget shapes (ie, the distractor) signaled the magnitude of the reward that could be earned, if participants fixated the target fast enough (ie, before 1000 ms elapsed). Depending on the color of this distractor (red or blue), participants received either a high or a low reward. Fixating on any of the four non-distractor shapes was considered an “error” response and no reward was delivered.

Each trial began with a centrally located fixation-cross mandating a 1-s fixation for the search display to appear, verifying that each trial began only when participants’ gaze was located at the screen’s center. The search display was then presented until a response was registered by the eye tracker or after 1000 ms elapsed with no response. A Response was defined as a fixation ≥100 ms made either on the target (ie, the diamond) or on any of the five circles—the reward-signaling distractor (ie, the red or blue circle) and the four nontarget shapes (the remaining colored circles). Next, the display blanked for 250 ms, followed by a 1250 ms feedback display. Participants were asked to locate the target “as accurately and quickly as possible” and fixate on it. See Figure 1 for an illustration of a single trial.

To gauge the location of each response, six areas of interest (AOIs), each with a diameter of 142pxl, were defined, one per shape. A response was deemed Correct when participants fixated on the target AOI. Correct responses on low-reward trials (ie, trials containing the distractor signaling low-reward) resulted in a “correct-1 point” feedback slide. Correct responses on high-reward trials (ie, trials containing the distractor signaling high-reward) resulted in “correct-10 points bonus trial!” slide. To encourage quick responses, a latency limit of 650 ms was set for correct responses. When correct responses were above this limit a feedback slide of “correct but too slow” appeared. These responses were still considered correct as participants located the target with no prior fixations on nontarget shapes. Conversely, a response was considered an Error if a fixation was registered within any of the five nontarget AOIs. This terminated the trial, omitting the possibility to obtain the reward, and resulting in an “error-0 points” feedback slide. Finally, as stated above, if no fixation was registered within any of the six AOIs before 1000 ms elapsed, a feedback slide reading “Too slow: please try to respond faster” was shown.

Participants first performed a no-reward 72-trial baseline block to get acquainted with the task and train on performing it. Here, the red and blue colored circles had no reward-signaling value, but rather served as ordinary nontarget shapes. Next, participants completed six 72-trial experimental blocks, as described above, for a total of 432 experimental trials. Before the experimental blocks, participants were told that they could now earn points depending on their performance, which would be converted to actual money (no information was given about how many points would be converted to how much money). Participants were also told that the presence of either a red or a blue circle in the display would indicate how many points could be earned (ie, high versus low reward) if they fixated on the target accurately and quickly enough, with no additional information. Half of the trials in each block featured the red distractor, and half the blue.

Before each block, a 5-point eye-tracking calibration followed by a 5-point validation procedure was performed. The block did not ensue unless a visual deviation <0.5° was achieved for each point on both the X- and Y-axes. A 2-minute break was given between blocks to reduce fatigue.

Eye-tracking apparatus

Eye-tracking data were collected and recorded using the remote head-free high-speed EyeLink Portable-Duo apparatus and the Experiment Builder software (SR-research, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada). Participants sat approximately 700 mm from the screen. Real-time monocular eye-tracking data were recorded continuously throughout the task at 500 Hz, with a 1920X1080-pixel display resolution. Eye-tracking data were processed using EyeLink Data Viewer software, version 3.1.246 (SR-research, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada). Fixations were defined as at least 100 ms of stable fixation within 1° visual angle.

Eye-tracking-based measures

Number of overall error responses was calculated per block, by counting the number of trials on which participants fixated any of the five nontarget AOIs, resulting in no reward. Number of distractor error responses was calculated per block, by counting the number of error trials resulting from specifically fixating the reward-signaling distractor. Finally, number of non-distractor error responses was calculated per block, by counting the number of error trials resulting from fixating one of the other nontarget shapes (excluding the reward-signaling distractor).

1.3. Procedure

Participants were tested individually in a quiet room at the university. They were seated in front of the eye-tracking monitor and were given the task instructions as described above. They were also informed that before the appearance of each display a fixation cross will appear at the screen’s center, on which they should fixate to make the display itself appear. Following the task participants filled out the self-report questionnaires. Then they were then thanked for participation, debriefed, and paid the monetary amount “won” during the task.

1.4. Data analysis

A sample of 82 has a power of 90% to detect a Group-by-Reward interaction at an alpha level of .05, of an effect size similar to that reported in previous studies using a similar version of the VMAC task (η2 p  = 0.12Reference Anderson, Faulkner, Rilee, Yantis and Marvel 49 ). Hence, 41 participants per group was determined as the minimum sample size. Power analysis was performed using G*Power 3.1.9.4Reference Faul, Erdfelder, Lang and Buchner 50 .

Independent sample t-tests compared between groups on age, OCI-R, DASS-21, and BATCAP scores, and a chi-square test compared groups on gender ratio.

We examined group differences in number of overall error responses, using a 2×6×2 mixed-model analysis of variance (ANOVA) with groups (HOC and LOC) as a between-subjects factor, and block (1–6) and reward (high, low) as within-subject factors. To further elucidate group differences on overall error responses and examine the specific effects of the reward-signaling distractors on task performance, we also compared groups on number of distractor-error responses, and number of non-distractor error responses. For significant findings, we performed an analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) entering both depression and BATCAP-addiction scores as covariates to the above-described main analyses. BATCAP-addiction scores were added to verify that emergent results are not due to addiction-related compulsivity, which was also found to be related to performance on the VMAC taskReference Albertella, Le Pelley and Chamberlain 33 , Reference Albertella, Le Pelley and Chamberlain 34 . Follow-up simple effects analyses were conducted using within-groups dependent-samples t-tests.

All statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS (IBM, version 27.0) and were two-sided, using an α of 0.05. Effect sizes are reported using η2 p for ANOVAs and Cohen’s d for group mean comparisons.

Results

Data are openly available in Open Science Foundation (OSF) at https://osf.io/n7sx5/?view_only=37e0c2d9de2f43cdaa57132ee803d81e

1.5. Demographic and clinical characteristics

Demographic and clinical characteristics of the two groups are described in Table 1. Significant group differences were noted on OCI-R scores, t(90) = 17.09, P < .000, Cohen’s d = 3.57, 95% CI [2.90, 4.22]; DASS-21 depression, t(89) = 5.87, P < .000, Cohen’s d = 1.23, 95% CI [0.78, 1.68] and anxiety scores, t(89) = 7.30, P < .000, Cohen’s d = 1.53, 95% CI [1.06, 1.99]. Groups also differed on BATCAP-OCD t(89) = 7.96, P < .000, Cohen’s d = 1.67, 95% CI [1.18, 2.15] and BATCAP-addiction t(89) = 4.94, P < .000, Cohen’s d = 1.04, 95% CI [0.59, 1.47]. No group differences were noted for age, t(90) = .72, P = .47, or gender ratio, $ \chi $ 2(1) = 1.27, P = .26.

Table 1. Demographic and Clinical Characteristics of the Two Groups

Note. Different superscripts signify differences between groups at P < .001.

OCI-R total: p < .001; DASS-21_depression: p < .001; DASS-21_anxiety: p < .001; BATCAP_OCD: p < .001; BATCAP_ADDICTION: p < .001.

Abbreviations: BATCAP, brief assessment tool for compulsivity associated problems; DASS-21, depression, anxiety and stress scales-21; HOC, high obsessive–compulsive tendencies; LOC, low obsessive–compulsive tendencies; OCI-R, obsessive–compulsive inventory-revised.

1.6. Main analysis

Whereas the omnibus Group×Block×Reward interaction for overall error responses was not significant, F(5, 83) = .72, P = .61, a significant Group×Reward interaction emerged F(1, 87) = 4.90, P = .03, η2 p  = .05 (Figure 2a), which remained significant following the ANCOVA, F(1,85) = 4.20, P = .04, η2 p  = .05. We therefore collapsed across blocks for the remaining analyses. Follow-up simple effects analysis showed that HOC participants fixated the nontarget shapes significantly more on high-reward trials (M = 17.14, SD = 11.87) than on low-reward trials (M = 11.31, SD = 8.81), t(41) = 3.64, P < .001, Cohen’s d = 0.56. LOC participants also fixated the nontarget shapes significantly more on high-reward trials (M = 10.46, SD = 6.78), than on low-reward trials (M = 8.46, SD = 6.44), t(46) = 2.52, P < .015, Cohen’s d = 0.37, but to a lesser extent.

Figure 2. Reward valence by Group (collapsed across blocks) for (a) Number of overall error responses; (b) Number of distractor error responses; (c) Number of non-distractor error responses. HOC, high obsessive-compulsive tendencies; LOC, low obsessive-compulsive tendencies; HR, high reward; LR, low reward.

For distractor error responses, the omnibus Group×Block×Reward was not significant, F(5, 83) = 1.06, P = .38. However, here, too, a significant Group×Reward interaction emerged F(1, 87) = 5.08, P = .03, η2 p  = .06 (see Figure 2b), which also remained significant following the ANCOVA, F(1,85) = 5.93, P = .02, η2 p  = .06. Simple effects analysis showed that HOC participants fixated the high-reward signaling distractor significantly more (M = 7.45, SD = 9.27) than the low-reward signaling distractor (M = 1.78, SD = 1.68), t(41) = 3.89, P < .000, Cohen’s d = 0.60. Although to a lesser extent, LOC participants also fixated on the high-reward signaling distractor significantly more times (M = 3.83, SD = 4.11) than the low-reward signaling distractor (M = 1.59, SD = 1.52), t(46) = 3.62, P < .001, Cohen’s d = 0.53.

For non-distractor error responses, the omnibus Group×Block×Reward interaction was nonsignificant, F(5, 83) = .68, P = .64, as was the Group×Reward interaction, F(1, 87) = .15, P = .70.

1.7. Exploratory analysis—Saccades data

To better understand the significant Group×Reward interaction for distractor error responses (Figure 2b), we also explored two measures of saccades (ie, rapid eye movements between two spatial locations). First, we examined the number of saccades ending within the distractor AOI, which reflects eye movements that ended within the distractor AOI, but that did not necessarily end with a fixationFootnote 1 . We then explored number of saccades ending within/nearest to the distractor AOI, which takes in to account eye movements that did not necessarily end within the distractor AOI, but closest to it (compared to all other AOIs), signaling an orientation response in the direction of the distractor AOIFootnote 2 . For both measures, we used the same analyses as described above for distractor error responses (ie, the Group-by-Reward interaction based on fixation data).

For number of saccades ending within the distractor AOI, a significant interaction emerged, F(1, 87) = 4.22, P = .04, η2 p  = .05 (Figure 3a), which became even more significant following the ANCOVA analysis, F(1,85) = 7.81, P = .006, η2 p  = .08. Simple effects analysis showed that HOC participants made significantly more saccades towards the high-value distractor (M = 18.40, SD = 15.31) than towards the low-value distractor (M = 4.00, SD = 2.75), t(41) = 6.06, P < .001, Cohen’s d = 0.93, as did LOC participants (high-value distractor; M = 12.55, SD = 11.35, low-value distractor; M = 3.96, SD = 2.82), t(46) = 5.29, P < .001, Cohen’s d = 0.77, but to a lesser extent.

Figure 3. Reward valence by Group for (a) Number of saccades ending within distractor AOIs (ie, reward-value signaling AOIs; high or low); and (b) Number of saccades ending within/nearest distractor AOIs (ie, reward-value signaling AOIs; high or low). HOC, high obsessive-compulsive tendencies; LOC, low obsessive-compulsive tendencies; HR, high reward; LR, low reward.

For number of saccades ending within/nearest to the distractor AOI, a significant interaction emerged, F(1, 87) = 4.82, P = .03, η2 p  = .05 (Figure 3b, which remained significant following the ANCOVA analysis, F(1,85) = 5.76, P = .02, η2 p  = .06. Simple effects analysis revealed that HOC participants made significantly more saccades toward the high-value distractor (M = 25.26, SD = 21.16) than the low-value distractor (M = 9.92, SD = 6.31), t(41) = 4.75, P < .001, Cohen’s d = 0.73, and to a higher extent compared to LOC participants (high-value distractor; M = 16.87, SD = 15.54, low-value distractor; M = 10.04, SD = 6.44), t(46) = 3.03, P < .004, Cohen’s d = 0.44.

Discussion

Building on recent research in OCD implicating alterations in reward functioning in the disorder,Reference Figee, Pattij and Willuhn 9 , Reference Figee, Vink and de Geus 10 , Reference Grassi, Makris and Pallanti 12 this study examined reward functioning in OCD from an attentional standpoint. Specifically, participants with high and low levels of OC symptoms completed an eye-tracking-based version of the VMAC task. We measured the extent to which reward-signaling distractors captured participants’ attention and impaired their performance, resulting in a lower possibility of obtaining a monetary reward. Results showed that while both groups performed more error responses on trials with a high-reward signaling distractor, compared to trials with a low-reward signaling distractor, this difference was significantly greater in the HOC group than in the LOC group. This results pattern was found to be driven by the specific effects of the reward-signaling distractors, with this finding emerging also when exploring saccade-based measures. Conversely, no group differences emerged for non-distractor error responses. Finally, controlling for both addiction-related compulsivity and depression symptoms did not change these results, strengthening the specificity of current results to OC symptoms.

Current findings are in line with previous studies using the VMAC task to explore reward-related attentional processes in healthy individuals.Reference Le Pelley, Pearson, Griffiths and Beesley 27 , Reference Failing, Nissens, Pearson, Le Pelley and Theeuwes 47 , Reference Pearson, Donkin, Tran, Most and Le Pelley 51 , Reference Pearson, Osborn, Whitford, Failing, Theeuwes and Le Pelley 52 Specifically, present results showed that across both groups, participants’ performance was poorer when high-reward signaling distractors were present, compared to when low-reward signaling distractors were present, which was mainly driven by the reward-signaling distractors (although, as stated above, this effect was stronger in the high OC group). These results replicate extensive prior research showing similar effects, thereby contributing to the establishment and validation of the current eye-tracking-based task as assessing reward-related visual attention capture.

Present results are also in line with the few studies that used the VMAC task to explore compulsivity,Reference Albertella, Chamberlain and Le Pelley 32 - Reference Albertella, Le Pelley and Chamberlain 34 that showed reward-related attentional capture to be associated with the severity of compulsivity. However, this study elaborates on previous research in several important ways. First, by using an eye-tracking-based task, rather than an RT-based one, we could explore attentional deployment before reaching/fixating the target, including both fixations and saccade data, pinpointing group differences in task performance to the reward-signaling distractors (while also eliminating the motor response-related effects). Relatedly, keypress indices of attention give rise to potential confounding elements related to the execution of the motor response (ie, key-presses), possibly obscuring the interpretation of emergent results.Reference Lazarov, Ben-Zion, Shamai, Pine and Bar-Haim 53 This is especially relevant given the high co-morbidity between OCD and depression,Reference Crino and Andrews 54 - Reference Nestadt, Samuels and Riddle 56 which is characterized by psychomotor slowness.Reference Degl’Innocenti, Ågren and Bäckman 57 - Reference Egeland, Rund and Sundet 59 This limitation was addressed by using an eye-tracking-based version of the task. Third, this study compared two groups of participants based on well-defined cutoff scores, rather than exploring correlations among non-selected participants. Fourth, this study was delivered in a controlled lab setting, rather than being delivered as an online procedure via Amazon Mechanical Turk,Reference Albertella, Chamberlain and Le Pelley 32 - Reference Albertella, Le Pelley and Chamberlain 34 eliminating concerns regarding data quality of online psychological-related research (eg, sample biases, reduced control and monitoring of data collection, and dropout ratesReference Hewson 60 , Reference Kraut, Olson, Banaji, Bruckman, Cohen and Couper 61 ). Finally, in prior research the signaling-reward distractors were also physically salient (ie, were the only colored stimulus in the display), and may have “popped out” capturing attention by involving bottom-up processes due to saliency, not necessarily related to their rewarding-signaling value.Reference Theeuwes 48 , Reference Theeuwes 62 - Reference Theeuwes, Kramer, Hahn, Irwin and Zelinsky 64 Conversely, here, all stimuli used in the search display of this study were uniquely colored, increasing our confidence in attributing emergent findings to the rewarding nature of the reward-signaling distractors, rather than to their physical saliency.

Present findings also echo previous research using the VMAC task to explore attentional capture by reward-signaling cues in addiction. Specifically, these studies show associations between value-modulated attentional capture and addiction disorder symptoms (eg, drug and alcohol misuse), with greater attentional capture toward reward-signaling distractors emerging in participants with addiction symptoms, compared to non-addictive participants.Reference Anderson, Faulkner, Rilee, Yantis and Marvel 49 , Reference Albertella, Copeland, Pearson, Watson, Wiers and Le Pelley 65 , Reference Albertella, Watson, Yücel and Le Pelley 66 Considering these findings in addiction, conjointly with present findings and the (above-cited) research on VMAC performance and compulsivity, may lend some support to the viewpoint of OCD as a behavioral addiction,Reference Denys, Zohar and Westenberg 67 - Reference Robbins and Clark 69 specifically to the mutual dimension of alterations in reward functioning, for example, Reference Figee, Pattij and Willuhn 9 , Reference Figee, Vink and de Geus 10 , Reference Grassi, Makris and Pallanti 12 , Reference Choi, Shin and Jung 16 , Reference Ferreira, Yücel, Dawson, Lorenzetti and Fontenelle 70 Yet, the fact that present results remained significant after controlling for addiction-related compulsivity also suggests a specific effect for OCD-related compulsivity, beyond that of addiction. Still, future research could more directly compare the performance of participants with high OC symptoms to that of participants with addictive symptoms.

This study has several limitations that need to be acknowledged. First, the study did not include participants with clinically diagnosed OCD, but a sample of participants with high levels of OC symptoms. Still, we used a high OCI-R cutoff score of 27, reflecting severe OCD,Reference Abramovitch, Abramowitz, Riemann and McKay 38 as our inclusion criterion. Also, OCI-R scores were assessed twice, once during initial participant screening and once on the day of study participation, to verify score stability. Finally, using subclinical samples with high scores on measures of OCD has been shown to be relevant to the understanding of the disorder (for a review seeReference Abramowitz, Fabricant, Taylor, Deacon, McKay and Storch 71 ), and proven useful in previous research conducted in our laboratory, in which results were later successfully replicated in clinical samples.Reference Lazarov, Liberman, Hermesh and Dar 39 , Reference Lazarov, Oren, Liberman, Gur, Hermesh and Dar 40 Still, future studies should replicate this study using patients with clinically diagnosed OCD. Second, while sign-tracking behaviors are known to be more resistant to extinction, compared to goal-tracking behaviors,Reference Ahrens, Singer, Fitzpatrick, Morrow and Robinson 72 - Reference Yager and Robinson 75 the present procedure did not include an extinction block (ie, a block in which distractors no longer predict reward value). Hence, this claim could not be explored. As this was the first study to use the VMAC task among participants with high levels of OC symptoms, we opted to focus on the more basic VMAC effect. Still, future studies may add an extinction block to examine the VMAC effect under extinction conditions. Finally, in the VMAC paradigm rewards are response-dependent, as only correct responses to the target elicit reward delivery. Conversely, in animal studies rewards are response-independent in that they are delivered non-contingent on a specific response (ie, the food will always be delivered). Thus, one could argue that the VMAC task does not entail “pure” sign-tracking.Reference Colaizzi, Flagel, Joyner, Gearhardt, Stewart and Paulus 73 While we chose to use a well-established VMAC task, future studies could better tap pure sign-tracking by using a paradigm in which reward-paired stimuli are both task-irrelevant and response-independent.Reference Colaizzi, Flagel, Joyner, Gearhardt, Stewart and Paulus 73 , Reference Bucker and Theeuwes 76

Conclusion

To conclude, current findings propose that reward-related aberrant attentional functioning may be related to OC symptoms, findings that need to be further elaborated to clinically diagnosed patients with OCD. From a therapeutic standpoint, this may suggest to acknowledge reward functioning, in addition to the traditional anxiety-avoidance aspect of OCD, as part of the psychoeducation phase in current OCD treatments, thereby expanding the patient’s understanding of his condition. This may include, for example, an educational reframing of compulsive behaviors not merely as related to obsession-related anxiety, but also to reward-related processes. Specifically, conceptualizing compulsive behaviors as gaining rewarding value through the processes of negative reinforcement—when reducing obsession-related anxiety and/or distress, or when yielding “just right” experiences, resulting in an addictive-like dependency upon these behaviors.Reference Denys 77 This reframing may help patients make sense of their compulsions and decrease negative feelings, such as shame and self-criticism, that usually accompany their self-experience. More speculatively, future research could also explore the extent in which modifying reward-related attentional processes may alleviate OCD and decrease symptoms.Reference Habedank, Lennartz, Arslan and Ertle 78 - Reference Rouel and Smith 80

Acknowledgments

We greatly appreciate and thank the participants who participated in this study.

Funding Statement

This research was supported by the Israel Science Foundation, Grant number 374/20. The funding organization had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Data Availability Statement

The data that support the findings of this study are openly available in Open Science Foundation (OSF) at https://osf.io/n7sx5/?view_only=37e0c2d9de2f43cdaa57132ee803d81e.

Disclosure

Dana Basel and Amit Lazarov do not have any known conflicts of interest associated with this publication.

Footnotes

1 This measure includes both saccades that ended with a fixation (and hence were included in the main analysis) and those that did not.

2 This measure includes both saccades that ended within the distractor AOI (also included in the first saccade analysis) and saccades that ended nearest to the distractor AOI (compared to all other AOIs).

References

Ruscio, AM, Stein, DJ, Chiu, WT, Kessler, RC. The epidemiology of obsessive-compulsive disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Mol Psychiatry. 2010;15(1):5363.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rasmussen, SA, Eisen, JL. The course and clinical features of obsessive-compulsive disorder. In: Neuropsychopharmacology-5th Generation of Progress. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott, Williams & Wilkins; 2002:15931607.Google Scholar
Ecker, W, Gönner, S. Incompleteness and harm avoidance in OCD symptom dimensions. Behav Res Ther. 2008;46(8):895904.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ettelt, S, Grabe, HJ, Ruhrmann, S, et al. Harm avoidance in subjects with obsessive-compulsive disorder and their families. J Affect Disord. 2008;107(1–3):265269.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, SJ, Kang, JI, Kim, C-H. Temperament and character in subjects with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Compr Psychiatry. 2009;50(6):567572.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pietrefesa, AS, Coles, ME. Moving beyond an exclusive focus on harm avoidance in obsessive-compulsive disorder: behavioral validation for the separability of harm avoidance and incompleteness. Behav Ther. 2009;40(3):251259.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benatti, B, Dell’Osso, B, Arici, C, Hollander, E, Altamura, AC. Characterizing impulsivity profile in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Int J Psychiatry Clin Pract. 2014;18(3):156160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ettelt, S, Ruhrmann, S, Barnow, S, et al. Impulsiveness in obsessive-compulsive disorder: results from a family study. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2007;115(1):4147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figee, M, Pattij, T, Willuhn, I, et al. Compulsivity in obsessive-compulsive disorder and addictions. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2016;26(5):856868.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figee, M, Vink, M, de Geus, F, et al. Dysfunctional reward circuitry in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2011;69(9):867874.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grassi, G, Pallanti, S, Righi, L, et al. Think twice: impulsivity and decision-making in obsessive-compulsive disorder. J Behav Addict. 2015;4(4):263272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grassi, G, Makris, N, Pallanti, S. Addicted to compulsion: assessing three core dimensions of addiction across obsessive-compulsive disorder and gambling disorder. CNS Spectr. 2020;25(3):392401.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grassi, G, Figee, M, Ooms, P, et al. Impulsivity and decision-making in obsessive-compulsive disorder after effective deep brain stimulation or treatment as usual. CNS Spectr. 2018;23(5):333339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sohn, SY, Kang, JI, Namkoong, K, Kim, SJ. Multidimensional measures of impulsivity in obsessive-compulsive disorder: cannot wait and stop. PLoS One. 2014;9(11):e111739.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cavedini, P, Zorzi, C, Baraldi, C, et al. The somatic marker affecting decisional processes in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2012;17(2):177190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Choi, J-S, Shin, Y-C, Jung, WH, et al. Altered brain activity during reward anticipation in pathological gambling and obsessive-compulsive disorder. PLoS One. 2012;7:e45938.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferreira, S, Moreira, P, Magalhães, R, et al. Frontoparietal hyperconnectivity during cognitive regulation in obsessive-compulsive disorder followed by reward valuation inflexibility. J Psychiatr Res. 2021;137:657666.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rouhani, N, Wimmer, GE, Schneier, FR, Fyer, AJ, Shohamy, D, Simpson, HB. Impaired generalization of reward but not loss in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Depress Anxiety. 2019;36(2):121129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marsh, R, Tau, GZ, Wang, Z, et al. Reward-based spatial learning in unmedicated adults with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2015;172(4):383392.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Remijnse, PL, Nielen, MM, van Balkom, AJ, et al. Reduced orbitofrontal-striatal activity on a reversal learning task in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63(11):12251236.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Le Pelley, ME, Mitchell, CJ, Beesley, T, George, DN, Wills, AJ. Attention and associative learning in humans: an integrative review. Psychol Bull. 2016;142(10):1111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feldmann-Wüstefeld, T, Busch, NA, Schubö, A. Failed suppression of salient stimuli precedes behavioral errors. J Cogn Neurosci. 2019;32(2):367377.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Desimone, R, Duncan, J. Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annu Rev Neuroscience. 1995;18(1):193222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Awh, E, Belopolsky, AV, Theeuwes, J. Top-down versus bottom-up attentional control: a failed theoretical dichotomy. Trends Cogn Sci. 2012;16(8):437443.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, BA. The attention habit: how reward learning shapes attentional selection. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2016;1369(1):2439.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Failing, M, Theeuwes, J. Selection history: how reward modulates selectivity of visual attention. Psychon Bull Rev. 2018;25(2):514538.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Le Pelley, ME, Pearson, D, Griffiths, O, Beesley, T. When goals conflict with values: counterproductive attentional and oculomotor capture by reward-related stimuli. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2015;144(1):158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boakes, RA. Performance on learning to associate a stimulus with positive reinforcement. In: Operant-Pavlovian Interactions. 1st ed. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum; 1977:35.Google Scholar
Hearst, E, Jenkins, HM Sign-tracking: the stimulus-reinforcer relation and directed action. In: HearstSign-Tracking: The Stimulus-Reinforcer Relation and Directed Action1974. Austin: The Psychonomic Society; 1974.Google Scholar
Anderson, BA, Laurent, PA, Yantis, S. Value-driven attentional capture. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2011;108(25):1036710371.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Failing, M, Theeuwes, J. Don’t let it distract you: how information about the availability of reward affects attentional selection. Atten Percept Psychophys. 2017;79(8):22752298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Albertella, L, Chamberlain, SR, Le Pelley, ME, et al. Compulsivity is measurable across distinct psychiatric symptom domains and is associated with familial risk and reward-related attentional capture. CNS Spectr. 2020;25(4):519526.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Albertella, L, Le Pelley, ME, Chamberlain, SR, et al. Reward-related attentional capture is associated with severity of addictive and obsessive-compulsive behaviors. Psychol Addict Behav. 2019;33(5):495.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Albertella, L, Le Pelley, ME, Chamberlain, SR, et al. Reward-related attentional capture and cognitive inflexibility interact to determine greater severity of compulsivity-related problems. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2020;69:101580.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lazarov, A, Suarez-Jimenez, B, Tamman, A, et al. Attention to threat in posttraumatic stress disorder as indexed by eye-tracking indices: a systematic review. Psychol Med. 2019;49(5):705726.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lazarov, A, Suarez-Jimenez, B, Zhu, X, Pine, DS, Bar-Haim, Y, Neria, Y. Attention allocation in posttraumatic stress disorder: an eye-tracking study. Psychol Med. 2021:110.Google ScholarPubMed
Foa, EB, Huppert, JD, Leiberg, S, et al. The obsessive-compulsive inventory: development and validation of a short version. Psychol Assess. 2002;14(4):485.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Abramovitch, A, Abramowitz, JS, Riemann, BC, McKay, D. Severity benchmarks and contemporary clinical norms for the obsessive-compulsive inventory-revised (OCI-R). J Obsessive Compuls Relat Disord. 2020;27:100557.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lazarov, A, Liberman, N, Hermesh, H, Dar, R. Seeking proxies for internal states in obsessive-compulsive disorder. J Abnorm Psychol. 2014;123(4):695.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lazarov, A, Oren, E, Liberman, N, Gur, S, Hermesh, H, Dar, R. Attenuated access to emotions in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behav Ther. 2022;53:110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hajcak, G, Huppert, JD, Simons, RF, Foa, EB. Psychometric properties of the OCI-R in a college sample. Behav Res Ther. 2004;42(1):115123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lazarov, A, Friedman, A, Comay, O, Liberman, N, Dar, R. Obsessive-compulsive symptoms are related to reduced awareness of emotional valence. J Affect Disord. 2020;272:2837.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lovibond, PF, Lovibond, SH. The structure of negative emotional states: comparison of the depression anxiety stress scales (DASS) with the beck depression and anxiety inventories. Behav Res Ther. 1995;33(3):335343.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Antony, MM, Bieling, PJ, Cox, BJ, Enns, MW, Swinson, RP. Psychometric properties of the 42-item and 21-item versions of the depression anxiety stress scales in clinical groups and a community sample. Psychol Assess. 1998;10(2):176. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.10.2.176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henry, JD, Crawford, JR. The short-form version of the depression anxiety stress scales (DASS-21): construct validity and normative data in a large nonclinical sample. Br J Clin Psychol. 2005;44(2):227239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saunders, JB, Aasland, OG, Babor, TF, De La Fuente, JR, Grant, M. Development of the alcohol use disorders identification test (AUDIT): WHO collaborative project on early detection of persons with harmful alcohol consumption-II. Addiction. 1993;88(6):791804.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Failing, M, Nissens, T, Pearson, D, Le Pelley, M, Theeuwes, J. Oculomotor capture by stimuli that signal the availability of reward. J Neurophysiol. 2015;114(4):23162327.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Theeuwes, J. Perceptual selectivity for color and form. Percept Psychophys. 1992;51(6):599606.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, BA, Faulkner, ML, Rilee, JJ, Yantis, S, Marvel, CL. Attentional bias for nondrug reward is magnified in addiction. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2013;21(6):499.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Faul, F, Erdfelder, E, Lang, A-G, Buchner, A. G* Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods. 2007;39(2):175191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pearson, D, Donkin, C, Tran, SC, Most, SB, Le Pelley, ME. Cognitive control and counterproductive oculomotor capture by reward-related stimuli. Vis Cogn. 2015;23(1–2):4166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pearson, D, Osborn, R, Whitford, TJ, Failing, M, Theeuwes, J, Le Pelley, ME. Value-modulated oculomotor capture by task-irrelevant stimuli is a consequence of early competition on the saccade map. Atten Percept Psychophys. 2016;78(7):22262240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lazarov, A, Ben-Zion, Z, Shamai, D, Pine, DS, Bar-Haim, Y. Free viewing of sad and happy faces in depression: a potential target for attention bias modification. J Affect Disord. 2018;238:94100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crino, RD, Andrews, G. Obsessive-compulsive disorder and axis I comorbidity. J Anxiety Disord. 1996;10(1):3746.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hong, JP, Samuels, J, Bienvenu, OJ III, et al. Clinical correlates of recurrent major depression in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Depress Anxiety. 2004;20(2):8691.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nestadt, G, Samuels, J, Riddle, M, et al. The relationship between obsessive-compulsive disorder and anxiety and affective disorders: results from the Johns Hopkins OCD Family Study. Psychol Med. 2001;31(3):481487. doi:10.1017/S0033291701003579.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Degl’Innocenti, A, Ågren, H, Bäckman, L. Executive deficits in major depression. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1998;97(3):182188.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Den Hartog, H, Derix, M, Van Bemmel, A, Kremer, B, Jolles, J. Cognitive functioning in young and middle-aged unmedicated out-patients with major depression: testing the effort and cognitive speed hypotheses. Psychol Med. 2003;33(8):14431451.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Egeland, J, Rund, B, Sundet, K, et al. Attention profile in schizophrenia compared with depression: differential effects of processing speed, selective attention and vigilance. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2003;108(4):276284.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hewson, C. Conducting research on the internet. Psychologist. 2003;16(6):290293.Google Scholar
Kraut, R, Olson, J, Banaji, M, Bruckman, A, Cohen, J, Couper, M. Psychological research online: report of Board of Scientific Affairs’ Advisory Group on the Conduct of Research on the Internet. Am Psychol. 2004;59(2):105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Theeuwes, J. Stimulus-driven capture and attentional set: selective search for color and visual abrupt onsets. J Exp Psychol: Hum Percept Perform. 1994;20(4):799. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.20.4.799.Google ScholarPubMed
Theeuwes, J. Top–down and bottom–up control of visual selection. Acta Psychol. 2010;135(2):7799.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Theeuwes, J, Kramer, AF, Hahn, S, Irwin, DE, Zelinsky, GJ. Influence of attentional capture on oculomotor control. J Exp Psychol: Hum Percept Perform. 1999;25(6):1595.Google ScholarPubMed
Albertella, L, Copeland, J, Pearson, D, Watson, P, Wiers, RW, Le Pelley, ME. Selective attention moderates the relationship between attentional capture by signals of nondrug reward and illicit drug use. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2017;175:99105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Albertella, L, Watson, P, Yücel, M, Le Pelley, ME. Persistence of value-modulated attentional capture is associated with risky alcohol use. Addict Behav Rep. 2019;10:100195.Google ScholarPubMed
Denys, D, Zohar, J, Westenberg, H. The role of dopamine in obsessive-compulsive disorder: preclinical and clinical evidence. J Clin Psychiatry. 2004;65(Suppl 14):1117.Google ScholarPubMed
Holden, C. ‘Behavioral’ addictions: do they exist? Science. 2001;294:980982.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robbins, TW, Clark, L. Behavioral addictions. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2015;30:6672.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferreira, GM, Yücel, M, Dawson, A, Lorenzetti, V, Fontenelle, LF. Investigating the role of anticipatory reward and habit strength in obsessive-compulsive disorder. CNS Spectr. 2017;22(3):295304.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Abramowitz, JS, Fabricant, LE, Taylor, S, Deacon, BJ, McKay, D, Storch, EA. The relevance of analogue studies for understanding obsessions and compulsions. Clin Psychol Rev. 2014;34(3):206217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ahrens, AM, Singer, BF, Fitzpatrick, CJ, Morrow, JD, Robinson, TE. Rats that sign-track are resistant to Pavlovian but not instrumental extinction. Behav Brain Res. 2016;296:418430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colaizzi, JM, Flagel, SB, Joyner, MA, Gearhardt, AN, Stewart, JL, Paulus, MP. Mapping sign-tracking and goal-tracking onto human behaviors. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2020;111:8494.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saunders, BT, Robinson, TE. Individual variation in resisting temptation: implications for addiction. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2013;37(9):19551975.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yager, LM, Robinson, TE. Cue-induced reinstatement of food seeking in rats that differ in their propensity to attribute incentive salience to food cues. Behav Brain Res. 2010;214(1):3034.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bucker, B, Theeuwes, J. Stimulus-driven and goal-driven effects on Pavlovian associative reward learning. Vis Cogn. 2018;26(2):131148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Denys, D. Obsessionality & compulsivity: a phenomenology of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Philos Ethics Humanit Med. 2011;6(1):17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Habedank, I, Lennartz, SJ, Arslan, RC, Ertle, A. Online attention bias modification for obsessive-compulsive disorder: a randomized controlled trial. 2017. https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/U7CVE CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Najmi, S, Amir, N. The effect of attention training on a behavioral test of contamination fears in individuals with subclinical obsessive-compulsive symptoms. J Abnorm Psychol. 2010;119(1):136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rouel, M, Smith, E. Attentional bias and its modification in contamination OCD symptomatology. Cognit Ther Res. 2018;42(5):686698.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Trial sequence of the value-modulated attentional capture (VMAC) task. Each trial of the task began with a centrally located fixation-cross mandating a 1-s fixation for the search display to appear. The search display was then presented until a fixation ≥100 ms was registered within any of the six presented shapes, or after 1000 ms have elapsed. Next, the display blanked for 250 ms, followed by the feedback display, presented for 1250 ms. Participants were asked to locate the target “as accurately and quickly as possible” and then to fixate on it. One of the nontarget shapes (ie, the distractor) signaled the magnitude of the reward that could be earned, if participants fixated the target fast enough (ie, before 1000 ms elapsed). Depending on the color of this distractor (red or blue), participants received either a high or a low reward. Fixating on any of the four non-distractor shapes was considered an “error” response and no reward was delivered.

Figure 1

Table 1. Demographic and Clinical Characteristics of the Two Groups

Figure 2

Figure 2. Reward valence by Group (collapsed across blocks) for (a) Number of overall error responses; (b) Number of distractor error responses; (c) Number of non-distractor error responses. HOC, high obsessive-compulsive tendencies; LOC, low obsessive-compulsive tendencies; HR, high reward; LR, low reward.

Figure 3

Figure 3. Reward valence by Group for (a) Number of saccades ending within distractor AOIs (ie, reward-value signaling AOIs; high or low); and (b) Number of saccades ending within/nearest distractor AOIs (ie, reward-value signaling AOIs; high or low). HOC, high obsessive-compulsive tendencies; LOC, low obsessive-compulsive tendencies; HR, high reward; LR, low reward.