Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-g8jcs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T19:32:20.415Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Improved intraindividual variability in cognitive performance following cognitive and exercise training in older adults

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 October 2023

Nárlon C. Boa Sorte Silva
Affiliation:
Djavad Mowafaghian Centre for Brain Health, Faculty of Medicine, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada Department of Physical Therapy, Faculty of Medicine, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada Centre for Aging SMART, Vancouver Coastal Health Research Institute, Vancouver, BC, Canada
Lisanne F. ten Brinke
Affiliation:
Djavad Mowafaghian Centre for Brain Health, Faculty of Medicine, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada Department of Physical Therapy, Faculty of Medicine, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
Allison A. M. Bielak
Affiliation:
Department of Human Development and Family Studies, Colorado State University, Fort Collins, CO, USA
Todd C. Handy
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Faculty of Arts, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
Teresa Liu-Ambrose*
Affiliation:
Djavad Mowafaghian Centre for Brain Health, Faculty of Medicine, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada Department of Physical Therapy, Faculty of Medicine, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada Centre for Aging SMART, Vancouver Coastal Health Research Institute, Vancouver, BC, Canada
*
Corresponding author: Teresa Liu-Ambrose; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective:

Increased intraindividual variability (IIV) of cognitive performance is a marker of cognitive decline in older adults. Whether computerized cognitive training (CCT) and aerobic exercise counteracts cognitive decline by reducing IIV is unknown. We investigated the effects of CCT with or without aerobic exercise on IIV in older adults.

Methods:

This was a secondary analysis of an 8-week randomized controlled trial. Older adults (aged 65–85 years) were randomized to CCT alone (n = 41), CCT with aerobic exercise (n = 41), or an active control group (n = 42). The CCT group trained using the Fit Brains® platform 3×/week for 1 hr (plus 3×/week of home-based training). The CCT with aerobic exercise group received 15 min of walking plus 45 min of Fit Brains® 3×/week (plus 3×/week of home-based training). The control group received sham exercise and cognitive training (3×/week for 1 hr). We computed reaction time IIV from the Dimensional Change Card Sort Test, Flanker Inhibitory Control and Attention Test (Flanker), and Pattern Comparison Processing Speed Test (PACPS).

Results:

Compared with the control group, IIV reduced in a processing speed task (PACPS) following CCT alone (mean difference [95% confidence interval]: −0.144 [−0.255 to −0.034], p < 0.01) and CCT with aerobic exercise (−0.113 [−0.225 to −0.001], p < 0.05). Attention (Flanker congruent) IIV was reduced only after CCT with aerobic exercise (−0.130 [−0.242 to −0.017], p < 0.05).

Conclusions:

A CCT program promoted cognitive health via reductions in IIV of cognitive performance and combining it with aerobic exercise may result in broader benefits.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © INS. Published by Cambridge University Press 2023

Introduction

The prevalence of cognitive impairment continues to increase with recent estimates suggesting higher worldwide prevalence than previously expected (e.g., ∼416 million individuals along the Alzheimer’s disease spectrum) (Gustavsson et al., Reference Gustavsson, Norton, Fast, Frölich, Georges, Holzapfel, Kirabali, Krolak‐Salmon, Rossini, Ferretti, Lanman, Chadha and van der Flier2022). Unfortunately, effective dementia treatment options are not yet widely available and the prospect for disease-modifying pharmacological therapies is discouraging (Mehta et al., Reference Mehta, Jackson, Paul, Shi and Sabbagh2017). Thus, efforts to prevent or reduce the rate of cognitive decline via management of lifestyle factors have become a research priority (Baumgart et al., Reference Baumgart, Snyder, Carillo, Fazio, Kim and Johns2015; Rosenberg et al., Reference Rosenberg, Mangialasche, Ngandu, Solomon and Kivipelto2020). As recently estimated, approximately 40% of dementia cases worldwide could be prevented with effective management of modifiable lifestyle risk factors (Livingston et al., Reference Livingston, Huntley, Sommerlad, Ames, Ballard, Banerjee, Brayne, Burns, Cohen-Mansfield, Cooper, Costafreda, Dias, Fox, Gitlin, Howard, Kales, Kivimäki, Larson, Ogunniyi and Mukadam2020).

Considering that the pathophysiological processes underlying cognitive decline and dementia may take place decades before disease diagnosis (Villemagne et al., Reference Villemagne, Burnham, Bourgeat, Brown, Ellis, Salvado, Szoeke, Macaulay, Martins, Maruff, Ames, Rowe and Masters2013), targeting risk factors prior to the onset of clinical symptoms could be critical for preventing or deaccelerating disease progression (Kivipelto et al., Reference Kivipelto, Mangialasche and Ngandu2018). As an acceptable and feasible intervention (ten Brinke et al., Reference ten Brinke, Best, Chan, Ghag, Erickson, Handy and Liu-Ambrose2020), computerized cognitive training is a promising strategy for promoting cognitive performance in older adults (Gavelin et al., Reference Gavelin, Lampit, Hallock, Sabatés and Bahar-Fuchs2020; Lampit et al., Reference Lampit, Hallock and Valenzuela2014). Benefits of this type of cognitive training have been reported on measures of global cognitive function and processing speed in healthy individuals (Gates et al., Reference Gates, Rutjes, Di Nisio, Karim, Chong, March, Martínez and Vernooij2020). Positive effects have also been reported in those with mild cognitive impairment in global cognitive function, episodic memory, and working memory (Gates et al., Reference Gates, Vernooij, Nisio, Karim, March, Martínez and Rutjes2019). Nonetheless, the benefits of computerized cognitive training to cognitive function are still very modest, with very small effect sizes across cognitive domains in healthy older individuals (Gavelin et al., Reference Gavelin, Lampit, Hallock, Sabatés and Bahar-Fuchs2020) and those with cognitive impairment (Gates et al., Reference Gates, Vernooij, Nisio, Karim, March, Martínez and Rutjes2019). Small effect sizes in part reflect the fact that quality of the evidence from randomized controlled trials (RCTs) in older individuals remains critically low due to small sample sizes and high risk of bias on several study characteristics, including selection and reporting bias (Gates et al., Reference Gates, Rutjes, Di Nisio, Karim, Chong, March, Martínez and Vernooij2020, Reference Gates, Vernooij, Nisio, Karim, March, Martínez and Rutjes2019; Gavelin et al., Reference Gavelin, Lampit, Hallock, Sabatés and Bahar-Fuchs2020). Therefore, high-quality evidence from well-controlled RCTs of computerized cognitive training is warranted (Gavelin et al., Reference Gavelin, Lampit, Hallock, Sabatés and Bahar-Fuchs2020).

Maintaining a physically active lifestyle is another important strategy to foster healthy cognitive aging (Barnes et al., Reference Barnes, Yaffe, Satariano and Tager2003; Bherer et al., Reference Bherer, Erickson and Liu-Ambrose2013; Livingston et al., Reference Livingston, Sommerlad, Orgeta, Costafreda, Huntley, Ames, Ballard, Banerjee, Burns, Cohen-Mansfield, Cooper, Fox, Gitlin, Howard, Kales, Larson, Ritchie, Rockwood, Sampson and Mukadam2017; Lourida et al., Reference Lourida, Hannon, Littlejohns, Langa, Hyppönen, Kuzma and Llewellyn2019). Aerobic exercise training is a type of physical activity that benefits cognition in individuals with or without known cognitive impairment (Colcombe & Kramer, Reference Colcombe and Kramer2003; Erickson & Kramer, Reference Erickson and Kramer2008; Liu-Ambrose et al., Reference Liu-Ambrose, Best, Davis, Eng, Lee, Jacova, Boyd, Brasher, Munkacsy, Cheung and Hsiung2016). Aerobic exercise training is associated with improvements on measures of executive function, memory (Northey et al., Reference Northey, Cherbuin, Pumpa, Smee and Rattray2017; Sanders et al., Reference Sanders, Hortobágyi, Gemert, Van Der Zee and Van Heuvelen2019), and processing speed (Barha et al., Reference Barha, Davis, Falck, Nagamatsu and Liu-Ambrose2017; Smith et al., Reference Smith, Blumenthal, Hoffman, Cooper, Strauman, Welsh-Bohmer, Browndyke and Sherwood2010) in older individuals with and without cognitive impairment. Nevertheless, meta-analytic evidence still indicates that the effect sizes of aerobic exercise on cognition remains small across cognitive domains, which is thought to reflect small sample sizes and variability of exercise training prescription and cognitive assessment (Barha et al., Reference Barha, Davis, Falck, Nagamatsu and Liu-Ambrose2017; Falck et al., Reference Falck, Davis, Best, Crockett and Liu-Ambrose2019; Northey et al., Reference Northey, Cherbuin, Pumpa, Smee and Rattray2017; Sanders et al., Reference Sanders, Hortobágyi, Gemert, Van Der Zee and Van Heuvelen2019); thus, identifying strategies to maximize exercise-induced benefits on cognition would be helpful.

Emerging evidence suggests that multimodal behavioral interventions that include exercise and cognitive training may be more efficacious than single modality approaches to improve cognition (Gavelin et al., Reference Gavelin, Dong, Minkov, Bahar-Fuchs, Ellis, Lautenschlager, Mellow, Wade, Smith, Finke, Krohn and Lampit2021; Kivipelto et al., Reference Kivipelto, Mangialasche and Ngandu2018; Rosenberg et al., Reference Rosenberg, Mangialasche, Ngandu, Solomon and Kivipelto2020). From a mechanistic point of view, the combined effects of multimodal behavioral interventions would result from synergistic and/or additive neuroplastic adaptations facilitated by the distinct, yet complimentary, nature of these strategies (Gavelin et al., Reference Gavelin, Dong, Minkov, Bahar-Fuchs, Ellis, Lautenschlager, Mellow, Wade, Smith, Finke, Krohn and Lampit2021). Aerobic exercise is known to transiently increase neurotrophic factors associated with neurogenesis and neuroplasticity (Cotman et al., Reference Cotman, Berchtold and Christie2007). Cognitive training modalities like computerized cognitive training could further promote synaptic plasticity and the survival and functional integration of the newly formed neurons into neural networks (Gonçalves et al., Reference Gonçalves, Schafer and Gage2016; Shors et al., Reference Shors, Anderson, Curlik and Nokia2012). When administered in immediate succession, engaging in moderate-intensity exercise prior to cognitive training could also augment cognitive benefits via increases in cortisol. That is, exercise transiently increases cortisol by stimulating the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis (Luger et al., Reference Luger, Deuster, Kyle, Gallucci, Montgomery, Gold, Loriaux and Chrousos1987). Cortisol levels remain elevated for up to 2 hr after exercise cessation, a period in which cortisol can enhance learning, memory consolidation, and prefrontal lobe functions (Basso & Suzuki, Reference Basso and Suzuki2017).

Furthermore, combining computerized cognitive training with exercise interventions may promote benefits to cognition while also impacting other health outcomes like physical function (Gavelin et al., Reference Gavelin, Dong, Minkov, Bahar-Fuchs, Ellis, Lautenschlager, Mellow, Wade, Smith, Finke, Krohn and Lampit2021). This means that combining interventions can be a resource-effective approach for offsetting the impact of multiple dementia risk factors (Gavelin et al., Reference Gavelin, Dong, Minkov, Bahar-Fuchs, Ellis, Lautenschlager, Mellow, Wade, Smith, Finke, Krohn and Lampit2021). Although the observed effects are small, the current literature supports this notion as combined interventions administering cognitive and physical training in older adults seem to benefit global and domain-specific cognition including executive function, processing speed, and memory as well physical function compared with either intervention alone (Gavelin et al., Reference Gavelin, Dong, Minkov, Bahar-Fuchs, Ellis, Lautenschlager, Mellow, Wade, Smith, Finke, Krohn and Lampit2021). We showed that 8 weeks of combined computerized cognitive training and aerobic exercise benefitted cognition, promoting more widespread improvements than cognitive training alone (ten Brinke et al., Reference ten Brinke, Best, Chan, Ghag, Erickson, Handy and Liu-Ambrose2020). Improvements resulting from the combined training were observed particularly in subdomains of executive function, which included response inhibition, set-shifting, and cognitive flexibility tasks (ten Brinke et al., Reference ten Brinke, Best, Chan, Ghag, Erickson, Handy and Liu-Ambrose2020).

Nonetheless, current evidence shows the overall effect of behavioral interventions on cognition in older adults is small (Gavelin et al., Reference Gavelin, Dong, Minkov, Bahar-Fuchs, Ellis, Lautenschlager, Mellow, Wade, Smith, Finke, Krohn and Lampit2021). Beyond the influence of limitations mentioned above (e.g., small sample sizes, selection bias), the observed small effects may be due to how cognition is measured in studies. In this regard, more sensitive measures to detect intervention-driven effects on cognition in older individuals are needed (Brydges & Bielak, Reference Brydges and Bielak2019; MacDonald & Stawski, Reference MacDonald and Stawski2015; Vrinceanu et al., Reference Vrinceanu, Blanchette, Intzandt, Lussier, Pothier, Vu, Nigam, Bosquet, Karelis, Li, Berryman and Bherer2021). One of such approaches is utilizing intraindividual variability of cognitive performance to determine whether intervention effects span beyond average (i.e., mean) performance on a given task (MacDonald & Stawski, Reference MacDonald and Stawski2015).

Intraindividual variability of cognitive performance can characterize cognitive impairment and predict longitudinal cognitive and physical function outcomes (Chow et al., Reference Chow, Rabi, Paracha, Vasquez, Hasher, Alain, Anderson and Gamaldo2022; Graveson et al., Reference Graveson, Bauermeister, McKeown and Bunce2016; Haynes, Bauermeister, et al., Reference Haynes, Bauermeister and Bunce2017). This is concomitant with the notion that cognitive processes become more unstable as one ages and increasing stability (i.e., reducing variability) would reflect improvements on overall cognitive performance (MacDonald & Stawski, Reference MacDonald and Stawski2015). Therefore, it is plausible that intervention effects on cognition could be more evident on measures of intraindividual variability than mean performance, as the former may be more sensitive to subtle improvements underling stability of cognitive processes even in the absence of changes in the latter (MacDonald & Stawski, Reference MacDonald and Stawski2015).

As an important limitation in the current literature, intraindividual variability has been poorly studied compared with measures of central tendency (e.g., mean, median) (Falck et al., Reference Falck, Davis, Best, Crockett and Liu-Ambrose2019; Lampit et al., Reference Lampit, Hallock and Valenzuela2014). While dominant, measures of central tendency do not paint the full picture of cognitive functioning as they fail to account for inconsistency on task performance (MacDonald et al., Reference MacDonald, Nyberg and Bäckman2006). As such, the study of intraindividual variability can shed light on understudied elements of cognitive decline in older adults (Haynes, Bauermeister, et al., Reference Haynes, Bauermeister and Bunce2017).

Intraindividual variability is defined as within-participant trial-to-trial variation in reaction time during cognitive tasks (Hultsch et al., Reference Hultsch, Strauss, Hunter and MacDonald2008). Higher levels of intraindividual variability are associated with poorer cognitive performance (Bielak & Anstey, Reference Bielak and Anstey2019; MacDonald et al., Reference MacDonald, Nyberg and Bäckman2006) and likely result from compromised integrity of neurobiological substrates (Haynes, Bauermeister, et al., Reference Haynes, Bauermeister and Bunce2017; MacDonald & Stawski, Reference MacDonald and Stawski2015). For instance, evidence suggest that intraindividual variability could reflect gray matter neurodegeneration especially in frontal regions (MacDonald et al., Reference MacDonald, Nyberg and Bäckman2006). Higher intraindividual variability has also been associated with age-related myelin degradation across the lifespan (Grydeland et al., Reference Grydeland, Walhovd, Tamnes, Westlye and Fjell2013), as well as increased white matter hyperintensities burden (Bunce et al., Reference Bunce, Bielak, Cherbuin, Batterham, Wen, Sachdev and Anstey2013; Haynes, Bunce, et al., Reference Haynes, Bunce, Kochan, Wen, Brodaty and Sachdev2017; Nilsson et al., Reference Nilsson, Thomas, O’Brien and Gallagher2014), a marker of cerebral small vessel disease (Alber et al., Reference Alber, Alladi, Bae, Barton, Beckett, Bell, Berman, Biessels, Black, Bos, Bowman, Brai, Brickman, Callahan, Corriveau, Fossati, Gottesman, Gustafson, Hachinski and Hainsworth2019). There is also the possibility that higher intraindividual variability reflects alteration in neurofunctional states during complex cognitive tasks (Garrett et al., Reference Garrett, Epp, Kleemeyer, Lindenberger and Polk2020; Garrett et al., Reference Garrett, McIntosh and Grady2014), and reduced cortical activation in regions responsible for efficient and dynamic regulation of attention and cognitive control (Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Pinar, Fornito, Nandam, Hester and Bellgrove2015). Thus, intraindividual variability metrics are not merely noise in assessment of cognitive performance but rather proxy measures of stability within neurobiological correlates of cognition (MacDonald et al., Reference MacDonald, Nyberg and Bäckman2006).

From a clinical perspective, intraindividual variability of cognitive performance might be a useful marker of cognitive and functional decline. A systematic review showed that higher reaction time intraindividual variability at baseline was associated with steeper cognitive decline, increased dementia risk, and overall higher mortality risk (Haynes, Bauermeister, et al., Reference Haynes, Bauermeister and Bunce2017). A meta-analysis further corroborates these findings wherein Mumme and colleagues report that greater intraindividual variability of cognitive performance is longitudinally associated with cognitive decline and subsequent conversion to dementia with a medium effect size (r = 0.20, 95% CI = 0.09 to 0.31) (Mumme et al., Reference Mumme, Pushpanathan, Donaldson, Weinborn, Rainey-Smith, Maruff and Bucks2021). The association between intraindividual variability of cognitive performance and functional status is less understood. Nevertheless, both cognitive function and mobility contribute to one’s functional status. High intraindividual variability is associated with poor gait and increased falls risk, which suggests that high intraindividual variability may also reflect compromised neurobiological substrates related to mobility and control of gait (Graveson et al., Reference Graveson, Bauermeister, McKeown and Bunce2016).

Due to its adaptive and neuroplastic nature (MacDonald & Stawski, Reference MacDonald and Stawski2015), intraindividual variability can serve as a critical outcome measure in lifestyle interventions designed to improve cognitive health, and potentially strengthen the evidence on the efficacy of these interventions in older adults. In recent investigations, intraindividual variability has been shown to improve after different types of interventions such as cognitive training (Vrinceanu et al., Reference Vrinceanu, Blanchette, Intzandt, Lussier, Pothier, Vu, Nigam, Bosquet, Karelis, Li, Berryman and Bherer2021), as well as combined physical, cognitive, and social engagement (Brydges et al., Reference Brydges, Carlson, Andrews, Rebok, Bielak and Anderson2021); however, not all types of interventions have shown change in intraindividual variability in older adults (Bielak & Brydges, Reference Bielak and Brydges2019). Evidence from studies combining computerized cognitive training and aerobic exercise to improve intraindividual variability is still limited.

The goal of this study was to determine the effect of an 8-week computerized cognitive training program with or without aerobic exercise on intraindividual variability parameters. This is a secondary exploratory analysis of a completed RCT in healthy older adults (ten Brinke et al., Reference ten Brinke, Best, Chan, Ghag, Erickson, Handy and Liu-Ambrose2020), the results of which we have mentioned above. It was expected that computerized cognitive training with or without additional aerobic exercise would result in significant reductions in intraindividual variability compared with an active control group.

Methods

The study protocol (ten Brinke et al., Reference ten Brinke, Best, Crockett and Liu-Ambrose2018) and the primary findings (ten Brinke et al., Reference ten Brinke, Best, Chan, Ghag, Erickson, Handy and Liu-Ambrose2020) of this RCT have been published previously. The main aspects of the study protocol are summarized in the following sections.

Study design

This is a secondary analysis of an 8-week, single-blinded, proof-of-concept RCT (ClinicalTrials.gov identifier: NCT02564809) conducted at The University of British Columbia and Vancouver General Hospital, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada. Outcome assessments were performed at baseline and trial completion (i.e., 8 weeks) by trained assessors blinded to group allocation.

Participants

Community-dwelling older adults from metro Vancouver were recruited between September 2015 and April 2017. After an initial screening, participants who met inclusion criteria and signed the informed consent were invited for baseline assessments. The study CONSORT diagram illustrating participant flow throughout the study has been published previously (ten Brinke et al., Reference ten Brinke, Best, Chan, Ghag, Erickson, Handy and Liu-Ambrose2020). Briefly, 379 individuals were screened, 130 consented to participate in the study, and 124 were randomized after baseline assessments (see Table 1). Ethical approval was obtained from The University of British Columbia Clinical Research Ethics Board and the Vancouver Coastal Health Research Institute ethics board. All human data included in this manuscript were obtained in compliance with the Helsinki Declaration.

Table 1. Baseline demographic and outcome data

Note: Data presented as either mean (SD) or n (%) unless otherwise stated. BAT = balanced and toned group; FBT = Fit Brains® training; Ex-FBT = exercise plus Fit Brains® training group; ISD = intraindividual standard deviation; ICV = intraindividual coefficient of variation.

a Data removed for one participant with only two reaction time trials.

Inclusion and exclusion criteria

Community-dwelling older adults meeting the following criteria were included: (a) aged 65 to 85 years; (b) completed high school education; (c) no prior diagnosis of cognitive impairment or dementia; (d) had preserved general cognitive function as indicated by a score ≥24/30 on the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) (Folstein et al., Reference Folstein, Folstein and McHugh1975); (e) scored ≥6/8 on the Lawton and Brody (Lawton & Brody, Reference Lawton and Brody1969) Instrumental Activities of Daily Living Scale; (f) were not expected to start or were stable on a fixed dose of antidementia medications (e. g., donepezil, galantamine) during the study period; and (g) were suitable to engage in 15 min of brisk walking based on the Physical Activity Readiness Questionnaire (Canadian Society of Exercise Physiology, 1994).

We excluded individuals who: (a) were diagnosed with dementia of any type; (b) had a neurodegenerative disease as the cause of mild cognitive impairment that was not Alzheimer’s disease, vascular dementia, or both (e.g., multiple sclerosis, Parkinson’s disease); (c) experienced clinically significant peripheral neuropathy or severe musculoskeletal or joint disease that impairs mobility; and (d) were taking medications that may negatively affect cognitive function.

Randomization

Participants (N = 124) were randomized at a 1:1:1 ratio to one of the following groups: Fit Brains® Training (FBT, n = 41), Exercise plus Fit Brains® Training (Ex-FBT, n = 41), or Balanced And Toned (BAT, n = 42) via www.randomization.com. Randomization was performed by a team member not involved in the study. Randomization took place after study enrollment and baseline assessments. Specifically, the study coordinator sent a list of participant identification numbers to an independent team member who provided the study coordinator with the group allocation. The study coordinator then informed participants of their group assignment. All outcome assessors were blinded after treatment allocation.

Sample size

The sample size for this study was calculated using the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVL) (Lezak, Reference Lezak1995) as the study primary outcome, which is reported elsewhere (ten Brinke et al., Reference ten Brinke, Best, Chan, Ghag, Erickson, Handy and Liu-Ambrose2020, Reference ten Brinke, Best, Crockett and Liu-Ambrose2018). Briefly, based on previous work (Diamond et al., Reference Diamond, Mowszowski, Cockayne, Norrie, Paradise, Hermens, Lewis, Hickie and Naismith2015), it was predicted that a RAVL mean change (z-score) of 0.31 for the FBT group, 0.40 for the Ex-FBT group, and −0.31 for the BAT group would occur. With a pooled standard deviation of 1.1, and alpha of 0.05, 36 participants were needed for a power of 0.80. With a 10% drop-out rate, the total sample size was 120 participants (i.e., 40 FBT, 40 Ex-FBT, and 40 BAT).

Intervention

A detailed description of each of the intervention arms in this study has been published previously (ten Brinke et al., Reference ten Brinke, Best, Crockett and Liu-Ambrose2018).

Fit Brains® Training

Participants randomized to FBT performed computerized cognitive training targeting six cognitive domains (i.e., focus, speed, memory, visual, problem solving, and language). Training sessions were delivered using an iPad; participants performed training sessions 3×/week for 60 min at the research center, as well as 3×/week for 60 min at home. Cognitive training was individualized and adaptive throughout the 8-week program.

Exercise plus Fit Brains® Training

Participants randomized to Ex-FBT attended training sessions 3×/week for 60 min at the research center and 3x/week for 60 min at home. The 60-min sessions consisted of a 15-min brisk walk (i.e., up to 13–14 on the 6–20 Borg’s Rating of Perceived Exertion scale) (Borg, Reference Borg1982) followed by 45 min of computerized cognitive training. Cognitive training took place immediately after the end of the 15-min brisk walk. For home sessions, participants were asked to fill out logs with start time, number of steps taken, and end time for aerobic training, while the home cognitive training was tracked through the Fit Brains® program app.

Balanced and Toned

Participants randomized to BAT (i.e., active control) attended training sessions 3×/week for 60 min at the research center for 8 weeks. Specifically, at the research center, participants completed 8 hr of sham cognitive training wherein they played casual online games that do not significantly tap into complex cognitive abilities (e.g., executive function) (Baniqued et al., Reference Baniqued, Lee, Voss, Basak, Cosman, DeSouza, Severson, Salthouse and Kramer2013), as well as group-based games (e.g., drawing using both dominant and non-dominant hand, writing captions on cartoons, and word games). We also delivered 8 hr of sham exercise training, which included stretching, range of motion, basic core-strength exercises, and balance training (e.g., Tai Chi-based forms like Crane and Tree Pose, tandem stand, tandem walking, and single leg stance). Participants also completed 8 hr of education relating to brain health (e.g., included lectures on sleep, goal setting, mindfulness, as well as educational project), which included a homework assignment (maximum of 3 hr/week).

Demographic variables

We collected demographic characteristics data at baseline (Table 1) including age, self-reported sex, education level, height, weight, and body mass index. We used the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) (Nasreddine et al., Reference Nasreddine, Phillips, Bedirian, Charbonneau, Whitehead, Collin, Cumming and Chertkow2005) and MMSE (Folstein et al., Reference Folstein, Folstein and McHugh1975) to assess the cognitive status of the study participants. The MoCA score is more sensitive to mild changes in cognitive function, and we used it better characterize our sample (Nasreddine et al., Reference Nasreddine, Phillips, Bedirian, Charbonneau, Whitehead, Collin, Cumming and Chertkow2005). The MMSE was applied to exclude participants with cognitive scoring indicating dementia (see “Inclusion and exclusion criteria” section) (Folstein et al., Reference Folstein, Folstein and McHugh1975).

Study outcomes

Change in intraindividual variability measures from baseline to trial completion were the outcomes in this investigation. We extracted reaction time data from three neuropsychological tests within the Cognition Battery of the National Institute of Health (NIH) Toolbox (Weintraub et al., Reference Weintraub, Dikmen, Heaton, Tulsky, Zelazo, Bauer, Carlozzi, Slotkin, Blitz, Wallner-Allen, Fox, Beaumont, Mungas, Nowinski, Richler, Deocampo, Anderson, Manly, Borosh and Gershon2013) namely the Dimensional Change Card Sort Test, the Flanker Inhibitory Control and Attention Test (Flanker), and the Pattern Comparison Processing Speed Test. The NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery is a computerized and automated battery to assess key cognitive domains affected by age- and pathology-related decline (Weintraub et al., Reference Weintraub, Dikmen, Heaton, Tulsky, Zelazo, Bauer, Carlozzi, Slotkin, Blitz, Wallner-Allen, Fox, Beaumont, Mungas, Nowinski, Richler, Deocampo, Anderson, Manly, Borosh and Gershon2013). The three tasks above were selected to derive intraindividual variability because these tasks rely heavily on reaction time latencies to assess participant’s performance, wherein accurate and/or faster responses indicate better performance (Weintraub et al., Reference Weintraub, Dikmen, Heaton, Tulsky, Zelazo, Bauer, Carlozzi, Slotkin, Blitz, Wallner-Allen, Fox, Beaumont, Mungas, Nowinski, Richler, Deocampo, Anderson, Manly, Borosh and Gershon2013) Furthermore, for the current secondary analysis, the NIH Toolbox Cognitive Battery tasks were also chosen as they assess cognitive domains which would be sensitive to the training intervention. The Fit Brains® program included 38 games in its mobile form (iPads) targeting six cognitive subdomains (i.e., focus, speed, memory, visual, problem solving, and language) (ten Brinke et al., Reference ten Brinke, Best, Crockett and Liu-Ambrose2018). The program offers a higher frequency of games targeting executive functions (four out of six domains), while remaining games targeted processing speed and memory (ten Brinke et al., Reference ten Brinke, Best, Crockett and Liu-Ambrose2018). Details of the NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery can be found elsewhere (Weintraub et al., Reference Weintraub, Dikmen, Heaton, Tulsky, Zelazo, Bauer, Carlozzi, Slotkin, Blitz, Wallner-Allen, Fox, Beaumont, Mungas, Nowinski, Richler, Deocampo, Anderson, Manly, Borosh and Gershon2013), below we provide a short description of the tasks included in the current study:

Flanker inhibitory control and attention test

The Flanker test measures visuospatial inhibitory attention; in this test, participants are requested to as quickly as possible indicate the direction of a central arrow (target) within a row of arrows (i.e., flankers) (Weintraub et al., Reference Weintraub, Dikmen, Heaton, Tulsky, Zelazo, Bauer, Carlozzi, Slotkin, Blitz, Wallner-Allen, Fox, Beaumont, Mungas, Nowinski, Richler, Deocampo, Anderson, Manly, Borosh and Gershon2013) by touching one of two arrows at the bottom on the screen. In congruent trials, the target arrow is in the same direction as the flankers, while in incongruent trials the target arrow faces the opposite direction. Performance on the task was measured over 20 random trials.

Dimensional change card sort test

This task is a measure of set-shifting in which participants must match a series of bivalent test pictures (i.e., yellow balls and blue trucks) to a target picture according to one of two dimensions (i.e., color or shape) as quickly as possible. For each trial, the dimension by which they need to “sort” appears on the screen and participants touch the screen to select one of two pictures that match the correct target dimension. Performance on the task was measured over 30 trials.

Pattern comparison processing speed test

This task measures choice reaction time whereby participants must indicate whether two images presented side-by-side are identical by touching “yes” or “no” on the screen. The task lasts 85 s and participants are to complete as many trials as possible with a maximum of 130 trials.

Data cleaning

Raw reaction time data were extracted for all accurate trials from each of the three tasks included in the study. We trimmed accurate reaction time by removing latencies lower than 150 ms or 3 standard deviations (SD) above the mean for the same participant, task, and timepoint (Bielak & Anstey, Reference Bielak and Anstey2019). Moreover, to conform with previous research (Bielak & Anstey, Reference Bielak and Anstey2019; Brydges et al., Reference Brydges, Liu-Ambrose and Bielak2020), we performed imputation of excluded data by applying a regression substitution procedure that creates individualized reaction time equations, which are then used to predict the missing values (Bielak & Anstey, Reference Bielak and Anstey2019). Missing data were not imputed if a participant had more than 50% of data missing across trials in which case data for that participant were excluded. Only ∼1.5% of raw data were imputed across all tasks and timepoints. As well, one participant had only two reaction time trials in the Pattern Comparison Processing Speed Test at baseline, this participant was not included in the analysis of this task.

Computation of intraindividual variability

The main measure of intraindividual variability applied in this study was residual intraindividual SD (residual ISD), which is an unbiased measure of variability that accounts for within (e.g., practice effects) and between participant (e.g., age differences) source of variation that could influence reaction time latencies (Bielak & Anstey, Reference Bielak and Anstey2019; Hultsch et al., Reference Hultsch, Strauss, Hunter and MacDonald2008). For each task and timepoint, we entered reaction time data for all participants in a regression model with reaction time trial, categorical age (i.e., 65–69, 70–74, 75–79, or 80–84), and an interaction term (trial × age) as model predictors (Bielak & Anstey, Reference Bielak and Anstey2019). Next, we extracted residuals from each model and transformed these residuals into T scores. We then computed the SD of the transformed residuals (i.e., residual ISD) separately for each participant, task, and timepoint. In addition, we calculated the commonly used SD of raw reaction time latencies (raw-score ISD) and intraindividual coefficient of variation (ICV, defined as raw-score ISD/mean) (Haynes, Bauermeister, et al., Reference Haynes, Bauermeister and Bunce2017). Although raw-score ISD and ICV are not considered entirely robust measures of intraindividual variability (Bielak & Anstey, Reference Bielak and Anstey2019), we included these measures as secondary outcomes in our analysis given their common use (Haynes, Bauermeister, et al., Reference Haynes, Bauermeister and Bunce2017). All intraindividual variability measures were log-transformed prior to analysis.

Lastly, we computed intraindividual variability metrics separately for the congruent and incongruent conditions in the Flanker task, and then derived the Flaker cost (i.e., interference score) subtracting the incongruent from the congruent intraindividual variability metrics after log-transformation. The Flanker cost is a robust index of inhibitory attention in which performance is unbiased by differences in response time (Kramer et al., Reference Kramer, Humphrey, Larish and Logan1994). Data derived from the Flanker congruent condition and Flanker cost were used in the analysis.

Statistical analysis

We applied multilevel modeling to determine differences between groups at 8 weeks on residual ISD, raw-score ISD, and ICV measures. Specifically, we applied linear mixed models with restricted maximum likelihood estimation including random intercepts, and fixed effects of group, task, and group-by-task interaction. Baseline performance, age, and MoCA were included as fixed effects covariates in each model. In total, three models were conducted for each intraindividual variability measure (i.e., residual ISD, raw-score ISD, and ICV).

Differences between groups were determined via post hoc, Bonferroni-corrected pairwise comparisons. Using BAT as the reference group, we extracted estimated mean difference between groups along with 95% confidence interval. In all post hoc analyses, two-sided, Bonferroni-corrected p values < 0.05 were considered as statistically significant.

Data preparation, cleaning, and analysis were performed in R version 4.2.0 (https://www.R-project.org/) using the tidyverse (version 1.3.1), lme4 (version 1.1-32), lmerTest (version 3.1-3), emmeans (version 1.7.4-1), knitr (version 1.39), and patchwork (version 1.1.1) packages within RStudio version 2023.06.1+524 (RStudio Team, 2023).

Results

Participant clinical and demographic characteristics

Descriptive characteristics and baseline intraindividual variability parameters are presented in Table 1 (see Supplementary Material for log-transformed intraindividual variability data at baseline). Overall, study participants were highly educated and mostly female. Adherence and adverse events have been reported previously (ten Brinke et al., Reference ten Brinke, Best, Chan, Ghag, Erickson, Handy and Liu-Ambrose2020). Briefly, all groups achieved >90% compliance on all elements of the interventions administered in the current study, and there were 7 dropouts across the whole study sample.

Differences between groups in study outcomes

Results for between-group differences are reported in Table 2. Compared with the control group (BAT), Ex-FBT significantly reduced attention intraindividual variability indexed as lower residual ISD in the Flanker congruent task (mean difference [95% confidence interval] −0.130 [−0.242 to −0.017], p < 0.05, Figure 1). Similarly, Ex-FBT significantly reduced Flanker congruent raw-score ISD (−0.124 [−0.239 to −0.009], p < 0.05) compared with BAT

Both experimental groups also significantly reduced processing speed intraindividual variability measured with the Pattern Comparison Processing Speed Test. These changes were seen as reduction in residual ISD in FBT (−0.144 [−0.255 to −0.034], p < 0.01) and Ex-FBT (−0.113 [−0.225 to −0.001], p < 0.05) compared with BAT. Also, for processing speed, both FBT (−0.170 [−0.282 to −0.058], p < 0.01) and Ex-FBT (−0.125 [−0.239 to −0.011], p < 0.05) groups showed greater reduction in raw-score ISD compared with BAT, while only FBT reduced ICV for this task (−0.108 [−0.200 to −0.017], p < 0.05).

Figure 1. Comparisons between groups in residual ISD after the 8-week intervention period.

Note: Lower values indicate reduced variability and thus better performance. Upper panel shows estimated marginal means at 8 weeks, lower panel shows distribution of data across groups. DCCS = Dimensional Change Card Sort; PACPS = Pattern Comparison Processing Speed; BAT = balanced and toned group; FBT = Fit Brains® training group; Ex-FBT = exercise plus Fit Brains® training group; ISD = intraindividual standard deviation. *Significant differences compared with the BAT group at Bonferroni-corrected p < 0.05. **Significant differences compared with the BAT group at Bonferroni-corrected p < 0.01.

Table 2. Log-transformed estimated marginal means and differences between groups after the 8-week intervention period

Note: CI = confidence interval; DCCS = Dimensional Change Card Sort; PACPS = Pattern Comparison Processing Speed; BAT = balanced and toned group; FBT = Fit Brains® training group; Ex-FBT = exercise plus Fit Brains® training group; ISD = intraindividual standard deviation; ICV = intraindividual coefficient of variation.

a All intraindividual variability data presented are log-transformed. All models were adjusted for baseline performance, age, and Montreal Cognitive Assessment scores.

b Data removed for one participant with only two reaction time trials at baseline.

* Significant between-group differences at Bonferroni-corrected p < 0.05.

** Significant between-group differences at Bonferroni-corrected p < 0.01.

There were no differences between the intervention groups compared with BAT in set-shifting (Dimensional Change Card Sort Test) in any of the studied intraindividual variability measures. As well, despite intraindividual variability reductions in the Flanker congruent following Ex-FBT reported above, we did not notice any changes in any of the intraindividual variability measures for the Flanker cost.

Discussion

This exploratory study investigated the effects of an 8-week computerized cognitive training with or without aerobic exercise in intraindividual variability measures in community-dwelling older adults aged 65 to 85 years old. Our findings indicated that computerized cognitive training improved intraindividual variability on a processing speed task relative to an 8-week sham exercise and cognitive training program. Moreover, a 15-min brisk walk prior to computerized cognitive training also conferred benefits to intraindividual variability on tasks measuring attention and processing speed. These findings expand upon the parent study results (ten Brinke et al., Reference ten Brinke, Best, Chan, Ghag, Erickson, Handy and Liu-Ambrose2020) showing that improvements in cognition following computerized cognitive training, with or without additional aerobic exercise, may result from increased consistency on task performance.

Our investigation shows novel findings suggesting improvements in intraindividual variability parameters following a computerized cognitive training with or without additional aerobic exercise in otherwise healthy older adults. Reduced variability in our study suggests preserved integrity of neurobiological processes underlying performance on processing speed tasks (Hultsch et al., Reference Hultsch, Strauss, Hunter and MacDonald2008). We also noted intervention effects in raw-score ISD and ICV on processing speed and attention tasks, suggesting agreement between the distinct measures employed in our investigation.

Our results partially support the findings from the main study in this sample (ten Brinke et al., Reference ten Brinke, Best, Chan, Ghag, Erickson, Handy and Liu-Ambrose2020) where effects on cognition were seen in the combined computerized cognitive training with aerobic exercise group (Ex-FBT). Specifically, the Ex-FBT group improved response inhibition and set-shifting compared with the control group, as measured via neuropsychological tests independent from the ones used in the current study (i.e., Stroop Test and Trail-Making Test, respectively). The previous study also found improvements in the Dimensional Change Card Sort Test and Flanker overall scores (combined congruent and incongruent) favoring computerized cognitive training with additional aerobic exercise, while the computerized cognitive training group (FBT) only improved on the Stroop Test. Therefore, our results suggest that healthy older adults benefit from computerized cognitive training not only on mean cognitive performance but also on indices of cognitive variability. Although our investigation was not powered to detect differences between intervention groups, the additional aerobic training seemed to confer additional benefits to intraindividual variability of attention measured with the Flanker congruent condition, while computerized cognitive training alone did not.

Notwithstanding, our results showed no intervention effects of computerized cognitive training with or without exercise on intraindividual variability for the Dimensional Change Card Sort Test task, which does not align with the results in the main trial (ten Brinke et al., Reference ten Brinke, Best, Chan, Ghag, Erickson, Handy and Liu-Ambrose2020). It is plausible that as a complex set-shifting task (Zelazo et al., Reference Zelazo, Anderson, Richler, Wallner-Allen, Beaumont, Conway, Gershon and Weintraub2014), the neurobiological substrates related to intraindividual variability in the Dimensional Change Card Sort Test are differently influenced by computerized cognitive training with aerobic exercise. This would be in contrast with simpler tasks like the Flanker congruent condition (Zelazo et al., Reference Zelazo, Anderson, Richler, Wallner-Allen, Beaumont, Conway, Gershon and Weintraub2014) and the Pattern Comparison Processing Speed Test (Weintraub et al., Reference Weintraub, Dikmen, Heaton, Tulsky, Zelazo, Bauer, Carlozzi, Slotkin, Blitz, Wallner-Allen, Fox, Beaumont, Mungas, Nowinski, Richler, Deocampo, Anderson, Manly, Borosh and Gershon2013). This conjecture can be supported by our results showing a lack of intervention effects on intraindividual variability in the Flanker cost (i.e., incongruent minus congruent), despite changes in the original Flanker congruent score. The Flanker cost theoretically comprises a more robust index of inhibitory attention, reflecting performance unbiased by differences in base response time. In this context, our findings indicate that the combined intervention effects seem to primarily impact performance on cognitive domains of less complexity such as processing speed and attention, with little effect on set-shifting.

The literature assessing the impact of interventions on intraindividual variability as an outcome measure is still developing, with recent studies showing mixed results (Bielak & Brydges, Reference Bielak and Brydges2019; Brydges & Bielak, Reference Brydges and Bielak2019; Brydges et al., Reference Brydges, Carlson, Andrews, Rebok, Bielak and Anderson2021, Reference Brydges, Liu-Ambrose and Bielak2020; Vrinceanu et al., Reference Vrinceanu, Blanchette, Intzandt, Lussier, Pothier, Vu, Nigam, Bosquet, Karelis, Li, Berryman and Bherer2021). Brydges and Bielak reported no significant intraindividual variability changes in inhibitory attention (measured with Flanker) in older adults following 15 weeks of either productive cognitive engagement or passive engagement interventions (Brydges & Bielak, Reference Brydges and Bielak2019). A study investigating changes in intraindividual variability following resistance exercise compared with exergaming in healthy older adults also showed no intraindividual variability differences between groups in response inhibition (Meneghini et al., Reference Meneghini, Barbosa, Lourenço and Borgatto2022). As well, little improvement in intraindividual variability in an inhibitory attention task was reported in older adults following a 2-year physical activity program compared with an education control group (Bielak & Brydges, Reference Bielak and Brydges2019). In contrast, reductions in intraindividual variability for response inhibition have been reported in older adults following a 2-year program of combined physical, cognitive, and social engagement (Brydges et al., Reference Brydges, Carlson, Andrews, Rebok, Bielak and Anderson2021). A 12-week RCT also reported improvements in intraindividual variability on a dual-task paradigm following computerized cognitive training, but not resistance exercise or gross-motor abilities training, in healthy older adults (Vrinceanu et al., Reference Vrinceanu, Blanchette, Intzandt, Lussier, Pothier, Vu, Nigam, Bosquet, Karelis, Li, Berryman and Bherer2021). Considering these mixed findings and the findings of the current study, cognitively engaging interventions with or without additional exercise might elicit similar changes in intraindividual variability, with additional aerobic exercise potentially expanding cognitive benefits; however, the different types, length of interventions, outcome measures, and population in previous studies limit our ability to draw definitive conclusions.

We consider lower intraindividual variability as marker of cognitive improvement. This notion stems from several cross-sectional and longitudinal investigations linking high intraindividual variability to poor cognitive performance in younger and older adults, as well as cognitive decline in older individuals. For instance, in healthy younger adults, increasing task difficulty results in decline in task accuracy (i.e., worse performance) and a linear increase in intraindividual variability measured with residual ISD (Garrett et al., Reference Garrett, McIntosh and Grady2014). Older individuals showing greater neurofunctional adaptability (measured with task-based fMRI) also show better cognitive performance and lower intraindividual variability across a series of cognitive tasks (Garrett et al., Reference Garrett, Epp, Kleemeyer, Lindenberger and Polk2020). Compared with healthy age-matched controls, older individuals with mild cognitive impairment show higher intraindividual variability on executive functions and poorer cognitive performance (Chow et al., Reference Chow, Rabi, Paracha, Vasquez, Hasher, Alain, Anderson and Gamaldo2022). Longitudinally, higher intraindividual variability at baseline in older adults has been associated with steeper cognitive decline, increased dementia risk, and overall higher mortality risk (Haynes, Bauermeister, et al., Reference Haynes, Bauermeister and Bunce2017). A recent meta-analysis further corroborates these findings wherein greater intraindividual variability is longitudinally associated with risk of cognitive decline and conversion to mild cognitive impairment and dementia with a moderate effect size (Mumme et al., Reference Mumme, Pushpanathan, Donaldson, Weinborn, Rainey-Smith, Maruff and Bucks2021). With substantial support in the previous literature, we can conclude that there is enough evidence to suggest that lower intraindividual variability of cognitive performance in older adults reflects healthier cognitive states. Although, we recognize that the field is evolving and there may very well be instances wherein lower intraindividual variability of cognitive performance may not be unequivocally better for all older individuals.

Limitations

One important limitation of the current study is that the reliability of intraindividual variability measures has not been fully established. However, the NIH Toolbox Cognitive Battery as a whole has good to excellent reliability across a large age range (Weintraub et al., Reference Weintraub, Dikmen, Heaton, Tulsky, Zelazo, Bauer, Carlozzi, Slotkin, Blitz, Wallner-Allen, Fox, Beaumont, Mungas, Nowinski, Richler, Deocampo, Anderson, Manly, Borosh and Gershon2013) and the methodology applied in the current study has been used in several other publications (Bauermeister & Bunce, Reference Bauermeister and Bunce2016; Brydges & Bielak, Reference Brydges and Bielak2019; Brydges et al., Reference Brydges, Liu-Ambrose and Bielak2020; Chow et al., Reference Chow, Rabi, Paracha, Vasquez, Hasher, Alain, Anderson and Gamaldo2022), Further, as stated by Hultsch and colleagues, the systematic associations between intraindividual variability measures with personal traits such as cognitive status, age, and physical function provide compelling evidence for the reliability of intraindividual variability measures (Hultsch et al., Reference Hultsch, Strauss, Hunter and MacDonald2008). Importantly, any differences in intraindividual variability related to measurement error would not bias study results for comparisons between groups, given that participants were randomly allocated at baseline. Additionally, we conducted an exploratory secondary analysis of intraindividual variability data and, given the potential for Type I error, our findings must be interpreted with caution. As well, we included highly educated individuals the majority of which were females. Our sample also may have included older adults with some degree of cognitive impairment as 61 (49.2%) individuals scored less than 26/30 on the MoCA, despite not having a formal diagnosis of cognitive impairment or dementia. To minimize the impact of baseline cognitive status on the study outcomes, we adjusted for MoCA scores in the models. Further, as research is still evolving in the field, it remains unclear what impact improving intraindividual variability would have on everyday activities such as driving or financial decision-making. Because of these reasons, future investigations are warranted to replicate our findings and establish whether intraindividual variability improvements reflect improvement in other aspects of health and everyday functioning. Finally, we did not record compliance data for homework completion in the control group (i.e., BAT), which hinders accurate comparison of hours spent working at home between control and computerized cognitive training groups.

Conclusions

In community-dwelling older adults without dementia, computerized cognitive training improved intraindividual variability on a processing speed task. Performing 15 min of brisk walking immediately prior to computerized cognitive training appeared to confer additive benefits on measures of processing speed and attention in this population. Overall, our findings strengthen the notion that a multidomain lifestyle intervention may promote cognitive health via reductions in indices of cognitive variability in otherwise healthy older adults.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617723000577

Acknowledgements

NCBSS is a post-doctoral fellow jointly funded by the Michael Smith Health Research BC, the Canadians for Leading Edge Alzheimer Research, and the Canadian Institutes of Health Research. LTB received funding for this project as a Mitacs Accelerate PhD trainee. TL-A is a Canada Research Chair (Tier I) in Healthy Aging. The authors would like to thank the study participants and their families for their contribution to the study.

Funding statement

This work was supported by Rosetta Stone Canada. They provided the Fit Brains® program and technical support for the program. The funder had no role in study design, study management, data collection, data analysis, data interpretation, and manuscript drafting, manuscript review for important intellectual content, or the decision to submit and publish the manuscript.

Competing interests

The authors report no conflict of interest.

References

Alber, J., Alladi, S., Bae, H.‐J., Barton, D. A., Beckett, L. A., Bell, J. M., Berman, S. E., Biessels, G. J., Black, S. E., Bos, I., Bowman, G. L., Brai, E., Brickman, A. M., Callahan, B. L., Corriveau, R. A., Fossati, S., Gottesman, R. F., Gustafson, D. R., Hachinski, V., …Hainsworth, A. H. (2019). White matter hyperintensities in vascular contributions to cognitive impairment and dementia (VCID): Knowledge gaps and opportunities. Alzheimer’s & Dementia: Translational Research & Clinical Interventions, 5(1), 107117. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trci.2019.02.001 Google ScholarPubMed
Baniqued, P. L., Lee, H., Voss, M. W., Basak, C., Cosman, J. D., DeSouza, S., Severson, J., Salthouse, T. A., & Kramer, A. F. (2013). Selling points: What cognitive abilities are tapped by casual video games? Acta Psychologica, 142(1), 7486. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.ACTPSY.2012.11.009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barha, C. K., Davis, J. C., Falck, R. S., Nagamatsu, L. S., & Liu-Ambrose, T. (2017). Sex differences in exercise efficacy to improve cognition: A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials in older humans. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 46, 7185. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yfrne.2017.04.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barnes, D. E., Yaffe, K., Satariano, W. A., & Tager, I. B. (2003). A longitudinal study of cardiorespiratory fitness and cognitive function in healthy older adults. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 51(4), 459465.10.1046/j.1532-5415.2003.51153.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Basso, J. C., & Suzuki, W. A. (2017). The effects of acute exercise on mood, cognition, neurophysiology, and neurochemical pathways: A review. Brain Plasticity, 2(2), 127152. https://doi.org/10.3233/bpl-160040 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bauermeister, S., & Bunce, D. (2016). Aerobic fitness and intraindividual reaction time variability in middle and old age. Journals of Gerontology - Series B Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 71(3), 431438. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbu152 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baumgart, M., Snyder, H. M., Carillo, M. C., Fazio, S., Kim, H., & Johns, H. (2015). Summary of the evidence on modifiable risk factors for cognitive decline and dementia: A population-based perspective. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 11(6), 718726. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2015.05.016 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bherer, L., Erickson, K. I., & Liu-Ambrose, T. (2013). A review of the effects of physical activity and exercise on cognitive and brain functions in older adults. Journal of Aging Research, 2013, 657508. https://doi.org/10.1155/2013/657508 Google ScholarPubMed
Bielak, A. A. M., & Anstey, K. J. (2019). Covariation of intraindividual variability in cognitive speed and cognitive performance across young, middle, and older adulthood. Developmental Psychology, 55(5), 9941004. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000688 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bielak, A. A. M., & Brydges, C. R. (2019). Can intraindividual variability in cognitive speed be reduced by physical exercise? Results from the LIFE study. Journals of Gerontology - Series B Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 74(8), 13351344. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gby101 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borg, G. (1982). Psychophysical bases of perceived exertion. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 14(5), 377381.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brydges, C. R., & Bielak, A. A. M. (2019). The impact of a sustained cognitive engagement intervention on cognitive variability: The synapse project. Journal of Cognitive Enhancement, 3(4), 365375. https://doi.org/10.1007/s41465-019-00140-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brydges, C. R., Carlson, M. C., Andrews, R. M., Rebok, G. W., Bielak, A. A. M., & Anderson, N. (2021). Using cognitive intraindividual variability to measure intervention effectiveness: Results from the baltimore experience corps trial. Journals of Gerontology - Series B Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 76(4), 661670. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbaa009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brydges, C. R., Liu-Ambrose, T., & Bielak, A. A. M. (2020). Using intraindividual variability as an indicator of cognitive improvement in a physical exercise intervention of older women with mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychology, 34(8), 825834. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000638 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bunce, D., Bielak, A. A. M., Cherbuin, N., Batterham, P. J., Wen, W., Sachdev, P., & Anstey, K. J. (2013). Utility of intraindividual reaction time variability to predict white matter hyperintensities: A potential assessment tool for clinical contexts? Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 19(9), 971976. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617713000830 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Canadian Society of Exercise Physiology (1994). Par-Q & You (pp. 12). Author.Google Scholar
Chow, R., Rabi, R., Paracha, S., Vasquez, B. P., Hasher, L., Alain, C., Anderson, N. D., & Gamaldo, A. (2022). Reaction time intraindividual variability reveals inhibitory deficits in single- and multiple-domain amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Journals of Gerontology - Series B Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 77(1), 7183. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbab051 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colcombe, S., & Kramer, A. F. (2003). Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: A meta-analytic study. Psychological Science, 14(2), 125130. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.t01-1-01430 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cotman, C. W., Berchtold, N. C., & Christie, L. A. (2007). Exercise builds brain health: Key roles of growth factor cascades and inflammation. Trends in Neurosciences, 30(9), 464472. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2007.06.011 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diamond, K., Mowszowski, L., Cockayne, N., Norrie, L., Paradise, M., Hermens, D. F., Lewis, S. J. G., Hickie, I. B., & Naismith, S. L. (2015). Randomized controlled trial of a healthy brain ageing cognitive training program: Effects on memory, mood, and sleep. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 44(4), 11811191. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-142061 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Erickson, K. I., & Kramer, A. F. (2008). Aerobic exercise effects on cognitive and neural plasticity in older adults. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 43(1), 2224. https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsm.2008.052498 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Falck, R. S., Davis, J. C., Best, J. R., Crockett, R. A., & Liu-Ambrose, T. (2019). Impact of exercise training on physical and cognitive function among older adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurobiology of Aging, 79, 119130. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2019.03.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12(3), 189198. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garrett, D. D., Epp, S. M., Kleemeyer, M., Lindenberger, U., & Polk, T. A. (2020). Higher performers upregulate brain signal variability in response to more feature-rich visual input. NeuroImage, 217, 116836. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116836 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garrett, D. D., McIntosh, A. R., & Grady, C. L. (2014). Brain signal variability is parametrically modifiable. Cerebral Cortex, 24(11), 29312940. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bht150 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gates, N. J., Rutjes, A. W. S., Di Nisio, M., Karim, S., Chong, L. Y., March, E., Martínez, G., & Vernooij, R. W. M. (2020, February 27). Computerised cognitive training for 12 or more weeks for maintaining cognitive function in cognitively healthy people in late life. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, 2020. https://doi.org/10.1002/14651858.CD012277.pub3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gates, N. J., Vernooij, R. W. M., Nisio, M. Di, Karim, S., March, E., Martínez, G., & Rutjes, A. W. S. (2019, March 13). Computerised cognitive training for preventing dementia in people with mild cognitive impairment. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, 2019. https://doi.org/10.1002/14651858.CD012279.pub2 Google Scholar
Gavelin, H. M., Dong, C., Minkov, R., Bahar-Fuchs, A., Ellis, K. A., Lautenschlager, N. T., Mellow, M. L., Wade, A. T., Smith, A. E., Finke, C., Krohn, S., & Lampit, A. (2021, March 1). Combined physical and cognitive training for older adults with and without cognitive impairment: A systematic review and network meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Ageing Research Reviews, 66. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.arr.2020.101232 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gavelin, H. M., Lampit, A., Hallock, H., Sabatés, J., & Bahar-Fuchs, A. (2020, June 1). Cognition-oriented treatments for older adults: A systematic overview of systematic reviews. Neuropsychology Review, 30, 167193. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-020-09434-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gonçalves, J. T., Schafer, S. T., & Gage, F. H. (2016, November 3). Adult neurogenesis in the hippocampus: From stem cells to behavior. Cell, 167(4), 897914. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2016.10.021 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graveson, J., Bauermeister, S., McKeown, D., & Bunce, D. (2016, September 1). Intraindividual reaction time variability, falls, and gait in old age: A systematic review. Journals of Gerontology - Series B Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 71, 857864. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbv027 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grydeland, H., Walhovd, K. B., Tamnes, C. K., Westlye, L. T., & Fjell, A. M. (2013). Intracortical myelin links with performance variability across the human lifespan: Results from T1- and T2- weighted MRI myelin mapping and diffusion tensor imaging. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(47), 1861818630. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2811-13.2013 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gustavsson, A., Norton, N., Fast, T., Frölich, L., Georges, J., Holzapfel, D., Kirabali, T., Krolak‐Salmon, P., Rossini, P. M., Ferretti, M. T., Lanman, L., Chadha, A. S., & van der Flier, W. M. (2022). Global estimates on the number of persons across the Alzheimer’s disease continuum. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 19(2), 658670. https://doi.org/10.1002/alz.12694 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haynes, B. I., Bauermeister, S., & Bunce, D. (2017). A systematic review of longitudinal associations between reaction time intraindividual variability and age-related cognitive decline or impairment, dementia, and mortality. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 23(5), 431445. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617717000236 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haynes, B. I., Bunce, D., Kochan, N. A., Wen, W., Brodaty, H., & Sachdev, P. S. (2017). Associations between reaction time measures and white matter hyperintensities in very old age. Neuropsychologia, 96, 249255. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.01.021 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hultsch, D. F., Strauss, E., Hunter, M. A., & MacDonald, S. W. S. (2008). Intraindividual variability, cognition, and aging. In The handbook of aging and cognition (3rd ed., pp. 491556). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Johnson, B. P., Pinar, A., Fornito, A., Nandam, L. S., Hester, R., & Bellgrove, M. A. (2015). Left anterior cingulate activity predicts intra-individual reaction time variability in healthy adults. Neuropsychologia, 72, 2226. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.03.015 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kivipelto, M., Mangialasche, F., & Ngandu, T. (2018). Lifestyle interventions to prevent cognitive impairment, dementia and Alzheimer disease. Nature Reviews Neurology, 14(11), 653666. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41582-018-0070-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kramer, A. F., Humphrey, D. G., Larish, J. F., & Logan, G. D. (1994). Aging and inhibition: Beyond a unitary view of inhibitory processing in attention. Psychology and Aging, 9(4), 491512. https://doi.org/10.1037//0882-7974.9.4.491 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lampit, A., Hallock, H., & Valenzuela, M. (2014). Computerized cognitive training in cognitively healthy older adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis of effect modifiers. PLoS Medicine, 11(11), e1001756. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1001756 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lawton, M. P., & Brody, E. M. (1969). Assessment of older people: Self-maintaining and instrumental activities of daily living. The Gerontologist, 9(3), 179186. https://doi.org/10.1093/geront/9.3_Part_1.179 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lezak, M. (1995). Neuropsychological assessment (3rd ed.). Oxford University Press, Inc.Google Scholar
Liu-Ambrose, T., Best, J. R., Davis, J. C., Eng, J. J., Lee, P. E., Jacova, C., Boyd, L. A., Brasher, P. M., Munkacsy, M., Cheung, W., & Hsiung, G-Y. R. (2016). Aerobic exercise and vascular cognitive impairment. Neurology, 87(20), 20822090. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000003332 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Livingston, G., Huntley, J., Sommerlad, A., Ames, D., Ballard, C., Banerjee, S., Brayne, C., Burns, A., Cohen-Mansfield, J., Cooper, C., Costafreda, S. G., Dias, A., Fox, N., Gitlin, L. N., Howard, R., Kales, H. C., Kivimäki, M., Larson, E. B., Ogunniyi, A., …Mukadam, N. (2020). Dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2020 report of the Lancet Commission. The Lancet, 396(10248), 413446. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30367-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Livingston, G., Sommerlad, A., Orgeta, V., Costafreda, S. G., Huntley, J., Ames, D., Ballard, C., Banerjee, S., Burns, A., Cohen-Mansfield, J., Cooper, C., Fox, N., Gitlin, L. N., Howard, R., Kales, H. C., Larson, E. B., Ritchie, K., Rockwood, K., Sampson, E. L., …Mukadam, N. (2017). Dementia prevention, intervention, and care. The Lancet, 6736(17), 26732734. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(17)31363-6 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lourida, I., Hannon, E., Littlejohns, T. J., Langa, K. M., Hyppönen, E., Kuzma, E., & Llewellyn, D. J. (2019). Association of lifestyle and genetic risk with incidence of dementia. JAMA, 322(5), 430. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2019.9879 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luger, A., Deuster, P. A., Kyle, S. B., Gallucci, W. T., Montgomery, L. C., Gold, P. W., Loriaux, D. L., & Chrousos, G. P. (1987). Acute hypothalamic-pituitary–adrenal responses to the stress of treadmill exercise. New England Journal of Medicine, 316(21), 13091315. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJM198705213162105 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacDonald, S. W. S., Nyberg, L., & Bäckman, L. (2006, August). Intra-individual variability in behavior: Links to brain structure, neurotransmission and neuronal activity. Trends in Neurosciences, 29(8), 474480. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2006.06.011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacDonald, S. W. S., & Stawski, R. S. (2015). Intraindividual variability—An indicator of vulnerability or resilience in adult development and aging? In Handbook of intraindividual variability across the life span (pp. 231257). Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group.Google Scholar
Mehta, D., Jackson, R., Paul, G., Shi, J., & Sabbagh, M. (2017). Why do trials for Alzheimer’s disease drugs keep failing? A discontinued drug perspective for 2010-2015. Expert Opinion on Investigational Drugs, 26(6), 735739. https://doi.org/10.1080/13543784.2017.1323868 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meneghini, V., Barbosa, A. R., Lourenço, C. L. M., & Borgatto, A. F. (2022). Effects of exergaming and resistance training on reaction time and intraindividual variability in older adults: A randomized clinical trial. Ageing International. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12126-022-09491-9 Google Scholar
Mumme, R., Pushpanathan, M., Donaldson, S., Weinborn, M., Rainey-Smith, S. R., Maruff, P., & Bucks, R. S. (2021). Longitudinal association of intraindividual variability with cognitive decline and dementia: A meta-analysis. Neuropsychology, 35(7), 669678. https://doi.org/10.1037/NEU0000746 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nasreddine, Z. S., Phillips, N. A., Bedirian, V., Charbonneau, S., Whitehead, V., Collin, I., Cumming, J. L., & Chertkow, H. (2005). The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: A brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. Journal of The American Geriatrics Society, 53(4), 695699. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nilsson, J., Thomas, A. J., O’Brien, J. T., & Gallagher, P. (2014, February 24). White matter and cognitive decline in aging: A focus on processing speed and variability. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 20, 262267. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617713001458 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Northey, J. M., Cherbuin, N., Pumpa, K. L., Smee, D. J., & Rattray, B. (2017). Exercise interventions for cognitive function in adults older than 50: A systematic review with meta-analysis. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 3(3), 154160. https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsports-2016-096587 Google Scholar
Rosenberg, A., Mangialasche, F., Ngandu, T., Solomon, A., & Kivipelto, M. (2020). Multidomain interventions to prevent cognitive impairment, Alzheimer’s disease, and dementia: From FINGER to World-Wide FINGERS. The Journal of Prevention of Alzheimer’s Disease, 7(1), 2936. https://doi.org/10.14283/jpad.2019.41 Google ScholarPubMed
Sanders, L. M. J., Hortobágyi, T., Gemert, S. B., Van Der Zee, E. A., & Van Heuvelen, M. J. G. (2019, January 1). Dose-response relationship between exercise and cognitive function in older adults with and without cognitive impairment: A systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS ONE, 14. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210036 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shors, T. J., Anderson, M. L., Curlik, D. M., & Nokia, M. S. (2012, February 14). Use it or lose it: How neurogenesis keeps the brain fit for learning. Behavioural Brain Research, 227(2), 450458. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2011.04.023 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, P. J., Blumenthal, J. A., Hoffman, B. M., Cooper, H., Strauman, T. A., Welsh-Bohmer, K., Browndyke, J. N., & Sherwood, A. (2010). Aerobic exercise and neurocognitive performance: A meta-analytic review of randomized controlled trials. Psychosomatic Medicine, 72(3), 239252. https://doi.org/10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181d14633 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
ten Brinke, L. F., Best, J. R., Chan, J. L. C., Ghag, C., Erickson, K. I., Handy, T. C., & Liu-Ambrose, T. (2020). The effects of computerized cognitive training with and without physical exercise on cognitive function in older adults: An 8-week randomized controlled trial. Journals of Gerontology - Series A Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 75(4), 755763. https://doi.org/10.1093/gerona/glz115 Google ScholarPubMed
ten Brinke, L. F., Best, J. R., Crockett, R. A., & Liu-Ambrose, T. (2018). The effects of an 8-week computerized cognitive training program in older adults: A study protocol for a randomized controlled trial. BMC Geriatrics, 18(1), 111. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12877-018-0730-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Villemagne, V. L., Burnham, S., Bourgeat, P., Brown, B., Ellis, K. A., Salvado, O., Szoeke, C., Macaulay, S. L., Martins, R., Maruff, P., Ames, D., Rowe, C. C., & Masters, C. L. (2013). Amyloid β deposition, neurodegeneration, and cognitive decline in sporadic Alzheimer’s disease: A prospective cohort study. The Lancet Neurology, 12(4), 357367. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(13)70044-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vrinceanu, T., Blanchette, C.-A., Intzandt, B., Lussier, M., Pothier, K., Vu, T. T. M., Nigam, A., Bosquet, L., Karelis, A. D., Li, K. Z., Berryman, N., & Bherer, L. (2021). A comparison of the effect of physical activity and cognitive training on dual-task performance in older adults. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, 77(6), 10691079. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbab216 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weintraub, S., Dikmen, S. S., Heaton, R. K., Tulsky, D. S., Zelazo, P. D., Bauer, P. J., Carlozzi, N. E., Slotkin, J., Blitz, D., Wallner-Allen, K., Fox, N. A., Beaumont, J. L., Mungas, D., Nowinski, C. J., Richler, J., Deocampo, J. A., Anderson, J. E., Manly, J. J., Borosh, B., …Gershon, R. C. (2013). Cognition assessment using the NIH Toolbox. Neurology, 80(11 Suppl 3), S54S64. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182872ded CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zelazo, P. D., Anderson, J. E., Richler, J., Wallner-Allen, K., Beaumont, J. L., Conway, K. P., Gershon, R., & Weintraub, S. (2014). NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (CB): Validation of executive function measures in adults. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 20(6), 620629. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617714000472 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Baseline demographic and outcome data

Figure 1

Figure 1. Comparisons between groups in residual ISD after the 8-week intervention period.Note: Lower values indicate reduced variability and thus better performance. Upper panel shows estimated marginal means at 8 weeks, lower panel shows distribution of data across groups. DCCS = Dimensional Change Card Sort; PACPS = Pattern Comparison Processing Speed; BAT = balanced and toned group; FBT = Fit Brains® training group; Ex-FBT = exercise plus Fit Brains® training group; ISD = intraindividual standard deviation. *Significant differences compared with the BAT group at Bonferroni-corrected p < 0.05. **Significant differences compared with the BAT group at Bonferroni-corrected p < 0.01.

Figure 2

Table 2. Log-transformed estimated marginal means and differences between groups after the 8-week intervention period

Supplementary material: File

Boa Sorte Silva et al. supplementary material

Boa Sorte Silva et al. supplementary material
Download Boa Sorte Silva et al. supplementary material(File)
File 20.4 KB