Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-cphqk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-01-31T01:36:35.009Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The association between recent stressful life events and brain structure: a UK Biobank longitudinal MRI study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 January 2025

Cheryl R.Z. See*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, UK
Annabel X. Tan
Affiliation:
Department of Epidemiology and Population Health, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA
Lucia R. Valmaggia
Affiliation:
Centre for Youth Mental Health, University of Melbourne, Parkville, VIC, Australia Department of Psychology, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, UK Department of Psychiatry, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
Matthew J. Kempton
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, UK
*
Corresponding author: Cheryl R.Z. See; Email: [email protected]

Abstract

Background

Recent stressful life events (SLEs) are an established risk factor for a range of psychiatric disorders. Animal studies have shown evidence of gray matter (GM) reductions associated with stress, and previous work has found similar associations in humans. However longitudinal studies investigating the association between stress and changes in brain structure are limited.

Methods

The current study uses longitudinal data from the UK Biobank and comprises 4,543 participants with structural neuroimaging and recent SLE data (mean age = 61.5 years). We analyzed the association between recent SLEs and changes in brain structure, determined using the longitudinal FreeSurfer pipeline, focusing on total GM volume and five a priori brain regions: the hippocampus, amygdala, anterior cingulate cortex, orbitofrontal cortex, and insula. We also examined if depression and childhood adversity moderated the relationship between SLEs and brain structure.

Results

Individuals who had experienced recent SLEs exhibited a slower rate of hippocampal decrease over time compared to individuals who did not report any SLEs. Individuals with depression exhibited smaller GM volumes when exposed to recent SLEs. There was no effect of childhood adversity on the relationship between SLEs and brain structure.

Conclusions

Our findings suggest recent SLEs are not directly associated with an accelerated decline in brain volumes in a population sample of older adults, but instead may alter brain structure via affective disorder psychopathology. Further work is needed to investigate the effects of stress in younger populations who may be more vulnerable to stress-induced changes, and may yet pinpoint brain regions linked to stress-related disorders.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of European Psychiatric Association

Introduction

Stressful life events (SLEs) are a recognized risk factor for a range of disorders including depression, psychosis, and infectious illnesses [Reference Cohen, Murphy and Prather1]. The brain is central to responding to external stressors and regulating the biological stress response [Reference McEwen2]. Findings from animal studies have suggested prolonged exposure to stress can cause reductions in gray matter (GM) volumes within the brain in areas such as the hippocampus, cingulate cortex, and prefrontal regions [Reference Kassem, Lagopoulos, Stait-Gardner, Price, Chohan and Arnold3Reference Lee, Jarome, Li, Kim and Helmstetter5]. This has been attributed to the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis, the main biological system that secretes glucocorticoids in response to stress, which is thought to be neurotoxic at sustained levels [Reference Dedovic, D’Aguiar and Pruessner6, Reference McEwen, Nasca and Gray7]. It is posited this could in part be due to excitotoxicity, where the over-stimulation of cells via glutamate receptors are further exacerbated by elevated glucocorticoids, resulting in neuronal damage [Reference Reagan and McEwen8, Reference Popoli, Yan, McEwen and Sanacora9].

Cross-sectional structural neuroimaging studies have reported associations between recent SLEs in adulthood and smaller gray matter (GM) volumes within regions including the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), insula, prefrontal cortex, hippocampus, and amygdala in healthy adults [Reference Ringwald, Meller, Schmitt, Andlauer, Stein and Brosch10Reference Ansell, Rando, Tuit, Guarnaccia and Sinha13]. To clarify the direction of effect, studies using longitudinal data are required, of which we are aware of only two in non-clinical samples. Papagni et al. [Reference Papagni, Benetti, Arulanantham, McCrory, McGuire and Mechelli14] (N = 26) found reductions in the hippocampus, parahippocampus, and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) associated with SLEs that occurred over a three-month period. Ringwald et al. [Reference Ringwald, Pfarr, Stein, Brosch, Meller and Thomas-Odenthal15] (N = 212) found a negative association between SLEs and GM volume changes over a 2-year follow-up period within the medial prefrontal cortex. These findings indicate that recent SLEs may have a detectable effect on macroscopic brain structure.

The current study investigated the effects of recent SLEs on brain structure using a large, longitudinal, population dataset from the UK Biobank (https://www.ukbiobank.ac.uk). Previous work examining the neural correlates of lifetime adulthood stress and early life adversity using cross-sectional UK Biobank data has been carried out by McManus et al. [Reference McManus, Haroon, Duncan, Elliott and Muhlert16], where the authors did not find a significant association with GM volumes in their hypothesized regions-of-interest (ROIs): the hippocampus, amygdala, and thalamus. Here, we sought to investigate whether recent SLEs are distinctly associated with brain structural changes in this population sample. We have focused on total GM volume and five subregions of the brain: the hippocampus, amygdala, ACC, orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), and insula. These regions have previously been implicated in the regulation of the stress response [Reference McEwen, Nasca and Gray7], with evidence of structural neuroanatomical changes within these regions associated with stress in non-clinical adult samples [Reference Ringwald, Meller, Schmitt, Andlauer, Stein and Brosch10Reference Ringwald, Pfarr, Stein, Brosch, Meller and Thomas-Odenthal15, Reference Li, Li, Wei, Chen, Jackson and Zhang17]. These brain regions have also been observed to exhibit aberrant functional connectivity associated with stress in healthy adults [Reference Wheelock, Rangaprakash, Harnett, Wood, Orem and Mrug18, Reference Henze, Konzok, Kreuzpointner, Bartl, Peter and Giglberger19], and in animal studies [Reference Dopfel and Zhang20]. One study further observed persistent anomalous resting-state connectivity in rodents a week after being exposed to a stressor within the prefrontal cortex and amygdala [Reference Liang, King and Zhang21].

We have analyzed a subset of UK Biobank participants who had available structural neuroimaging and recent SLE data measured at two timepoints. We categorized participants into two groups based on whether they had any or no recent SLE exposure and examined for group differences in brain structure. We hypothesized that individuals exposed to recent SLEs would have smaller brain volumes and lower cortical thickness, and that changes in their brain structure over the follow-up period would be more adversely affected when compared to individuals without recent SLE exposure. Among individuals with recent SLE exposure, we hypothesized that brain structural measures would be negatively associated with the number of events.

Methods

UK Biobank data

The UK Biobank is a population-based cohort of over 500,000 participants from across the United Kingdom (https://www.ukbiobank.ac.uk), recruited between the ages of 40 and 69 [Reference Sudlow, Gallacher, Allen, Beral, Burton and Danesh22]. Recruitment began in 2006, and baseline data were collected covering an extensive range of variables relating to health and wellbeing, sociodemographic measures, and lifestyle. There have since been three follow-up assessments, where imaging data were collected in the latter two [Reference Littlejohns, Holliday, Gibson, Garratt, Oesingmann and Alfaro-Almagro23]. Between 2014 and 2020, participants completed their first MRI scan, while data collection for the second MRI scan occurred between 2019 and 2022. At each follow-up, participants who completed the MRI scan also completed the main assessment suite on the same day, which included recent SLE data and depressive symptom data. The current study used data from participants who had complete structural MRI data and recent SLE data at both imaging visits (N = 4,543). In between assessments, participants were also invited to complete one-off online questionnaires such as the 2016 Mental Health Questionnaire (see Supplementary Figure S1 for a timeline illustrating data collection). The current study obtained only childhood adversity data from the Mental Health Questionnaire. Supplementary Figure S2 presents a flow chart depicting sample sizes of the analyses and a list of the variables used is reported in Supplementary Table S1.

The UK Biobank obtained ethical approval from the Research Ethics Committee (Ref:11/NW/0382), and participants provided written, informed consent. Data in the current study (application ID: 87152) were retrieved from the UK Biobank in July 2023.

Recent stressful life events

At each imaging assessment, participants were asked if they had experienced any SLEs within the last 2 years (Data-Field 6145). Participants selected events from a pre-specified list of six events which included: a serious illness, injury or assault to self or to a close relative, death of a close relative or spouse/partner, marital separation/divorce, or financial difficulties. We calculated an SLE score based on the number of events (0–6). Participants were assigned group membership at each timepoint to either SLE−, for scores of zero, or SLE+, for scores greater than zero. In our analyses, we compared brain structural measures between the two groups, and we also examined the association between the SLE score and brain structure within the SLE+ group.

Neuroimaging measures

The UK Biobank’s MRI acquisition protocol and quality control have been previously described [Reference Alfaro-Almagro, Jenkinson, Bangerter, Andersson, Griffanti and Douaud24]. Participants were scanned at four centers (Cheadle, Reading, Newcastle, and Bristol) using the same scanner model (3 T Siemens Skyra). At the time of data retrieval in the current study, there were no repeat imaging scans completed at Bristol, and therefore only data from three centers have been included.

T1-weighted scans from both timepoints were processed using the longitudinal stream in the software FreeSurfer (v7.3.2) (https://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu) [Reference Reuter, Schmansky, Rosas and Fischl25], which has demonstrated reliable structural measurements for longitudinal neuroimaging analysis [Reference Hedges, Dimitrov, Zahid, Brito Vega, Si and Dickson26]. Segmented regions were derived based on the Desikan–Killiany Atlas [Reference Desikan, Ségonne, Fischl, Quinn, Dickerson and Blacker27]. In the current study, we focused on global total GM volume and five brain regions that have been previously associated with recent stress in healthy adults: (1) hippocampus [Reference Papagni, Benetti, Arulanantham, McCrory, McGuire and Mechelli14, Reference Gianaros, Jennings, Sheu, Greer, Kuller and Matthews28, Reference Ganzel, Kim, Glover and Temple29]; (2) amygdala [Reference Sublette, Galfalvy, Oquendo, Bart, Schneck and Arango12, Reference Ganzel, Kim, Glover and Temple29]; (3) OFC [Reference Ringwald, Meller, Schmitt, Andlauer, Stein and Brosch10, Reference Ansell, Rando, Tuit, Guarnaccia and Sinha13]; (4) ACC [Reference Kuhn, Scharfenort, Schumann, Schiele, Munsterkotter and Deckert11, Reference Ansell, Rando, Tuit, Guarnaccia and Sinha13, Reference Papagni, Benetti, Arulanantham, McCrory, McGuire and Mechelli14]; and (5) insula [Reference Ansell, Rando, Tuit, Guarnaccia and Sinha13, Reference Li, Li, Wei, Chen, Jackson and Zhang17]. Results from the FreeSurfer processing were assessed following the ENIGMA Quality Control (QC) Protocol (https://enigma.ini.usc.edu/protocols/imaging-protocols/), where seven participants were excluded due to poor data quality. Further details are reported in the supplement.

For the subcortical regions, the hippocampus and amygdala, we analyzed bilateral GM volumes summing left and right volume measures as obtained from FreeSurfer. For the cortical regions, the OFC, ACC, and insula, we analyzed the mean cortical thickness, which was calculated by averaging the FreeSurfer thickness estimates across hemispheres for each region.

Other non-imaging variables

Time

The time between assessments was considered as the time from the first imaging assessment and calculated using the assessment date (Data-Field 53) for each participant. Time at the first imaging assessment was therefore zero across all participants. The time to the second imaging assessment was calculated in days by subtracting assessment dates and dividing by 365 to convert it to years.

Depressive symptoms

Recent depressive symptoms were measured using the total score of the Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ)-2 (Data-Fields 2050–2080), the depression subscale of the PHQ-4 [Reference Kroenke, Spitzer, Williams and Löwe30, Reference Lowe, Wahl, Rose, Spitzer, Glaesmer and Wingenfeld31]. We selected the PHQ-2 as these data were collected on the same day as the MRI scans and were the most complete measure of psychopathology. As a large number of participants scored zero, indicating no recent depressive symptoms, we grouped participants based on the established PHQ-2 cut-off score, where scores of ≥ 3 indicated probable depression (PHQ+) and < 3 indicated no probable depression (PHQ−) [Reference Kroenke, Spitzer, Williams and Löwe30].

Childhood adversity

Participants completed the Childhood Trauma Screener (CTS-5) [Reference Grabe, Schulz, Schmidt, Appel, Driessen and Wingenfeld32] as part of the online 2016 Mental Health Questionnaire (Data-Fields 20489–20491), and a total CTS-5 score was calculated. Not all participants completed the online assessment, which was issued between follow-up assessments, and 1,219 participants were missing data. As just over half of the participants reported experiencing no childhood adversity, we created a childhood adversity (CA) grouping where participants were assigned membership based on whether they experienced any (CA+) or no childhood adversity (CA−).

Sociodemographic variables

Other variables used in the current study as potential confounders included: employment status (Data-Field 6142), the presence of a long-standing illness, disability, or infirmity (Data-Field 2188), alcohol intake frequency (Data-Field 1558), smoking status (Data-Field 20116), and the Townsend deprivation index (Data-Field 22189). The Townsend deprivation index measures socioeconomic deprivation, with higher scores indicating higher deprivation [Reference Townsend, Phillimore and Beattie33]. Further details regarding the treatment of the variables are reported in the supplement.

Statistical analysis

All analyses were conducted in R (v4.3.1), with the statistical significance level set at p < 0.05 (two-tailed).

Sample characteristics of the SLE− and SLE+ groups were compared using independent sample t-tests or chi-square tests as appropriate, using data from the first imaging assessment or at recruitment.

Our primary analysis was to compare brain structural measures between the groups SLE+ and SLE−. We employed linear mixed models (LMM), using the R package lme4 [Reference Bates, Mächler, Bolker and Walker34], and participants were modeled with random intercepts to account for the repeated measures. LMMs do not require data to be measured at consistent time intervals making it suitable for analyzing longitudinal data [Reference Detry and Ma35]. We fitted separate LMMs with each brain structural measure as the outcome variable, and SLE group, time, and the interaction term SLE group × time as the main fixed effects. The interaction term allowed us to examine whether there were group differences in brain structural changes over the study period. Time was measured in years from the date of the first imaging assessment.

In an additional analysis we fitted LMMs using SLE score with data from only the SLE+ group, to examine whether the number of SLEs were associated with changes in brain structure. Separate LMMs were modeled for each brain structural measure as the outcome variable, and with SLE score, time, and SLE score × time as the main fixed effects.

In exploratory analysis, we investigated whether recent SLEs influenced the relationship between depression and brain structure, given the strong evidence linking recent stress and the onset of depressive disorders [Reference Stroud, Davila and Moyer36, Reference Tennant37]. We fitted LMMs with depression group (PHQ+ or PHQ−), SLE group, and the interaction term SLE group × depression group as the main fixed effects, controlling for time. We also considered the effects of childhood adversity as it has been associated with smaller brain volumes [Reference Begemann, Schutte, van Dellen, Abramovic, Boks and van Haren38, Reference Calem, Bromis, McGuire, Morgan and Kempton39], and is linked to an increased sensitivity to stress in later life, potentially amplifying the effects of stress in adulthood [Reference Duprey, Handley, Manly, Cicchetti and Toth40, Reference Bandoli, Campbell-Sills, Kessler, Heeringa, Nock and Rosellini41]. To examine for the effects of childhood adversity, we fitted LMMs to include CA group (CA+ or CA−), SLE group, and the interaction term CA group × SLE group as the main fixed effects, controlling for time.

In all models, where the interaction term was not significant, we re-fitted the models excluding the interaction term to report the fixed effects of the variables of interest [Reference Engqvist42]. All models were adjusted for age, age2 (where age was taken at the first imaging assessment), sex, total intracranial volume (ICV), and scan center, included as fixed effects. Age and total ICV were standardized to avoid varying scales across covariates affecting model convergence [Reference Bates, Mächler, Bolker and Walker34]. Neuroimaging and SLE data were used across both timepoints in all models.

To adjust for multiple comparisons, we used a 5% false discovery rate (FDR) correction inclusive of the main and exploratory analyses (51 p-values). The p-values reported in the results section are uncorrected, with a superscript indicating whether significant p-values had passed correction.

We conducted several sensitivity analyses to test for changes to the significance of our results in our main SLE group analysis. Firstly, we excluded data from participants who had experienced a stroke in their lifetime (n = 52), and who had outlier total ICV (n = 36), defined in the ENIGMA QC protocol as 2.698 standard deviations above or below the sample mean ICV. Next, we adjusted the models for potential confounding sociodemographic variables, which were found to be different between SLE groups (see Table 1): employment status, the presence of a long-standing illness, disability or infirmity, alcohol intake frequency, smoking status, and the Townsend deprivation index. Finally, we re-fit the models to include a broader range of neuroimaging confounders as identified by Alfaro-Almagro et al. [Reference Alfaro-Almagro, McCarthy, Afyouni, Andersson, Bastiani and Miller43], which included non-linear terms for time, age and sex interactions, and head motion measures. Further details are provided in the supplement.

Table 1. Sample characteristics of participants at the first imaging assessment (n = 4,543)

Abbreviations: SLE, stressful life event; SLE−, no recent SLE; SLE+, one or more recent SLE; CA+, any childhood adversity; PHQ+, probable depression based on the Patient Health Questionnaire-4 depression subscale.

a Townsend deprivation index measures the socioeconomic deprivation of a participant’s census area (based on postcode) at point of recruitment. Higher scores indicate higher socioeconomic deprivation.

* p < 0.05.

** p < 0.01.

Whole-brain exploratory analysis

We conducted a final exploratory analysis looking at group differences in structural measures across all FreeSurfer regions in the brain between SLE+ and SLE−. This analysis was to provide further insight into potential stress-affected brain regions separate from our analytical plan detailed above. Using LMMs with SLE group as the main fixed effect, and controlling for time, we examined left and right cortical thickness and surface area measures for a total of 68 regions, and left and right subcortical volumes for a total of 18 regions. Results were corrected for multiple comparisons using a 5% FDR correction.

Results

Sample characteristics

The current study used 4,543 participants from the UK Biobank who had available neuroimaging and recent SLE data at both imaging assessments. Sample characteristics and group differences between SLE+ and SLE- at the first imaging assessment are reported in Table 1. The SLE+ group were younger, consisted of more females, were more likely to be in employment, consumed less alcohol, more likely to be current smokers, and lived in more socioeconomically deprived areas. More SLE+ individuals reported having a long-standing illness, disability or infirmity and depression, and had a mean SLE score of 1.26 (SD = 0.53) at the first imaging assessment. The frequencies of SLE types are reported in Supplementary Table S2. All participants completed two imaging assessments with a mean time of 2.65 years (SD = 1.09; Range 1.00–7.34 years) between assessments.

Associations between recent SLEs and brain structure

The estimates of the main effects of SLE group and time are reported in Table 2 for the LMMs fitted for each brain structure. Where the interaction term for SLE group × time was not significant, the reported coefficient estimates are from the models where we have excluded the interaction term.

Table 2. Fixed effect estimates from the linear mixed models comparing brain structure between participants who have or have not reported recent stressful life events

Abbreviations: SLE, stressful life events.

The linear mixed models included participants (n = 4,543) modeled as random intercepts. All models controlled for sex, age at first imaging assessment, age2, total intracranial volume, and scan centre.

a Reference category for SLE group is SLE– (coded as 0) comprising individuals who have not reported a recent SLE.

b Where the interaction term of SLE group × time was not significant, the fixed effects from the model excluding the interaction term are reported. Only the hippocampus estimates are from the model that included the interaction term. Full results are present in Supplementary Table S3.

* p < 0.05.

** p < 0.01.

Findings survived false discovery rate correction for multiple comparisons (p-values reported are uncorrected).

Only the hippocampus revealed a significant SLE group × time interaction, where hippocampal volumes decreased over time at a slower rate in the SLE+ group as compared to the SLE− group (seen by the different slopes in Figure 1). In all other brain regions, SLE group did not have a significant effect, suggesting there was no difference in brain structure between SLE+ and SLE− when controlling for time. All brain regions, except for the ACC, reported a significant effect of time, exhibiting a reduction in GM volumes and in mean cortical thickness over the follow-up period. The full model results are reported in Supplementary Table S3.

Figure 1. The predicted marginal effects from the linear mixed model showing different trajectories of the SLE− and SLE+ groups for hippocampal volume changes over the study follow-up period. The shaded regions represent the 95% confidence interval. Abbreviations: SLE, stressful life event; SLE−, individuals who reported no recent SLEs; SLE+, individuals who reported one or more recent SLE.

When we examined for associations between the SLE score and brain structure within the SLE+ group, we did not find any significant effect of the interaction term SLE score × time, nor of the SLE score as a main effect (see Supplementary Table S4). This suggests the number of events was not associated with brain structure.

Exploratory analyses of the effects of depression and childhood adversity

The results from the exploratory analysis investigating the effects of depression group (PHQ+ or PHQ–) are reported in Table 3. The interaction term SLE group × depression group only had a significant effect on total GM volume. Individuals with probable depression exhibited smaller total GM volumes where they reported recent SLEs, compared to those who did not report any recent SLEs (Figure 2). We also observed a significant interaction effect of SLE group × depression group on hippocampal volumes, however this did not pass correction (pcorrected = 0.070). The full model estimates are reported in Supplementary Table S5.

Table 3. Fixed effect estimates from the linear mixed models examining the association between brain structure and recent stressful life events and depression

Abbreviations: PHQ, Patient Health Questionnaire; SLE, stressful life events.

The linear mixed models included participants (n = 4,506) modeled as random intercepts. All models controlled for time (in years from first imaging assessment), sex, age at first imaging assessment, age2, total intracranial volume, and scan centre.

a Reference category for SLE group is SLE– (coded as 0), comprising individuals who have not reported a recent SLE.

b Reference category for the depression group is PHQ– (coded as 0) comprising individuals who are unlikely to have a depressive disorder based on the cut-off score (<3) for the PHQ-2.

c Where the interaction term of SLE group × depression group was not significant, the fixed effects from the model excluding the interaction term are reported. Only total GMV and the hippocampus estimates are from the model that included the interaction term. Full results are present in Supplementary Table S5.

* p < 0.05.

** p < 0.01.

Findings survived false discovery rate correction for multiple comparisons (p-values reported are uncorrected).

Figure 2. The predicted marginal effects from the linear mixed model showing the differences in total GM volumes between depression groups depending on recent SLE exposure (SLE group). The error bars represent the 95% confidence interval. Abbreviations: GM, gray matter; SLE, stressful life event; SLE−, individuals who reported no recent SLEs; SLE+, individuals who reported one or more recent SLE; PHQ+, individuals with probable depression; PHQ−, individuals without probable depression.

We did not find a significant interaction between SLE group and CA group associated with brain structure, nor was there a main effect of CA group on brain structure in the subsequent models excluding the interaction term (all p > 0.05). Our results indicate there were no differences in brain structure between individuals who had experienced childhood adversity and those who had not. The full model estimates are reported in Supplementary Table S6.

Sensitivity analyses

There were no changes to the significance of our results when we excluded individuals who reported having a stroke in their lifetime or outlier total ICV, nor when we included the lifestyle and sociodemographic variables as covariates of no interest. When we expanded our model to include the additional neuroimaging confounders, the interaction between SLE group × time associated with the hippocampus was no longer significant.

Whole-brain exploratory analysis

The results of the whole-brain analysis are reported in the Supplementary Table S7 for group differences between SLE+ and SLE−. None of the findings survived correction for multiple comparisons.

Discussion

We investigated the effects of recent SLEs on brain structure using a longitudinal neuroimaging dataset from a large population cohort. The hippocampus exhibited a slower decline in GM volume over the study period in individuals with recent SLE exposure compared to those without recent SLE exposure. In exploratory analysis, total GM volume differed between SLE exposure groups in individuals with depression but not in non-depressed individuals. We found childhood adversity had no effect on the relationship between recent SLEs and brain structure.

Our results exhibited a decrease in hippocampal volumes with time, which is expected in terms of aging-related changes [Reference MacDonald and Pike44]. However, contrary to our expectations, the SLE+ group exhibited a slower rate of volume reduction over the follow-up period. The hippocampus is highly plastic, and while this may make it a region of vulnerability in many disorders [Reference Bartsch and Wulff45], hippocampal GM volume reduction might be countered through mental stimulation, exercise, or social interaction [Reference Fotuhi, Do and Jack46Reference Phillips50], which may serve as protective factors. This may explain our findings, as a higher proportion of the SLE+ group were still in employment, which could suggest higher mental stimulation, and were found to consume less alcohol, a risk factor associated with brain shrinkage [Reference Yang, Wen, Erus, Govindarajan, Melhem and Mamourian51, Reference Daviet, Aydogan, Jagannathan, Spilka, Koellinger and Kranzler52]. However, there were no changes to our results when we controlled for employment status and alcohol intake in a sensitivity analysis. The interaction between SLE group and time was no longer significant when we expanded our model to include a wider set of neuroimaging confounders. However, the difference in the rate of hippocampal volume change between SLE groups was very subtle, estimated to be slower by 0.007 ml/year in the SLE+ group (see the interaction term of SLE group × time in Table 2).

It is possible that stress may pose more of a risk at a younger age, given the global median age of onset for stress-related disorders was found to be below 35 years [Reference Solmi, Radua, Olivola, Croce, Soardo and Salazar de Pablo53]. Neuroimaging studies investigating stress in older cohorts (>60 years) are limited. One longitudinal study, in a depressed and non-depressed sample (N = 159, mean age = 70 years), found that SLEs were associated with larger hippocampal volumes at baseline but there was no evidence of a temporal association between SLEs and brain structure [Reference Zannas, McQuoid, Payne, Steffens, MacFall and Ashley-Koch54]. Another cross-sectional study (N = 466, mean age = 71 years) reported that SLEs that occurred over the age of 65 were associated with greater amygdala volumes, but not with hippocampal volumes [Reference Gerritsen, Kalpouzos, Westman, Simmons, Wahlund and Backman55]. Previous longitudinal studies by Papagni et al. [Reference Papagni, Benetti, Arulanantham, McCrory, McGuire and Mechelli14] and Ringwald et al. [Reference Ringwald, Pfarr, Stein, Brosch, Meller and Thomas-Odenthal15] that found significant associations between recent SLEs and changes in brain structure analyzed younger samples with mean ages of 25.2 years (N = 26) and 32.8 years (N = 212) respectively, suggesting stress-induced changes could be more prominent in younger populations.

Stress is subjective to an individual’s experience, and it may be the perception of stressful events that is more relevant to structural brain changes rather than the occurrence of an event. Previous cross-sectional work has reported associations between higher perceived stress levels and smaller GM volumes within the prefrontal cortex [Reference Moreno, Bruss and Denburg56] and insula [Reference Li, Li, Wei, Chen, Jackson and Zhang17], and a longitudinal study has suggested that a smaller hippocampus represents a vulnerability to stress [Reference Lindgren, Bergdahl and Nyberg57]. There is also some evidence of rumination being associated with larger GM volumes within the prefrontal cortex and ACC [Reference Demnitz-King, Goehre and Marchant58], which may have affected our results. As such, future studies could incorporate subjective measures of stress and rumination.

We found total GM volume differed between depressed individuals with and without recent SLE exposure, but not in nondepressed individuals. Stress is linked to the onset of depression [Reference Stroud, Davila and Moyer36, Reference Tennant37] and severe subclinical depressive symptoms have been associated with smaller GM volumes [Reference Besteher, Gaser and Nenadic59, Reference Schrader, Meller, Evermann, Pfarr and Nenadic60]. Our findings indicate that recent stress may influence the association between depressive symptoms and total GM volume as has been previously reported [Reference Frodl, Koutsouleris, Bottlender, Born, Jager and Scupin61]. However, further work is required to clarify the direction of effect as smaller GM volumes have been associated with major depressive disorder in non-stress studies [Reference Schmaal, Veltman, van Erp, Samann, Frodl and Jahanshad62, Reference Gray, Muller, Eickhoff and Fox63]. In addition, affective-disorder psychopathology could result in an individual becoming susceptible to SLEs [Reference Alloy, Liu and Bender64, Reference Liu65], subsequently leading to further harmful effects. We did not observe any effects of childhood adversity on the association between recent SLEs and brain structure. However, as childhood adversity is thought to increase sensitivity to stress [Reference Duprey, Handley, Manly, Cicchetti and Toth40, Reference Bandoli, Campbell-Sills, Kessler, Heeringa, Nock and Rosellini41], perceived stress levels may be more relevant in this context.

The current study had several limitations. Firstly, the questionnaire capturing recent SLEs was limited to six events. While these events are found in other validated life event questionnaires [Reference Norbeck66, Reference Brugha and Cragg67], they did not capture other event types such as having serious problems with a friend or being the victim of theft. The questionnaire also did not facilitate the reporting of multiple events of the same type, meaning the data may have underreported the number of SLEs. The UK Biobank data comprised individuals from mainly white European ethnic backgrounds (97% of the current sample), and older adults, affecting the generalizability of results to other racial and ethnic groups. In addition, we may be observing a survival bias in the study sample as participants have actively participated in repeated data collection, which could indicate that they are overall healthier and perhaps more resilient to stress. The time between assessments varied with some participants completing a follow-up assessment more than 2 years after their first imaging assessment. It is, therefore, possible that SLEs with potentially impactful or lasting effects may have occurred outside of the 2-year period defining a recent SLE, and were therefore not accounted for in the study. Future study design using experience sampling methods to record daily stressors over a shorter period may be an alternative approach to capture an individual’s experience of stress.

In conclusion, using longitudinal neuroimaging data from a large population cohort, we have found that recent SLEs may not accelerate brain structure reductions in older adults, but may influence changes through affective disorder psychopathology. Further research is needed to uncover the effects of stress on the general population, with a particular focus on younger populations, who may be more vulnerable to stress-induced changes. This work may yet pinpoint vulnerable brain regions linked to stress-related disorders.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1192/j.eurpsy.2025.2.

Data availability statement

Data are available via the UK Biobank (https://www.ukbiobank.ac.uk).

Acknowledgments

This work uses data provided by patients and collected by the NHS as part of their care and support. We are grateful to the UK Biobank participants for their commitment and contribution, and the UK Biobank team for managing the data. This research has been conducted using the UK Biobank Resource under Application Number 87152.

Author contribution

C.S. conceptualized the study with M.K. and L.V. C.S. managed the project administration of the database, developed the methodology, performed the data processing and analysis, and prepared the original draft of the manuscript and carried out all revisions under the supervision of M.K. and L.V. A.T. reviewed the statistical approach. M.K., L.V., and A.T. reviewed and edited the manuscript. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript.

Financial support

C. See is supported by the UK Medical Research Council (MR/N013700/1) and King’s College London member of the MRC Doctoral Training Partnership in Biomedical Sciences.

Ethics approval

The UK Biobank has ethics approval from the North West Multi-centre Research Ethics Committee (Ref:11/NW/0382), which was renewed in 2021. Further details are provided on the UK Biobank website (https://www.ukbiobank.ac.uk).

Competing interest

The authors declare no competing interests.

References

Cohen, S, Murphy, MLM, Prather, AA. Ten surprising facts about stressful life events and disease risk. Annu Rev Psychol. 2019;70:577–97. https://doi/org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010418-102857Google Scholar
McEwen, BS. Neurobiological and systemic effects of chronic stress. Chron Stress (Thousand Oaks). 2017;1. https://doi/org/10.1177/2470547017692328Google Scholar
Kassem, MS, Lagopoulos, J, Stait-Gardner, T, Price, WS, Chohan, TW, Arnold, JC, et al. Stress-induced grey matter loss determined by MRI is primarily due to loss of dendrites and their synapses. Mol Neurobiol. 2013;47(2):645–61. https://doi/org/10.1007/s12035-012-8365-7Google Scholar
Hei, M, Chen, P, Wang, S, Li, X, Xu, M, Zhu, X, et al. Effects of chronic mild stress induced depression on synaptic plasticity in mouse hippocampus. Behav Brain Res. 2019;365:2635. https://doi/org/10.1016/j.bbr.2019.02.044Google Scholar
Lee, T, Jarome, T, Li, SJ, Kim, JJ, Helmstetter, FJ. Chronic stress selectively reduces hippocampal volume in rats: a longitudinal magnetic resonance imaging study. Neuroreport. 2009;20(17):1554–8. https://doi/org/10.1097/WNR.0b013e328332bb09Google Scholar
Dedovic, K, D’Aguiar, C, Pruessner, JC. What stress does to your brain: a review of neuroimaging studies. Can J Psychiatry. 2009;54(1):615. https://doi/org/10.1177/070674370905400104Google Scholar
McEwen, BS, Nasca, C, Gray, JD. Stress effects on neuronal structure: hippocampus, amygdala, and prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2016;41(1):323. https://doi/org/10.1038/npp.2015.171Google Scholar
Reagan, LP, McEwen, BS. Controversies surrounding glucocorticoid-mediated cell death in the hippocampus. J Chem Neuroanat. 1997;13(3):149–67. https://doi/org/10.1016/s0891-0618(97)00031-8Google Scholar
Popoli, M, Yan, Z, McEwen, BS, Sanacora, G. The stressed synapse: the impact of stress and glucocorticoids on glutamate transmission. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2011;13(1):2237. https://doi/org/10.1038/nrn3138Google Scholar
Ringwald, KG, Meller, T, Schmitt, S, Andlauer, TFM, Stein, F, Brosch, K, et al. Interaction of developmental factors and ordinary stressful life events on brain structure in adults. Neuroimage Clin. 2021;30:102683. https://doi/org/10.1016/j.nicl.2021.102683Google Scholar
Kuhn, M, Scharfenort, R, Schumann, D, Schiele, MA, Munsterkotter, AL, Deckert, J, et al. Mismatch or allostatic load? Timing of life adversity differentially shapes gray matter volume and anxious temperament. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2016;11(4):537–47. https://doi/org/10.1093/scan/nsv137Google Scholar
Sublette, ME, Galfalvy, HC, Oquendo, MA, Bart, CP, Schneck, N, Arango, V, et al. Relationship of recent stress to amygdala volume in depressed and healthy adults. J Affect Disord. 2016;203:136–42. https://doi/org/10.1016/j.jad.2016.05.036Google Scholar
Ansell, EB, Rando, K, Tuit, K, Guarnaccia, J, Sinha, R. Cumulative adversity and smaller gray matter volume in medial prefrontal, anterior cingulate, and insula regions. Biol Psychiatry. 2012;72(1):5764. https://doi/org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.11.022Google Scholar
Papagni, SA, Benetti, S, Arulanantham, S, McCrory, E, McGuire, P, Mechelli, A. Effects of stressful life events on human brain structure: a longitudinal voxel-based morphometry study. Stress. 2011;14(2):227–32. https://doi/org/10.3109/10253890.2010.522279Google Scholar
Ringwald, KG, Pfarr, JK, Stein, F, Brosch, K, Meller, T, Thomas-Odenthal, F, et al. Association between stressful life events and grey matter volume in the medial prefrontal cortex: a 2-year longitudinal study. Hum Brain Mapp. 2022;43(11):3577–84. https://doi/org/10.1002/hbm.25869Google Scholar
McManus, E, Haroon, H, Duncan, NW, Elliott, R, Muhlert, N. The effects of stress across the lifespan on the brain, cognition and mental health: a UK biobank study. Neurobiol Stress. 2022;18:100447. https://doi/org/10.1016/j.ynstr.2022.100447Google Scholar
Li, H, Li, W, Wei, D, Chen, Q, Jackson, T, Zhang, Q, et al. Examining brain structures associated with perceived stress in a large sample of young adults via voxel-based morphometry. Neuroimage. 2014;92:17. https://doi/org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.01.044Google Scholar
Wheelock, MD, Rangaprakash, D, Harnett, NG, Wood, KH, Orem, TR, Mrug, S, et al. Psychosocial stress reactivity is associated with decreased whole-brain network efficiency and increased amygdala centrality. Behav Neurosci. 2018;132(6):561–72. https://doi/org/10.1037/bne0000276Google Scholar
Henze, GI, Konzok, J, Kreuzpointner, L, Bartl, C, Peter, H, Giglberger, M, et al. Increasing deactivation of limbic structures over psychosocial stress exposure time. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2020;5(7):697704. https://doi/org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2020.04.002Google Scholar
Dopfel, D, Zhang, N. Mapping stress networks using functional magnetic resonance imaging in awake animals. Neurobiol Stress. 2018;9:251–63. https://doi/org/10.1016/j.ynstr.2018.06.002Google Scholar
Liang, Z, King, J, Zhang, N. Neuroplasticity to a single-episode traumatic stress revealed by resting-state fMRI in awake rats. Neuroimage. 2014;103:485–91. https://doi/org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.08.050Google Scholar
Sudlow, C, Gallacher, J, Allen, N, Beral, V, Burton, P, Danesh, J, et al. UK biobank: an open access resource for identifying the causes of a wide range of complex diseases of middle and old age. PLoS Med. 2015;12(3):e1001779. https://doi/org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1001779Google Scholar
Littlejohns, TJ, Holliday, J, Gibson, LM, Garratt, S, Oesingmann, N, Alfaro-Almagro, F, et al. The UK Biobank imaging enhancement of 100,000 participants: rationale, data collection, management and future directions. Nat Commun. 2020;11(1):2624. https://doi/org/10.1038/s41467-020-15948-9Google Scholar
Alfaro-Almagro, F, Jenkinson, M, Bangerter, NK, Andersson, JLR, Griffanti, L, Douaud, G, et al. Image processing and Quality Control for the first 10,000 brain imaging datasets from UK Biobank. Neuroimage. 2018;166:400–24. https://doi/org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.10.034Google Scholar
Reuter, M, Schmansky, NJ, Rosas, HD, Fischl, B. Within-subject template estimation for unbiased longitudinal image analysis. Neuroimage. 2012;61(4):1402–18. https://doi/org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.084Google Scholar
Hedges, EP, Dimitrov, M, Zahid, U, Brito Vega, B, Si, S, Dickson, H, et al. Reliability of structural MRI measurements: the effects of scan session, head tilt, inter-scan interval, acquisition sequence, FreeSurfer version and processing stream. Neuroimage. 2022;246:118751. https://doi/org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118751Google Scholar
Desikan, RS, Ségonne, F, Fischl, B, Quinn, BT, Dickerson, BC, Blacker, D, et al. An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. NeuroImage. 2006;31(3):968–80. https://doi/org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021Google Scholar
Gianaros, PJ, Jennings, JR, Sheu, LK, Greer, PJ, Kuller, LH, Matthews, KA. Prospective reports of chronic life stress predict decreased grey matter volume in the hippocampus. Neuroimage. 2007;35(2):795803. https://doi/org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.10.045Google Scholar
Ganzel, BL, Kim, P, Glover, GH, Temple, E. Resilience after 9/11: multimodal neuroimaging evidence for stress-related change in the healthy adult brain. Neuroimage. 2008;40(2):788–95. https://doi/org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.12.010Google Scholar
Kroenke, K, Spitzer, RL, Williams, JBW, Löwe, B. An ultra-brief screening scale for anxiety and depression: the PHQ–4. Psychosomatics. 2009;50(6):613–21. https://doi/org/10.1016/s0033-3182(09)70864-3Google Scholar
Lowe, B, Wahl, I, Rose, M, Spitzer, C, Glaesmer, H, Wingenfeld, K, et al. A 4-item measure of depression and anxiety: validation and standardization of the patient health questionnaire-4 (PHQ-4) in the general population. J Affect Disord. 2010;122(1–2):8695. https://doi/org/10.1016/j.jad.2009.06.019Google Scholar
Grabe, HJ, Schulz, A, Schmidt, CO, Appel, K, Driessen, M, Wingenfeld, K, et al. A brief instrument for the assessment of childhood abuse and neglect: the childhood trauma screener (CTS). Psychiatr Prax. 2012;39(3):109–15. https://doi/org/10.1055/s-0031-1298984Google Scholar
Townsend, P, Phillimore, P, Beattie, A. Health and deprivation: inequality and the north. London: Routledge; 1988.Google Scholar
Bates, D, Mächler, M, Bolker, B, Walker, S. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J Stat Soft. 2015;67(1). https://doi/org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01Google Scholar
Detry, MA, Ma, Y. Analyzing repeated measurements using mixed models. JAMA. 2016;315(4):407–8. https://doi/org/10.1001/jama.2015.19394Google Scholar
Stroud, CB, Davila, J, Moyer, A. The relationship between stress and depression in first onsets versus recurrences: a meta-analytic review. J Abnorm Psychol. 2008;117(1):206–13. https://doi/org/10.1037/0021-843X.117.1.206Google Scholar
Tennant, C. Life events, stress and depression: a review of recent findings. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2002;36(2):173–82. https://doi/org/10.1046/j.1440-1614.2002.01007.xGoogle Scholar
Begemann, MJH, Schutte, MJL, van Dellen, E, Abramovic, L, Boks, MP, van Haren, NEM, et al. Childhood trauma is associated with reduced frontal gray matter volume: a large transdiagnostic structural MRI study. Psychol Med. 2023;53(3):741–9. https://doi/org/10.1017/S0033291721002087Google Scholar
Calem, M, Bromis, K, McGuire, P, Morgan, C, Kempton, MJ. Meta-analysis of associations between childhood adversity and hippocampus and amygdala volume in non-clinical and general population samples. Neuroimage Clin. 2017;14:471–9. https://doi/org/10.1016/j.nicl.2017.02.016Google Scholar
Duprey, EB, Handley, ED, Manly, JT, Cicchetti, D, Toth, SL. Child maltreatment, recent stressful life events, and suicide ideation: a test of the stress sensitivity hypothesis. Child Abuse Negl. 2021;113:104926. https://doi/org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2020.104926Google Scholar
Bandoli, G, Campbell-Sills, L, Kessler, RC, Heeringa, SG, Nock, MK, Rosellini, AJ, et al. Childhood adversity, adult stress, and the risk of major depression or generalized anxiety disorder in US soldiers: a test of the stress sensitization hypothesis. Psychol Med. 2017;47(13):2379–92. https://doi/org/10.1017/S0033291717001064Google Scholar
Engqvist, L. The mistreatment of covariate interaction terms in linear model analyses of behavioural and evolutionary ecology studies. Anim Behav. 2005;70(4):967–71. https://doi/org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.01.016Google Scholar
Alfaro-Almagro, F, McCarthy, P, Afyouni, S, Andersson, JLR, Bastiani, M, Miller, KL, et al. Confound modelling in UK Biobank brain imaging. Neuroimage. 2021;224:117002. https://doi/org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117002Google Scholar
MacDonald, ME, Pike, GB. MRI of healthy brain aging: a review. NMR Biomed. 2021;34(9):e4564. https://doi/org/10.1002/nbm.4564Google Scholar
Bartsch, T, Wulff, P. The hippocampus in aging and disease: From plasticity to vulnerability. Neuroscience. 2015;309:116. https://doi/org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.07.084Google Scholar
Fotuhi, M, Do, D, Jack, C. Modifiable factors that alter the size of the hippocampus with ageing. Nat Rev Neurol. 2012;8(4):189202. https://doi/org/10.1038/nrneurol.2012.27Google Scholar
Fotuhi, M, Lubinski, B, Trullinger, M, Hausterman, N, Riloff, T, Hadadi, M, et al. A personalized 12-week “Brain Fitness Program” for improving cognitive function and increasing the volume of hippocampus in elderly with mild cognitive impairment. J Prev Alzheimers Dis. 2016;3(3):133–7. https://doi/org/10.14283/jpad.2016.92Google Scholar
Kobe, T, Witte, AV, Schnelle, A, Lesemann, A, Fabian, S, Tesky, VA, et al. Combined omega-3 fatty acids, aerobic exercise and cognitive stimulation prevents decline in gray matter volume of the frontal, parietal and cingulate cortex in patients with mild cognitive impairment. Neuroimage. 2016;131:226–38. https://doi/org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.09.050Google Scholar
Mortimer, JA, Ding, D, Borenstein, AR, DeCarli, C, Guo, Q, Wu, Y, et al. Changes in brain volume and cognition in a randomized trial of exercise and social interaction in a community-based sample of non-demented Chinese elders. J Alzheimers Dis. 2012;30(4):757–66. https://doi/org/10.3233/JAD-2012-120079Google Scholar
Phillips, C. Lifestyle modulators of neuroplasticity: how physical activity, mental engagement, and diet promote cognitive health during aging. Neural Plast. 2017;2017:3589271. https://doi/org/10.1155/2017/3589271Google Scholar
Yang, Z, Wen, J, Erus, G, Govindarajan, ST, Melhem, R, Mamourian, E, et al. Brain aging patterns in a large and diverse cohort of 49,482 individuals. Nat Med. 2024. https://doi/org/10.1038/s41591-024-03144-xGoogle Scholar
Daviet, R, Aydogan, G, Jagannathan, K, Spilka, N, Koellinger, PD, Kranzler, HR, et al. Associations between alcohol consumption and gray and white matter volumes in the UK Biobank. Nat Commun. 2022;13(1):1175. https://doi/org/10.1038/s41467-022-28735-5Google Scholar
Solmi, M, Radua, J, Olivola, M, Croce, E, Soardo, L, Salazar de Pablo, G, et al. Age at onset of mental disorders worldwide: large-scale meta-analysis of 192 epidemiological studies. Mol Psychiatry. 2022;27(1):281–95. https://doi/org/10.1038/s41380-021-01161-7Google Scholar
Zannas, AS, McQuoid, DR, Payne, ME, Steffens, DC, MacFall, JR, Ashley-Koch, A, et al. Negative life stress and longitudinal hippocampal volume changes in older adults with and without depression. J Psychiatr Res. 2013;47(6):829–34. https://doi/org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.02.008Google Scholar
Gerritsen, L, Kalpouzos, G, Westman, E, Simmons, A, Wahlund, LO, Backman, L, et al. The influence of negative life events on hippocampal and amygdala volumes in old age: a life-course perspective. Psychol Med. 2015;45(6):1219–28. https://doi/org/10.1017/S0033291714002293Google Scholar
Moreno, GL, Bruss, J, Denburg, NL. Increased perceived stress is related to decreased prefrontal cortex volumes among older adults. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2017;39(4):313–25. https://doi/org/10.1080/13803395.2016.1225006Google Scholar
Lindgren, L, Bergdahl, J, Nyberg, L. Longitudinal evidence for smaller hippocampus volume as a vulnerability factor for perceived stress. Cereb Cortex. 2016;26(8):3527–33. https://doi/org/10.1093/cercor/bhw154Google Scholar
Demnitz-King, H, Goehre, I, Marchant, NL. The neuroanatomical correlates of repetitive negative thinking: a systematic review. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. 2021;316:111353. https://doi/org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2021.111353Google Scholar
Besteher, B, Gaser, C, Nenadic, I. Brain structure and subclinical symptoms: a dimensional perspective of psychopathology in the depression and anxiety spectrum. Neuropsychobiology. 2020;79(4–5):270–83. https://doi/org/10.1159/000501024Google Scholar
Schrader, J, Meller, T, Evermann, U, Pfarr, JK, Nenadic, I. Multi-modal morphometric association study of subclinical depressive symptoms using voxel-based morphometry, cortical thickness, and diffusion tensor imaging (DTI). J Affect Disord. 2024;351:755–64. https://doi/org/10.1016/j.jad.2024.01.221Google Scholar
Frodl, TS, Koutsouleris, N, Bottlender, R, Born, C, Jager, M, Scupin, I, et al. Depression-related variation in brain morphology over 3 years: effects of stress? Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008;65(10):1156–65. https://doi/org/10.1001/archpsyc.65.10.1156Google Scholar
Schmaal, L, Veltman, DJ, van Erp, TG, Samann, PG, Frodl, T, Jahanshad, N, et al. Subcortical brain alterations in major depressive disorder: findings from the ENIGMA major depressive disorder working group. Mol Psychiatry. 2016;21(6):806–12. https://doi/org/10.1038/mp.2015.69Google Scholar
Gray, JP, Muller, VI, Eickhoff, SB, Fox, PT. Multimodal abnormalities of brain structure and function in major depressive disorder: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Am J Psychiatry. 2020;177(5):422–34. https://doi/org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2019.19050560Google Scholar
Alloy, LB, Liu, RT, Bender, RE. Stress generation research in depression: a commentary. Int J Cogn Ther. 2010;3(4):380–8. https://doi/org/10.1521/ijct.2010.3.4.380Google Scholar
Liu, RT. Stress generation: future directions and clinical implications. Clin Psychol Rev. 2013;33(3):406–16. https://doi/org/10.1016/j.cpr.2013.01.005Google Scholar
Norbeck, JS. Modification of life event questionnaires for use with female respondents. Res Nurs Health. 1984;7(1):6171. https://doi/org/10.1002/nur.4770070110Google Scholar
Brugha, TS, Cragg, D. The list of threatening experiences: the reliability and validity of a brief life events questionnaire. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1990;82(1):7781. https://doi/org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.1990.tb01360.xGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Sample characteristics of participants at the first imaging assessment (n = 4,543)

Figure 1

Table 2. Fixed effect estimates from the linear mixed models comparing brain structure between participants who have or have not reported recent stressful life events

Figure 2

Figure 1. The predicted marginal effects from the linear mixed model showing different trajectories of the SLE− and SLE+ groups for hippocampal volume changes over the study follow-up period. The shaded regions represent the 95% confidence interval. Abbreviations: SLE, stressful life event; SLE−, individuals who reported no recent SLEs; SLE+, individuals who reported one or more recent SLE.

Figure 3

Table 3. Fixed effect estimates from the linear mixed models examining the association between brain structure and recent stressful life events and depression

Figure 4

Figure 2. The predicted marginal effects from the linear mixed model showing the differences in total GM volumes between depression groups depending on recent SLE exposure (SLE group). The error bars represent the 95% confidence interval. Abbreviations: GM, gray matter; SLE, stressful life event; SLE−, individuals who reported no recent SLEs; SLE+, individuals who reported one or more recent SLE; PHQ+, individuals with probable depression; PHQ−, individuals without probable depression.

Supplementary material: File

See et al. supplementary material

See et al. supplementary material
Download See et al. supplementary material(File)
File 411.1 KB
Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.