Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-ndw9j Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-16T07:30:15.298Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Camouflaging: psychopathological meanings and clinical relevance in autism spectrum conditions

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 May 2020

Liliana Dell’Osso
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical and Experimental Medicine, University of Pisa, Pisa, Italy
Primo Lorenzi
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical and Experimental Medicine, University of Pisa, Pisa, Italy
Barbara Carpita*
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical and Experimental Medicine, University of Pisa, Pisa, Italy
*
Author for correspondence: Barbara Carpita, Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

In the last decade, increasing literature focused on camouflaging as a strategy adopted to cope with social environment by patients with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). A better understanding of this phenomenon may shed more light on cognitive mechanisms and coping strategies of patients in the autism continuum, eventually leading to reconsider some previous “dogmas” in this field, such as the gender discrepancy in ASD diagnosis. Moreover, shared features can be observed in the camouflaging strategies adopted among the general population, among patients of the autism spectrum, and among patients with different kinds of psychiatric disorders, further challenging our perspectives. Camouflaging behaviors might be considered as a transdiagnostic element, closely associated with the continuous distribution of the autism spectrum among the general and the clinical population.

Type
Editorial
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

“Camouflaging” can be seen as the way in which everyone tries to disguise fewer desirable aspects of his personality. In our everyday life, it may help to “camouflage” difficulties during social situations, or hide behaviors considered socially unprofitable.

Ethologically, camouflaging can be considered an adaptation to the perception (and cognitive mechanisms) of another animal, with the aim to remain untargeted while trying to reach individual objectives. Many species have their own typical “camouflaging strategy,” adopted to cope with everyday life tasks, from surviving to courtship rituals.Reference Merilaita, Scott-Samuel and Cuthill 1

Imitation of gestures and simulated relational performances are common among animals, playing a key role in social learning.Reference Zentall 2 Among humans, the ability of telling lies, supported by higher cognitive functions, is of particular importance and could be severely impaired in certain forms of mental retardation.Reference Dell’Osso, Lorenzi and Carpita 3 So camouflaging may be considered as a strategy supported by a broad set of individual resources, from cognitively implicit behaviors to the most sophisticated and intellectual expressions “to put on the best.”Reference Hull, Petrides and Allison 4

Common Psychopathological Expressions of Camouflaging

Behaviors linked to camouflaging are detectable in different kinds of psychiatric patients. One of the simplest examples could be found in body dysmorphic disorder: in this case, the efforts focus on masking the supposed body’s flaws with several expedients, from clothes to surgical interventions.Reference Grant and Phillips 5 In other conditions, the experience of a perceived weakness is masked by an overcompensatory, opposite behavior: for example, some patients may cope with social anxiety adopting an arrogant or aggressive style.Reference Kashdan and McKnight 6

Another psychopathological symptom similar to camouflaging may be identified in mannerism, which features an exaggerated, abnormal expression of common gestures and behaviors, to the point of interfering with the purpose.Reference Brüne 7 Mannerism is a common symptom in psychotic disorders such as schizophrenia.Reference Brüne 7 According to recent studies, a poorer adjustment to community living was associated with higher cognitive disorganization and worse premorbid functioning among psychotic patients.Reference Bucci, Galderisi and Mucci 8 , Reference Rocca, Galderisi and Rossi 9 It should be noted how mannerism may be the result of a failed attempt of the patient to conform his behavior to that of others.Reference Brüne 7 The proportion, the intensity, and the balance of the imitated behavior are inadequate, and it appears as a pantomime, unmasking its imitative nature: mannerism might be considered as an unsuccessful camouflaging attempt.

Camouflaging in Autism Spectrum Continuum

Recent researches suggest that autism spectrum disorder (ASD) could be the iceberg tip of a broader range of conditions, including also subthreshold manifestations.Reference Dell’Osso, Muti and Carpita 10 Subthreshold autistic traits (AT), initially highlighted among relatives of patients with ASD, seem to be distributed in a continuum across the general population.Reference Carpita, Carmassi and Calderoni 11 Moreover, AT are particularly frequent among patients with other psychiatric disorders, and they seem to be associated with a higher vulnerability toward developing mental disorders and suicidal thoughts.Reference Dell’Osso, Lorenzi and Carpita 3 , Reference Dell’Osso, Muti and Carpita 10 -Reference Dell’Osso, Carpita and Bertelloni 14 In the last decade, increasing literature focused on camouflaging as a strategy adopted to cope with social environment by patients with ASD or AT.Reference Hull, Petrides and Allison 4 It seems to be particularly common among females, possibly due to a higher awareness toward their own social difficulties.Reference Hull, Petrides and Allison 4 , Reference Bargiela, Steward and Mandy 15 -Reference Hull, Mandy and Lai 17 In this framework, camouflaging may be considered as a spectrum of features associated with different conditions, following the continuous distribution of the autism spectrum.

In some cases, the habit of camouflaging become so deeply rooted that the presence of AT may result impossible to detect, covered by a work of masking and compensation. Among these patients, some of the main goals of camouflaging strategies could be identified in reaching a better integration between verbal and nonverbal behaviors, more reciprocity in conversations and relationships, or minimizing responses toward sensory over stimulations.Reference Hull, Petrides and Allison 4 , Reference Grant and Phillips 5 , Reference Hull, Mandy and Lai 17 In order to reach these objectives, patients of the autism spectrum may use in social relationships learned phrases, jokes, or scripts; they often purposely maintain eye contact and personal contact. They may also imitate their pears’ expressions, gestures, and behaviors (or even clothing style and interests). Sometimes personal remarks are accurately avoided, with an over-involvement in maintaining privacy.Reference Hull, Petrides and Allison 4 , Reference Bargiela, Steward and Mandy 15 , Reference Hull, Mandy and Lai 17

The habit to cope with social difficulties relying on camouflaging implies a continuous cognitive and emotive effort, leading to feelings of social overload and exhaustion, stress responses, and clinical expressions of anxiety and mood symptoms.Reference Hull, Petrides and Allison 4 , Reference Bargiela, Steward and Mandy 15 Reference Hull, Mandy and Lai 17 Camouflaging may lead to the construction of a full-fledged other identity, a mask, associated with continuous fear and shame toward the possibility to be “discovered”.Reference Hull, Petrides and Allison 4 , Reference Bargiela, Steward and Mandy 15 -Reference Hull, Mandy and Lai 17 However, it may also result hyper-adaptive in specific life tasks, depending from environmental factors and from the kind and severity of the patient’s vulnerability: for example, spending time preparing topics of conversation before a socially relevant interaction may actually result in a better performance. From this point of view, camouflaging may change a deficit in an efficient tool for competing with others and reaching better, eventually extraordinary, results.Reference Hull, Mandy and Lai 17 , Reference Dell’Osso, Lorenzi and Carpita 18

How Common is Camouflaging Among the General Population and How Much in Autism-Related Conditions?

Limited literature focused on this topic. Hull et al.Reference Hull, Mandy and Lai 17 developed a questionnaire for investigating camouflaging among clinical and general population. They also reported that, while females with AT show higher camouflaging scores than males, no gender difference is detectable among patients without AT.Reference Hull, Lai and Baron-Cohen 19

Camouflaging is supposed to be represented in a continuum among the general population, following the distribution of AT. Although the quality and quantity of camouflaging seem to be linked to the presence of autistic-like features, in the most severe forms of ASD the ability to camouflage disappears.Reference Hull, Mandy and Lai 17 The breaking point might be the severe impairment of cognitive functions, which would prevent the ability to lie or even to build subjective experiences.

May Some Therapeutic Intervention Promote “Social Camouflaging”?

Although the aim of camouflaging strategies is to alter how other people see us, a common consequence seems to be a distortion of self-representation.Reference Hull, Petrides and Allison 4 , Reference Bargiela, Steward and Mandy 15 -Reference Hull, Mandy and Lai 17 Camouflaging may result hyper-adaptive for reaching specific goals, but it implies nevertheless an altered experience of the self.Reference Bargiela, Steward and Mandy 15 -Reference Hull, Mandy and Lai 17 In the last years, the attention toward autistic-like condition progressively increased, and many public health organizations promoted attempts to social support. However, the specific kind of the provided social and rehabilitation support should be considered carefully, avoiding those which may indirectly promote a more complex social camouflaging tendency. This could be the case of some techniques focused on the mnemonic acquisition of performance skills that do not reflect the desires and necessities of the patient, who would still perceive them as “without sense.” These strategies may not necessarily lead to an improvement of the subjective experience, but may risk instead increasing the distress.Reference Hull, Petrides and Allison 4 , Reference Bargiela, Steward and Mandy 15 -Reference Hull, Mandy and Lai 17

Can Camouflaging Explain Missed or Late Diagnosis Among Females With ASD?

The gender ratio for ASD is strikingly high. However, increasing evidence pointed out how females would receive less frequently a diagnosis of ASD with respect to males with comparable levels of symptoms.Reference Hull, Mandy and Lai 17 , Reference Dell’Osso, Lorenzi and Carpita 18 Female presentations seem to be less evident also due to the higher tendency toward camouflaging their social difficulties by imitating others’ behaviors.Reference Hull, Petrides and Allison 4 , Reference Bargiela, Steward and Mandy 15 -Reference Dell’Osso, Lorenzi and Carpita 18 Some authors reported also a significant link between social anxiety and the autism spectrum, especially in high-functioning or subthreshold presentations, which feature a higher awareness of patient’s social difficulties.Reference Carpita, Carmassi and Calderoni 11 Social anxiety disorder implies, as ASD, an impairment of the social brain, but it is more prevalent among females,Reference Marazziti, Abelli and Baroni 20 leading to hypothesize that this diagnosis may mask further gender bias in ASD assessment.Reference Hull, Mandy and Lai 17 , Reference Dell’Osso, Lorenzi and Carpita 18 Moreover, the female-typical restricted interests frequently feature more socially acceptable activities such as spending time with animals, enjoying fictions, or focusing on food and diet.Reference Dell’Osso, Muti and Carpita 10 , Reference Lai, Lombardo and Auyeung 21 Globally, several bias may mask the expression of the autism spectrum among females, including the very diagnostic criteria of ASD, which were built without adequate attention to gender-specific manifestations.

Conclusions

Camouflaging should be considered as an important confounding factor for the recognition of the autism spectrum. It may lead to reconsider some previous “dogmas” in this field, such as the gender discrepancy in ASD diagnosis. Shedding light on this phenomenon may improve our ability to understand the cognitive mechanisms and the coping strategies of patients in the autism continuum. More attention should be paid also to factors involved in the balance between risks and benefits for this kind of behavior. Shared features can be detected in camouflaging strategies observed among the general population, among patients with ASD, and among patients with other psychiatric disorders, leading us to further broaden the perspective. Camouflaging behaviors might be considered as a transdiagnostic element, closely associated with the continuous distribution of the autism spectrum.

The frequently reported presence of AT among patients with other mental disorders led some authors to hypothesize that a neurodevelopmental alteration could be considered at the basis of many psychiatric conditions, while the different psychopathological trajectories would be determined by the interactions among the severity (and the timing) of the alteration itself, the specific neurobiological asset of the patient, and the environment.Reference Dell’Osso, Lorenzi and Carpita 3 , Reference Dell’Osso, Lorenzi and Carpita 18 , Reference Carpita, Muti and Dell’Osso 22 The link between autism spectrum and camouflaging, together with the presence of camouflaging-like behaviors in other psychiatric disorders, further stresses the possibility that AT, and the underlying neurodevelopmental alteration, should be considered the primary source of several kinds of psychopathological expressions among the general and the clinical population.

Disclosures

The authors do not have anything to disclose.

References

Merilaita, S, Scott-Samuel, NE, Cuthill, IC. How camouflage works. Phil Trans R Soc B. 2017;372:20160341.10.1098/rstb.2016.0341CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zentall, TR. Imitation in animals: evidence, function and mechanisms. Cybernet Syst. 2001;32:5396.10.1080/019697201300001812CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dell’Osso, L, Lorenzi, P, Carpita, B. The neurodevelopmental continuum towards a neurodevelopmental gradient hypothesis. J Psychopathol. 2019;25(4):179182.Google Scholar
Hull, L, Petrides, KV, Allison, C, et al. “Putting on my best normal”: social camouflaging in adults with autism spectrum conditions. J Autism Dev Disord. 2017;47:25192534.10.1007/s10803-017-3166-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grant, JE, Phillips, KA. Recognizing and treating body dysmorphic disorder. Ann Clin Psychiatry. 2005;17(4):205210.10.1080/10401230500295313CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kashdan, TB, McKnight, PE. The darker side of social anxiety: when aggressive impulsivity prevails over shy inhibition. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2010;19(1):4750.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brüne, M. Ethological remarks on mannerisms. Conceptualisation and proposal for a definition. Off. Psychopathology. 1998;31(4):188196.10.1159/000029039CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bucci, P, Galderisi, S, Mucci, A, et al. Premorbid academic and social functioning in patients with schizophrenia and its associations with negative symptoms and cognition. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2018;138(3):253266.10.1111/acps.12938CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rocca, P, Galderisi, S, Rossi, A, et al. Disorganization and real-world functioning in schizophrenia: results from the multicenter study of the Italian Network for Research on Psychoses. Schizophr Res. 2018;201:105112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dell’Osso, L, Muti, D, Carpita, B, et al. The Adult Autism Subthreshold Spectrum (AdAS) model: a neurodevelopmental approach to mental disorders. J Psychopathol. 2018;24(3):118124.Google Scholar
Carpita, B, Carmassi, C, Calderoni, S, et al. The broad autism phenotype in real-life: clinical and functional correlates of autism spectrum symptoms and rumination among parents of patients with autism spectrum disorder. CNS Spectr. 2019; [Epub ahead of print]. doi:10.1017/S1092852919001615.Google ScholarPubMed
Dell’Osso, L, Conversano, C, Corsi, M, et al. Polysubstance and behavioral addictions in a patient with bipolar disorder: role of lifetime subthreshold autism spectrum. Case Rep Psychiatry. 2018;2018:1547975.Google Scholar
Dell’Osso, L, Carpita, B, Muti, D, et al. Mood symptoms and suicidality across the autism spectrum. Compr Psychiatry. 2019;91:3438.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dell’Osso, L, Carpita, B, Bertelloni, CA, et al. Subthreshold autism spectrum in bipolar disorder: prevalence and clinical correlates. Psychiatry Res. 2019;281:112605.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bargiela, S, Steward, R, Mandy, W. The experiences of late-diagnosed women with autism spectrum conditions: an investigation of the female autism phenotype. J Autism Dev Disord. 2016;46(10):328194.10.1007/s10803-016-2872-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lai, MC, Lombardo, MV, Ruigrok, ANV, et al. Quantifying and exploring camouflaging in men and women with autism. Autism. 2017;21(6):690702.10.1177/1362361316671012CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hull, L, Mandy, W, Lai, MC, et al. Development and validation of the Camouflaging Autistic Traits Questionnaire (CAT-Q). J Autism Dev Disord. 2019;49(3):819833.10.1007/s10803-018-3792-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dell’Osso, L, Lorenzi, P, Carpita, B. Autistic traits and illness trajectories. Clin Pract Epidemiol Ment Health. 2019;15:9498.10.2174/1745017901915010094CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hull, L, Lai, MC, Baron-Cohen, S, et al. Gender differences in self-reported camouflaging in autistic and non-autistic adults. Autism. 2020;24(2):352363.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marazziti, D, Abelli, M, Baroni, S, et al. Recent findings on the pathophysiology of social anxiety disorder. Clin Neuropsychiatry. 2014;11(2):91100.Google Scholar
Lai, MC, Lombardo, MV, Auyeung, B, et al. Sex/gender differences and autism: setting the scene for future research. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2015;54(1):1124.10.1016/j.jaac.2014.10.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carpita, B, Muti, D, Dell’Osso, L. Oxidative stress, maternal diabetes, and autism spectrum disorders. Oxid Med Cell Longev. 2018;2018:3717215.10.1155/2018/3717215CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed