Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-28T12:13:33.118Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Adolescent friendships predict later resilient functioning across psychosocial domains in a healthy community cohort

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 April 2017

A.-L. van Harmelen*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
R. A. Kievit
Affiliation:
Medical Research Council, Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge, UK Wellcome trust Center for Neuroimaging, University College London, London, UK
K. Ioannidis
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
S. Neufeld
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
P. B. Jones
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
E. Bullmore
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
R. Dolan
Affiliation:
Wellcome trust Center for Neuroimaging, University College London, London, UK
P. Fonagy
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology, University College London, London, UK
I. Goodyer
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
The NSPN Consortium
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK Wellcome trust Center for Neuroimaging, University College London, London, UK
*
*Address for correspondence: A.-L. van Harmelen, Ph.D., Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK. (Email: [email protected])
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Adolescence is a key time period for the emergence of psychosocial and mental health difficulties. To promote adolescent adaptive (‘resilient’) psychosocial functioning (PSF), appropriate conceptualisation and quantification of such functioning and its predictors is a crucial first step. Here, we quantify resilient functioning as the degree to which an individual functions better or worse than expected given their self-reported childhood family experiences, and relate this to adolescent family and friendship support.

Method

We used Principal Component and regression analyses to investigate the relationship between childhood family experiences and PSF (psychiatric symptomatology, personality traits and mental wellbeing) in healthy adolescents (the Neuroscience in Psychiatry Network; N = 2389; ages 14–24). Residuals from the relation between childhood family experiences and PSF reflect resilient functioning; the degree to which an individual is functioning better, or worse, than expected given their childhood family experiences. Next, we relate family and friendship support with resilient functioning both cross-sectionally and 1 year later.

Results

Friendship and family support were positive predictors of immediate resilient PSF, with friendship support being the strongest predictor. However, whereas friendship support was a significant positive predictor of later resilient functioning, family support had a negative relationship with later resilient PSF.

Conclusions

We show that friendship support, but not family support, is an important positive predictor of both immediate and later resilient PSF in adolescence and early adulthood. Interventions that promote the skills needed to acquire and sustain adolescent friendships may be crucial in increasing adolescent resilient PSF.

Type
Original Articles
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017

Introduction

Adolescence is a key developmental time period for the emergence of psychosocial difficulties, and mental health disorders (Thapar et al. Reference Thapar, Collishaw, Pine and Thapar2012; Blakemore & Mills, Reference Blakemore and Mills2014). To promote adaptive psychosocial functioning (PSF) during adolescence, appropriate conceptualisation and quantification of resilient functioning and its predictors is a crucial first step. In psychiatry, resilience refers to “a dynamic process wherein individuals display positive adaptation despite experiences of significant adversity or trauma” (Luthar & Cicchetti, Reference Luthar and Cicchetti2000). In the general population, it is well established that negative childhood experiences such as parental discord and/or lack of parental affection can have a negative impact on adolescent PSF (Egeland, Reference Egeland2009; Trocmé et al. Reference Trocmé, Fallon, Maclaurin, Chamberland, Chabot and Esposito2011; van Harmelen, Reference van Harmelen2013; Harpur et al. Reference Harpur, Polek and van Harmelen2015; Stoltenborgh et al. Reference Stoltenborgh, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Alink and van IJzendoorn2015). Adolescent resilient PSF may therefore be seen as reflecting positive adaptation compared to others with similar experiences in the family environment. However, individuals with comparable experiences may not appraise their experiences in the same way (Rutter, Reference Rutter1985, Reference Rutter2012). For instance, perceived levels of threat, rather than actual threat, determine later stress reactivity (van Wingen et al. Reference van Wingen, Geuze, Vermetten and Fernández2011). Therefore, including self-reported appraisal of childhood family experiences may contribute to a more valid and quantifiable measure of adolescent resilient functioning.

Resilience captures positive adaptation across emotional, cognitive, behavioural and social domains of functioning (Masten, Reference Masten2015), and should be relevant to the environmental events and difficulties experienced (Luthar et al. Reference Luthar, Cicchetti and Becker2000). From this multidimensional perspective the presence of personal impairment or psychopathology does not necessarily preclude concurrent resilient functioning (Luthar et al. Reference Luthar, Cicchetti and Becker2000). For example, an adolescent can suffer considerable distress after a personal loss, but simultaneously continue to attend school and learn and see friends and can therefore be considered to be functioning ‘resiliently’ in those domains despite experiencing berievement. This multidimensional perspective indicates that a valid measure of adolescent resilient functioning in the general population should capture adaptive behaviour across a comprehensive range and level of psychosocial domains. Furthermore, resilient functioning is not a personality trait that is constant over time (Luthar & Cicchetti, Reference Luthar and Cicchetti2000; Rutter, Reference Rutter2012; Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti2013; Masten, Reference Masten2015). Rather, resilient functioning waxes and wanes, possibly under the influence of protective factors such as family and friendship support (Rutter, Reference Rutter1985; Afifi & Macmillan, Reference Afifi and Macmillan2011; Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti2013; van Harmelen et al. Reference van Harmelen, Gibson, St Clair, Owens, Brodbeck, Dunn, Lewis, Croudace, Jones, Kievit and Goodyer2016). Therefore, having low resilience at some time does not preclude the presence of future resilience or vice versa. Consequently, it is important to study adolescent resilient functioning, and its influences, over time (Bonanno et al. Reference Bonanno, Romero and Klein2015). Understanding how adolescent resilient functioning varies over time and revealing how various factors influence such variation remains to be fully elucidated.

Adolescent friendships and family support are important protective factors after early life stress (Rutter, Reference Rutter1985, Reference Rutter2012, van Harmelen et al. Reference van Harmelen, Gibson, St Clair, Owens, Brodbeck, Dunn, Lewis, Croudace, Jones, Kievit and Goodyer2016). Recently, we showed that adolescent family support reduces later depressive symptoms after differential levels of childhood family adversity, whereas adolescent friendship support reduced later depressive symptoms after childhood family adversity and/or peer victimisation (van Harmelen et al. Reference van Harmelen, Gibson, St Clair, Owens, Brodbeck, Dunn, Lewis, Croudace, Jones, Kievit and Goodyer2016). These findings support the stress-support matching hypothesis; support should match the type of adversity experienced in order to be most beneficial (Cohen & Wills, Reference Cohen and Wills1985). This is also evidence for multidimensionality of resilient functioning and further evidences the value of developing a resilient index across domains of experiences. Such a measure has however yet to be reported. Thus, the concurrent and predictive role of adolescent friendships and family supports on adolescent psychosocial resilient functioning across multiple domains (whilst taking self-reported childhood family experiences into account) is unknown.

Here, we investigate the relationship between adolescent family and friendship support on concurrent and prospective adolescent resilient PSF in a community sample (N = 2389) of healthy adolescents and young adults (ages 14–24) from the longitudinal Neuroscience in Psychiatry Network (NSPN; http://www.NSPN.org). We quantify a measure of resilient functioning by taking into account both functioning across multiple psychosocial domains (i.e. psychiatric symptoms, personality traits and mental wellbeing) and self-reported experiences of the family environment in childhood in a healthy population. This allows us to create a multidimensional index of functioning from which we can ascertain the degree to which an individual functions better or worse than expected given their family environment in early life. Finally, we test whether such functioning is associated specifically with family and friendship factors concurrently and prospectively 1 year later using path models.

Methods

Sample

Participants in this report were part of the NSPN study cohort. NSPN is a multi-centre accelerated longitudinal community cohort study focusing on normative adolescent to young adult (‘adolescent’) development between the ages of 14 and 24. The NSPN cohort (N = 2389) completed a home questionnaire pack (HPQ) at baseline (Time 1), and ~1 year later [median = 1 year, mean = 1.11 (s.e. = 0.01) year, min–max: 0.91–2.69 years], N = 1674 individuals from the NSPN cohort completed the same HPQ at Time 2.

For our cross-sectional analyses we had complete data on all measures used (online Supplementary Table S1) for N = 1890. This cross-sectional sample did not differ from the entire NSPN cohort (N = 2389) on age [t (4055) = 0.02, p = 0.98], gender (χ2 = 0.01, df = 1, p = 0.91), socio-economic status [SES; index of multiple deprivation based on participant postcodes; t (4058) = 1.416, p = 0.16], nor ethnicity (χ2 = 4.19, df = 5, p = 0.52). Overall, online Supplementary Table S1 shows that this sample (N = 1890) can be described as a healthy sample reporting low levels of psychopathological symptoms, behaviours and personality traits, and average mental wellbeing scores.

For our longitudinal analyses we had complete data for N = 1093. This longitudinal sample was not different from the sample used in our cross-sectional analyses (N = 1890), nor the entire NSPN cohort (N = 2389) in terms of age [t (2058 & 2218)< −0.74, p > 0.46], and ethnicity distribution [i.e. N = 1890: (χ2 = 2.73, df = 5, p = 0.74); N = 2389: (χ2 = 9.15, df = 5, p = 0.10)]. However, there were slightly more females in the longitudinal sample (N = 1093; 57% females) when compared with the cross-sectional sample (N = 1890) and the NSPN cohort (N = 2389) (χ2 > 4.16, df = 1, p < 0.04), that both had 53% females. Finally, the longitudinal sample (N = 1093) had similar SES compared with the cross-sectional sample (N = 1890) t (2364) = −1.5, p = 0.13). However, the longitudinal sample (N = 1093) had lower SES compared with the NSPN cohort (N = 2389) (mean's 15.5 & 16.9, t (2237) = −2.82, p = 0.005).

Measures

Psychosocial functioning (PSF)

Negative family environments in early life form a risk factor for maladaptive-psychiatric symptomatology (van Harmelen et al. Reference van Harmelen, De Jong, Glashouwer, Spinhoven, Penninx and Elzinga2010), -personality traits (Hart et al. Reference Hart, Binggeli, Brassard, Bingelli and Brassard1997; Rogosch & Cicchetti, Reference Rogosch and Cicchetti2004) and reduced overall mental wellbeing (Hart et al. Reference Hart, Binggeli, Brassard, Bingelli and Brassard1997). Therefore, we focussed our measure of resilient functioning relative to these psychosocial domains to assess overall ‘PSFFootnote Footnote 1 . To do so, we included sum scores of all questionnaires (assessed both at times 1 and 2) that focussed on:

Psychopathological symptoms: The mood and feelings questionnaire (Angold et al. Reference Angold, Costello, Messer and Pickles1995), Revised Children's Manifest Anxiety Scale RCMAS self-report questionnaire (Reynolds & Richmond, Reference Reynolds and Richmond1997), Short Leyton Obsessional Inventory (Bamber et al. Reference Bamber, Tamplin, Park, Kyte and Goodyer2002), Kessler Psychological Distress scale (K10; Kessler et al. Reference Kessler, Andrews, Colpe, Hiripi, Mroczek, Normand, Walters and Zaslavsky2002), behaviours checklist.

Personality characteristics: The Antisocial Process Screening Device (Frick et al. Reference Frick, Bodin and Barry2000), The Child and Adolescent Dispositions Scale (Lahey et al. Reference Lahey, Applegate, Chronis, Jones, Williams, Loney and Waldman2008), the inventory of Callous-unemotional traits (ICU) to measure callous and unemotional traits (Roose et al. Reference Roose, Bijttebier, Decoene, Claes and Frick2010), the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (SPQ) (Raine, Reference Raine1991) and the Barratt Impulsivity Scale (BIS) (Stanford et al. Reference Stanford, Mathias, Dougherty, Lake, Anderson and Patton2009).

Mental wellbeing: the Warwick-Edinburgh Mental Well Being Scale (WEMWBS) (Tennant et al. Reference Tennant, Hiller, Fishwick, Platt, Joseph, Weich, Parkinson, Secker and Stewart-Brown2007).

More information about these measures is provided in the online Supplement.

Childhood family experiences

Appraisal of early life parenting behaviours were measured at baseline and time 2 with two self-report measures; the Alabama Parenting Questionnaire (APQ) and the measure of parenting styles (MOPS).

Measure of Parenting Style: The MOPS is a 12-item self-report measure that assesses perceived parenting styles across three domains; indifference, overcontrol and abuse (Parker et al. Reference Parker, Roussos, Hadzi-Pavlovic, Mitchell, Wilhelm and Austin1997). Participants were asked to rate both their mother's and father's parenting behaviour on 15 statements, on a 4-point scale. The full response range is ‘not true at all’, ‘slightly true’, ‘moderately true’, ‘extremely true’. The ‘abuse’ scale consisted of five items, asking whether maternal/paternal behaviours were verbally abusive, unpredictable, physically violent, elicited feelings of danger or elicited feelings of lack of safety. The ‘overly controlling’ scale consisted of four items where maternal/paternal behaviour was overprotective, over controlling, critical, or made the participant feel guilty. Finally, the ‘indifference’ scale assessed six items of maternal/paternal behaviour where the parent was ‘ignoring, uncaring, rejecting, uninterested in, would forget about, or would leave the participant on his/her own a lot. Sum scores to responses in these items were calculated with higher scores representing more abusive, over controlling or indifferent behaviour reported. Internal consistency was good for the maternal subscales (Cronbach's alpha maternal over control = 0.70, indifference = 0.86, abuse = 0.78). For paternal parenting, the internal consistency at baseline ranged from acceptable (Cronbach's alpha paternal over control = 0.65) to excellent (Cronbach's alphas paternal abuse = 0.88, paternal indifference = 0.93).

Alabama Parenting Questionnaire: The APQ measures parenting practices. We used the nine-item short-form (Elgar et al. Reference Elgar, Waschbusch, Dadds and Sigvaldason2006), and added the ‘Corporal Punishment’ (three items) and ‘Involvement’ scale (three items). Participants were asked to rate how typical each item occurred or used to occur in their family home on a 5-point scale ranging from ‘never’, ‘almost never’, ‘sometimes’, ‘oftentoalways’. We calculated sum scores for the five subscales: Positive Parenting, Inconsistent Discipline, Poor Supervision, Involvement, and Corporal Punishment, with higher scores reflecting higher frequency of the behaviour. Thus, high scores can indicate positive parenting (i.e. involvement, positive parenting) or negative parenting (i.e. inconsistent discipline, poor supervision, corporal punishment). Internal consistency at baseline was acceptable (inconsistent discipline & poor supervision: Cronbach's alpha > 0.62) and good (positive parenting, involvement, Corporal Punishment Cronbach's alpha > 0.71). Note that all results remained when the positive parenting scores (APQ positive parenting and APQ involvement) were removed from the analyses.

Predictors of resilient functioning

Family Assessment Device (FAD):

Adolescent family support was assessed at baseline and time 2 with the McMaster FAD-General Functioning Scale (FAD-GF; Epstein et al. Reference Epstein, Baldwin and Bishop1983), administered to adolescents. The FAD-GF is a 12-item self-report questionnaire where respondents rate statements such as ‘we can express our feelings to each other’ or ‘there are lots of bad feelings in the family’. Responses ranged from ‘Strongly Agree’ to ‘Strongly Disagree’. The FAD-GF yields an estimate of overall family functioning (Miller et al. Reference Miller, Epstein, Bishop and Keitner1985). In our analyses, high scores reflect a positive family environment (‘family support’). Internal consistency at baseline was very high (Cronbach's alpha = 0.92).

Cambridge Friendship Questionnaire (CFQ): Perceived quality of friendships at baseline and time 2 were assessed with the self-report CFQ (J. Memarzia et al. unpublished observations; van Harmelen et al. Reference van Harmelen, Gibson, St Clair, Owens, Brodbeck, Dunn, Lewis, Croudace, Jones, Kievit and Goodyer2016). The CFQ is an eight-item questionnaire assessing the number, availability and quality of friendships (e.g. ‘Do you feel that your friends understand you?’, ‘Are you happy with the number of friends that you've got at the moment’, ‘Can you confide in your friends?’). Higher scores indicate better perceived overall quality of friendships (i.e. ‘Friendships’). The CFQ has good measurement invariance and external validity, and adequate test–retest reliability across 2-week intervals (Kappa = 0.80) (J. Memarzia et al. unpublished observations). Within NSPN, baseline internal consistency was good (Cronbach's alpha = 0.72).

Stats and results

All analyses were conducted in R version 3.03 (Warm Puppy), using the packages Dplyr (Wickham & Romain, Reference Wickham and Romain2016), Psych (Revelle, Reference Revelle2014), Lavaan (Rosseel, Reference Rosseel2012) and ggplot2 (Wickham, Reference Wickham2009). All data and code for the below analyses are available from (http://www.annelauravanharmelen.com/data & https://figshare.com/authors/_/1376682).

To calculate a multi-modal composite score for PSF we conducted a principal component analysis (PCA) for PSF on standard-normally transformed individual total scores on the MFQ, RCMAS, S-LOI, K10, BCL, APSD, CADS, ICU, SPQ, BIS-11 and WEMBES. Similarly, we conducted a PCA, including standard-normally transformed sum scores for the MOPS the APQ subscales to create a composite score for childhood family experiences. From both analyses, we extracted individual scores for the first component to reflect individual current PSF and recalled childhood family experience scores. Next, we regressed the PSF component score against the childhood family experiences score, testing for possible linear, quadratic or cubic relationships. From the best-fitting regression we extracted the residual scores as these reflect a spectrum ranging from risk to resilient functioning: the extent to which an individual has better, or worse, PSF outcomes than the average score expected given their childhood family experiences (see for a similar approach Bowes et al. Reference Bowes, Maughan, Caspi, Moffitt and Arseneault2010; Miller-Lewis et al. Reference Miller-Lewis, Searle, Sawyer, Baghurst and Hedley2013; Sapouna & Wolke, Reference Sapouna and Wolke2013; Collishaw et al. Reference Collishaw, Hammerton, Mahedy, Sellers, Owen, Craddock, Thapar, Harold, Rice and Thapar2016). For parsimony, we will refer to these scores as ‘resilient functioning’ with higher scores reflecting better (conditional) PSF outcomes.

Next, we predicted resilient functioning from adolescent family and friendship support. Age, gender (coded 0-1, 1 being males) and socio-economic status (SES) were specified as covariates. Note that all results remained the same when these covariates were not included in the regressions. We examined these relations cross-sectionally at baseline in N = 1890 using multiple regression.

Finally, we investigated whether the relationships between friendship and family support and our multidimensional measure of resilient functioning is dependent on the cross-sectional (i.e. simultaneous) timing of assessments, (potentially reflecting reporting bias) or whether these relationships also appeared over time. Therefore, we conducted longitudinal analyses using Structural Equation Modelling (SEM) in Lavaan (Rosseel, Reference Rosseel2012). We specified a full identified model that tested the relations and interrelations of baseline and later friendships, family support and resilient functioning. In this model, gender, age and SES were specified as covariates on friendships, family support and resilient functioning at baseline and follow-up.

Results

Resilient functioning; functioning that is better than expected given one's childhood family experiences

A PCA for PSF (MFQ, RCMAS, S-LOI, K10, BCL, APSD, CADS, ICU, SPQ, BIS-11 and WEMBES) revealed a first component that explained 44% variance. Higher scores on the PSF factor suggest better PSF (see online Supplementary Table S2). The PCA for child family experiences revealed a first component that explained 37% variance in the MOPS and APQ subscales (online Supplementary Table S2 for loadings). The childhood family experiences principle component scores were inverted so that a higher score reflects more negative family experiences. We next regressed the childhood family experiences component score on the component score for PSF. This relationship could best be described as quadratic (Fig. 1) (Est = −0.76, s.e. = 0.03, t = −24.87, p < 2 × 10−16, quadratric term: Est = 0.05, s.e. = 0.006, t = 7.32, p = 3.66 × 10−13, additional information in online Supplement). Next, individual residual scores were extracted from this relationship as these residuals reflect degree of risk to resilient functioning: the extent to which an individual functioned better than expected (‘high, or resilient’; green lines Fig. 1), or worse than expected (‘low or riskred lines Fig. 1), given their childhood family experiences. Note that higher residual scores reflect more resilient functioning.

Fig. 1. Relationship between PSF and Childhood family experiences in N = 1890.

Association between adolescent friendships and family support and resilient functioning

Adolescent friendships had a strong positive association with concurrent resilient functioning; more friendship support related to more resilient functioning [r = 0.43, t (1834) = 20.57, p < 2.2 × 10−16, Fig. 2 a]. Similarly, family support was positively associated with concurrent resilient functioning [r = 0.23, t (1853) = 10.37, p < 2.2 × 10−16, Fig. 2 b].

Fig. 2. The relationship between friendships (a) and family support (b) and baseline resilient functioning (N = 1890).

Friendships and family support were correlated ]r = 0.39, t (1834) = 18.67, p < 2.2 × 10−16]. Therefore, we next investigated their unique relations with resilient functioning using multiple regression. We defined friendships, family support, gender, age and SES as predictors of resilient functioning. This analysis showed that friendships and family support were both positive predictors of resilient scores, with friendships being the strongest predictor (Table 1). Furthermore, age and male gender, but not SES, were also associated with resilient functioning.

Table 1. Predictors of resilient functioning at baseline (time 1)

Longitudinal predictors of resilient PSF

To investigate the relationship between friendships and family support at baseline (time 1) with resilient functioning at time 2 (~1 year later) we recalculated resilient functioning scores in a subset of the sample that had complete data on all measures at both times (N = 1093; see online Supplement and Supplementary Table S4 for details). Resilient functioning at times 1 and 2 had a strong positive association (r = 0.66, t = 28.93, df = 1091, p < 2.2 × 10−16), suggesting that resilient functioning is relatively stable over the course of 1 year in our sample.

A path analysis showed that adolescent friendships and resilient functioning were significant positive predictors of psychosocial resilient functioning over the course of 1 year (Table 2 and Fig. 3). In contrast, adolescent family support was negatively associated with later psychosocial resilient functioning. Friendships at time 2 were only positively predicted by baseline resilient functioning; whereas family support at time 2 was positively predicted by baseline family and friendship support. Interestingly, family support at time 2 was negatively predicted by baseline resilient functioning. Note that as this path model is saturated, model fit is not informative, but included in the caption of Table 2 for completeness.

Fig. 3. Significant paths in the Structural Equation Model. For reasons of parsimony we only depict significant positive (green) or negative (red) paths (unstandardised Estimates and s.e.). Thicker lines indicate stronger associations.

Table 2. Predictors of later resilient functioning (N = 1093)

Discussion

Here we examine predictors of adolescent resilient functioning across a range of psychosocial domains (‘PSF’; i.e. psychiatric symptoms, personality traits and mental wellbeing), whilst taking into account individual childhood family experiences. We create a measure of resilient PSF in three steps. First, we use a data-reduction technique (i.e. PCA) to establish individual composite scores for PSF, and childhood family experiences. Second, we regressed PSF on childhood family experiences. Third, we extract residual scores from this relationship as these reflect individual level of psychosocial resilient functioning: the degree to which a participant is functioning better or worse than expected based on his/her childhood family experiences: see for a similar approach (Bowes et al. Reference Bowes, Maughan, Caspi, Moffitt and Arseneault2010; Miller-Lewis et al. Reference Miller-Lewis, Searle, Sawyer, Baghurst and Hedley2013; Sapouna & Wolke, Reference Sapouna and Wolke2013; Collishaw et al. Reference Collishaw, Hammerton, Mahedy, Sellers, Owen, Craddock, Thapar, Harold, Rice and Thapar2016). We found that childhood family experiences have a significant association with PSF in our community sample of healthy adolescents (N = 1890). Specifically, recalling more negative family experiences was associated with worsening current PSF, supporting previous studies (Gilbert et al. Reference Gilbert, Widom, Browne, Fergusson, Webb and Janson2009; van Harmelen et al. Reference van Harmelen, De Jong, Glashouwer, Spinhoven, Penninx and Elzinga2010). We then related adolescent friendship and family support with continuous risk to resilient PSF measure. We found that adolescent friendship support, but not adolescent family support, was positively related with immediate and later resilient PSF.

Friendship and family support were both positive predictors of immediate resilient PSF. Notably, friendship support was a stronger predictor of immediate resilient functioning than family support, which is in line with the notion that adolescents are especially sensitive to their peer environment (Crone & Dahl, Reference Crone and Dahl2012). Furthermore, we found that adolescent friendship support was also a positive predictor of later resilient PSF, which was apparent even after accounting for the effect of baseline resilient functioning and family support. These findings suggest that friendship support may be an important protective factor in adolescence. Our findings corroborate and extend those that showed that adolescent friendship support promotes subsequent resilient functioning in those exposed to negative childhood family environments (Collishaw et al. Reference Collishaw, Pickles, Messer, Rutter, Shearer and Maughan2007; Powers et al. Reference Powers, Ressler and Bradley2009; van Harmelen et al. Reference van Harmelen, Gibson, St Clair, Owens, Brodbeck, Dunn, Lewis, Croudace, Jones, Kievit and Goodyer2016).

The exact mechanisms through which adolescent friendships increases resilient functioning are yet unknown. One potential explanation may be that our friendships score captures individual skills that promote social competence, such as social interaction and relationship building, and social competence could mediate the link between resilient functioning and friendship interactions. However, in our model, the relationship between baseline resilient functioning and later friendships was weak at best. This suggests that our interpretation that friendship promote resilient functioning over time is unlikely to be explained by the alternative notion that prior resilient functioning promotes better social competence (and friendships) and thereby later resilience function. Future studies should however test the specific role of social competence in the link between resilient functioning and subsequent friendships. Other explanations for the link between friendships and resilient functioning may come from studies that suggest that adolescent friendship support may increase resilient functioning through offering companionship (Cohen & Wills, Reference Cohen and Wills1985) when these interactions are pro-social, as adolescent prosocial peer relationships, but not anti-social relationships, reduced later behavioural problems (Fergusson & Lynskey, Reference Fergusson and Lynskey1996; Fergusson et al. Reference Fergusson, Lynskey and Horwood1996). Friendships may also increase resilient functioning is through increasing interpersonal skills (Buhrmester, Reference Buhrmester1990), and through supporting social decision making skills (Jehn & Shah, Reference Jehn and Shah1997). Additionally, adolescent friendships may reduce feelings of loneliness (Parker & Asher, Reference Parker and Asher1993), and dampen stress responses (Cohen & Wills, Reference Cohen and Wills1985; Masten et al. Reference Masten, Telzer, Fuligni, Lieberman and Eisenberger2012). Furthermore, friendship support may increase resilient functioning through reducing negative experiences with peers (Pellegrini & Bartini, Reference Pellegrini and Bartini2000).

Overall therefore these emotion–cognition mechanisms accruing via positive adolescent friendships may increase resilient functioning through updating negative self-cognitions. Negative self-cognitions are found in children that have low peer support; those that have experienced peer victimisation (Sinclair et al. Reference Sinclair, Cole, Dukewich, Felton, Weitlauf, Maxwell, Tilghman-osborne and Jacky2012), or report to be lonely (Vanhalst et al. Reference Vanhalst, Soenens, Luyckx, Van Petegem, Weeks and Asher2015). Negative self-cognitions colour individuals' appraisal and behaviour in interpersonal situations and negatively influence individuals' memories of these situations (Beck, Reference Beck2008). Negative self-cognitions mediate the link between very negative family environments and poor mental health (van Harmelen et al. Reference van Harmelen, De Jong, Glashouwer, Spinhoven, Penninx and Elzinga2010). Adolescent friendship support may offer a unique opportunity to learn from positive peer experiences, which perhaps results in a more positive update of self-cognitions. Examining the potential mechanisms through which adolescent friendship support increases psychosocial resilient functioning is an important avenue for future research.

The relationship between adolescent family support and resilient functioning across psychosocial domains appeared to be more complicated in our sample. Although family support had a positive relationship with immediate resilient PSF, family support was negatively related with later adolescent resilient functioning (when baseline resilient functioning and friendship support were taken into account). These findings are in line with findings that family support is not linked to positive adaptation in more severely maltreated children than those studied here (Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti2013). It may be that, in adolescence, family involvement is not adaptive, especially in the context of a negative family environment. In line with this idea, adolescent family support was not associated resilient functioning when peer relationships were taken into account (Fergusson & Lynskey, Reference Fergusson and Lynskey1996). Similarly, family support was not associated with teacher-reported mental health resilient functioning in young children with parental report of high cumulative family adversity (Miller-Lewis et al. Reference Miller-Lewis, Searle, Sawyer, Baghurst and Hedley2013). Although, family support was positively related with mental health resilient functioning if functioning was reported by parents in these children (Miller-Lewis et al. Reference Miller-Lewis, Searle, Sawyer, Baghurst and Hedley2013). Finally, our findings are in line with those that friendships, but not family support, are related with self-reported resilient functioning rates on a resilient functioning questionnaire in young adults with histories of child abuse (Howell & Miller-Graff, Reference Howell and Miller-Graff2014). However, our findings are in contrast with those that suggest that family support is predictive of childhood and early adolescent (ages 13–14) resilient functioning against depressive symptoms after child adversity (Bowes et al. Reference Bowes, Maughan, Caspi, Moffitt and Arseneault2010; Sapouna & Wolke, Reference Sapouna and Wolke2013). These findings also contrast our previous report in a different sample that adolescent family support at age 14 reduces adolescent depressive symptoms at age 17 after CFA (van Harmelen et al. Reference van Harmelen, Gibson, St Clair, Owens, Brodbeck, Dunn, Lewis, Croudace, Jones, Kievit and Goodyer2016). Taken together, whereas previous studies suggest that early adolescent family support may predict resilient functioning against later depression, our current findings suggest that adolescent family support is not related with later adolescent resilient functioning when resilient functioning is assessed across multiple psychosocial domains.

Contrary to common concepts of resilient functioning where only the outcomes (e.g. absence of psychopathology, above average functioning) are taken into account [e.g., see for an overview (Klika & Herrenkohl, Reference Klika and Herrenkohl2013)], we use an approach that allows individuals who have moderate outcomes in the face of very negative childhood family experiences to be included as ‘resilient’ (Bowes et al. Reference Bowes, Maughan, Ball, Shakoor, Ouellet-Morin, Caspi, Moffitt and Arseneault2013; Miller-Lewis et al. Reference Miller-Lewis, Searle, Sawyer, Baghurst and Hedley2013; Sapouna & Wolke, Reference Sapouna and Wolke2013; Collishaw et al. Reference Collishaw, Hammerton, Mahedy, Sellers, Owen, Craddock, Thapar, Harold, Rice and Thapar2016). This approach paints a more complete picture of adolescent PSF. A limitation of this approach is that taking the subjectivity of self-reported childhood family experiences into account when quantifying resilient functioning may be inherently biased: those that are highly resilient may report more positive childhood family experiences, whereas those that are less resilient may report more negative childhood family experiences. However, current psychopathology has not been found to bias self-report of child abuse and neglect (Spinhoven et al. Reference Spinhoven, Elzinga, Hovens, Roelofs, Zitman, van Oppen and Penninx2010). In fact, a previous work suggests that negative childhood experiences are more likely to be underreported rather than overreported (Brewin, Reference Brewin2007). Finally, even if those with low psychosocial resilient functioning overreported negative family experiences, and those with high psychosocial resilient functioning over reported positive family experiences this would only lead to a reduction in power to find associations with resilient functioning. For these reasons, it is unlikely that this limitation would explain our current findings. Finally, an important limitation is that, on average, our sample reported only low levels of negative family experiences at best, and the childhood family experiences score explained only moderate variance r = 0.37% in the MOPS and APQ assessments. Future studies should investigate whether friendship support similarly predicts resilient PSF after more severe childhood family experiences including a sufficient sample of adolescents with manifest histories of physical and sexual maltreatment in childhood that are not studied in this investigation.

In sum, we quantify resilient functioning by taking into account functioning across a range of psychosocial domains and individual childhood family experiences. We show that friendship support, but not family support, is an important positive predictor of both immediate and later resilient PSF in adolescence and early adulthood. Therefore, interventions that promote the skills needed to acquire and sustain adolescent affiliate friendships may be crucial in increasing adolescent resilient functioning.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291717000836.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by a strategic award from the Wellcome Trust to the University of Cambridge and University College London (095844/Z/11/Z), a Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research Rubicon grant (AlvH, NO 446-13-006), and a Royal Society Dorothy Hodgkin Fellowship (AlvH; No DH150176). Study data were collected and managed using REDCap electronic data capture tools hosted at the University of Cambridge. P.F is in receipt of a National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Senior Investigator Award (NF-SI-0514-10157)and is in part supported by the NIHR Collaboration for Leadership in Applied Health Research and Care (CLAHRC) North Thames at Barts Health NHS Trust. E.T.B. is employed half-time by the University of Cambridge and half-time by GlaxoSmithKline; he holds stock in GlaxoSmithKline. The funders had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; and preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript. We would like to thank the NSPN sample for their participation. This study was designed and analysed by AlvH. The corresponding author (AlvH) had full access to the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. All authors contributed to the interpretation of the data, the writing of the paper, and approved the final manuscript.

Declaration of Interest

None.

Ethical Standard

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

Footnotes

The notes appear after the main text.

1 As our aim is to capture resilient functioning in its broadest sense, we choose to incorporate the more stable personality traits in our measure of PSF. Note that our results remain the same when we repeated all analyses, whilst only including psychiatric symptoms and mental wellbeing in our PSF variable.

References

Afifi, TO, Macmillan, HL (2011). Resilience following child maltreatment: a review of protective factors. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry 56, 266272.Google Scholar
Angold, A, Costello, E, Messer, S, Pickles, A (1995). Development of a short questionnaire for use in epidemiological studies of depression in children and adolescents. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research 5, 237249.Google Scholar
Bamber, D, Tamplin, A, Park, RJ, Kyte, ZA, Goodyer, IM (2002). Development of a short leyton obsessional inventory for children and adolescents. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 41, 12461252.Google Scholar
Beck, AT (2008). The evolution of the cognitive model of depression and its neurobiological correlates. The American Journal of Psychiatry 165, 969977.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blakemore, S-J, Mills, KL (2014). Is adolescence a sensitive period for sociocultural processing? Annual Review of Psychology 65, 187207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bonanno, GA, Romero, SA, Klein, SI (2015). The temporal elements of psychological resilience: an integrative framework for the study of individuals, families, and communities. Psychological Inquiry 26, 139169.Google Scholar
Bowes, L, Maughan, B, Ball, H, Shakoor, S, Ouellet-Morin, I, Caspi, A, Moffitt, TE, Arseneault, L (2013). Chronic bullying victimization across school transitions: the role of genetic and environmental influences. Development and Psychopathology 25, 333346.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowes, L, Maughan, B, Caspi, A, Moffitt, TE, Arseneault, L (2010). Families promote emotional and behavioural resilience to bullying: evidence of an environmental effect. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 51, 809817.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brewin, CR (2007). Autobiographical memory for trauma: update on four controversies. Memory 15, 227248.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buhrmester, D (1990). Intimacy of friendship, interpersonal competence, and adjustment during preadolescence and adolescence. Child Development 61, 11011111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D (2013). Annual Research Review: resilient functioning in maltreated children – past, present, and future perspectives. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 54, 402422.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, S, Wills, TA (1985). Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin 98, 310357.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Collishaw, S, Hammerton, G, Mahedy, L, Sellers, R, Owen, MJ, Craddock, N, Thapar, AK, Harold, GT, Rice, F, Thapar, A (2016). Mental health resilience in the adolescent off spring of parents with depression: a prospective longitudinal study. The Lancet Psychiatry 3, 4957.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collishaw, S, Pickles, A, Messer, J, Rutter, M, Shearer, C, Maughan, B (2007). Resilience to adult psychopathology following childhood maltreatment: evidence from a community sample. Child Abuse & Neglect 31, 211229.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crone, EA, Dahl, RE (2012). Understanding adolescence as a period of social-affective engagement and goal flexibility. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 13, 636650.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Egeland, B (2009). Taking stock: childhood emotional maltreatment and developmental psychopathology. Child Abuse & Neglect 33, 2226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elgar, FJ, Waschbusch, DA, Dadds, MR, Sigvaldason, N (2006). Development and validation of a short form of the Alabama parenting questionnaire. Journal of Child and Family Studies 16, 243259.Google Scholar
Epstein, NB, Baldwin, LM, Bishop, DS (1983). The McMaster family assessment device. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy 9, 171180.Google Scholar
Fergusson, DM, Lynskey, MT (1996). Adolescent resiliency to family adversity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 37, 281292.Google Scholar
Fergusson, DM, Lynskey, MT, Horwood, LJ (1996). Factors associated with continuity and changes in disruptive behavior patterns between childhood and adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 24, 533553.Google Scholar
Frick, PJ, Bodin, SD, Barry, CT (2000). Psychopathic traits and conduct problems in community and clinic-referred samples of children: further development of the Psychopathy Screening Device. Psychological Assessment 12, 382393.Google Scholar
Gilbert, R, Widom, CS, Browne, K, Fergusson, D, Webb, E, Janson, S (2009). Burden and consequences of child maltreatment in high-income countries. Lancet 373, 6881.Google Scholar
Harpur, LJ, Polek, E, van Harmelen, A-L (2015). The role of timing of maltreatment and child intelligence in pathways to low symptoms of depression and anxiety in adolescence. Child Abuse & Neglect 47, 2437.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hart, SNSN, Binggeli, NJ, Brassard, MR, Bingelli, NJ, Brassard, MR (1997). Evidence for the effects of psychological maltreatment. Journal of Emotional Abuse 1, 2758.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howell, KH, Miller-Graff, LE (2014). Protective factors associated with resilient functioning in young adulthood after childhood exposure to violence. Child Abuse & Neglect 38, 19851994.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jehn, KA, Shah, PP (1997). Interpersonal relationships and task performance: an examination of mediation processes in friendship and acquaintance groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 72, 775790.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kessler, RC, Andrews, G, Colpe, LJ, Hiripi, E, Mroczek, DK, Normand, SLT, Walters, EE, Zaslavsky, AM (2002). Short screening scales to monitor population prevalences and trends in non-specific psychological distress. Psychological Medicine 32, 959976.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klika, JB, Herrenkohl, TI (2013). A review of developmental research on resilience in maltreated children. Trauma, Violence & Abuse 14, 222234.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lahey, BB, Applegate, B, Chronis, AM, Jones, HA, Williams, SH, Loney, J, Waldman, ID (2008). Psychometric characteristics of a measure of emotional dispositions developed to test a developmental propensity model of conduct disorder. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology 37, 794807.Google Scholar
Luthar, SS, Cicchetti, D (2000). The construct of resilience: implications for interventions and social policies. Development and Psychopathology 12, 857885.Google Scholar
Luthar, SS, Cicchetti, D, Becker, B (2000). The construct of resilience: a critical evaluation and guidelines for future work. Child Development 71, 543562.Google Scholar
Masten, AS (2015). Pathways to integrated resilience science. Psychological Inquiry 26, 187196.Google Scholar
Masten, CL, Telzer, EH, Fuligni, AJ, Lieberman, MD, Eisenberger, NI (2012). Time spent with friends in adolescence relates to less neural sensitivity to later peer rejection. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 7, 106114.Google Scholar
Miller, IW, Epstein, NB, Bishop, DS, Keitner, GI (1985). The McMaster family assessment device: reliability and validity. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy 11, 345356.Google Scholar
Miller-Lewis, LR, Searle, AK, Sawyer, MG, Baghurst, PA, Hedley, D (2013). Resource factors for mental health resilience in early childhood: an analysis with multiple methodologies. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health 7, 6.Google Scholar
Parker, G, Roussos, J, Hadzi-Pavlovic, D, Mitchell, P, Wilhelm, K, Austin, MP (1997). The development of a refined measure of dysfunctional parenting and assessment of its relevance in patients with affective disorders. Psychological Medicine 27, 11931203.Google Scholar
Parker, JG, Asher, SR (1993). Friendship and friendship quality in middle childhood: links with peer group acceptance and feelings of loneliness and social dissatisfaction. Developmental Psychology 29, 611621.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pellegrini, AD, Bartini, M (2000). A longitudinal study of bullying, victimization, and peer affiliation during the transition from primary school to middle school. American Educational Research Journal 37, 699725.Google Scholar
Powers, A, Ressler, KJ, Bradley, RG (2009). The protective role of friendship on the effects of childhood abuse and depression. Depression and Anxiety 26, 4653.Google Scholar
Raine, A (1991). The SPQ: a scale for the assessment of schizotypal personality based on DSM-III-R criteria. Schizophrenia Bulletin 17, 555564.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Revelle, W (2014). An overview of the psych package.Google Scholar
Reynolds, CR, Richmond, BO (1997). What I think and feel: a revised measure of Children's Manifest Anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 25, 1520.Google Scholar
Rogosch, FA, Cicchetti, D (2004). Child maltreatment and emergent personality organization: perspectives from the five-factor model. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 32, 123145.Google Scholar
Roose, A, Bijttebier, P, Decoene, S, Claes, L, Frick, PJ (2010). Assessing the affective features of psychopathy in adolescence: a further validation of the inventory of callous and unemotional traits. Assessment 17, 4457.Google Scholar
Rosseel, Y (2012). lavaan: an R package for structural equation. 48.Google Scholar
Rutter, M (1985). Resilience in the face of adversity: protective factors and resistance to psychiatric disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry 147, 598611.Google ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M (2012). Resilience as a dynamic concept. Development and Psychopathology 24, 335344.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sapouna, M, Wolke, D (2013). Resilience to bullying victimization: the role of individual, family and peer characteristics. Child Abuse & Neglect 37, 9971006.Google Scholar
Sinclair, KR, Cole, DA, Dukewich, T, Felton, J, Weitlauf, AS, Maxwell, MA, Tilghman-osborne, C, Jacky, A (2012). Impact of physical and relational peer victimization on depressive cognitions in children and adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology 41, 570583.Google Scholar
Spinhoven, P, Elzinga, BMM, Hovens, JGFMGFM, Roelofs, K, Zitman, FGG, van Oppen, P, Penninx, BWJHWJH (2010). The specificity of childhood adversities and negative life events across the life span to anxiety and depressive disorders. Journal of Affective Disorders 126, 103112.Google Scholar
Stanford, MS, Mathias, CW, Dougherty, DM, Lake, SL, Anderson, NE, Patton, JH (2009). Fifty years of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale: an update and review. Personality and Individual Differences 47, 385395.Google Scholar
Stoltenborgh, M, Bakermans-Kranenburg, MJ, Alink, LRA, van IJzendoorn, MH (2015). The prevalence of child maltreatment across the globe: review of a series of meta-analyses. Child Abuse Review 24, 3750.Google Scholar
Tennant, R, Hiller, L, Fishwick, R, Platt, S, Joseph, S, Weich, S, Parkinson, J, Secker, J, Stewart-Brown, S (2007). The Warwick-Edinburgh Mental Well-being Scale (WEMWBS): development and UK validation. Health and Quality of Life Outcomes 5, 63.Google Scholar
Thapar, A, Collishaw, S, Pine, DS, Thapar, AK (2012). Depression in adolescence. Lancet 379, 10561067.Google Scholar
Trocmé, N, Fallon, B, Maclaurin, B, Chamberland, C, Chabot, M, Esposito, T (2011). Shifting definitions of emotional maltreatment: an analysis child welfare investigation laws and practices in Canada. Child Abuse & Neglect 35, 831840.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vanhalst, J, Soenens, B, Luyckx, K, Van Petegem, S, Weeks, MS, Asher, SR (2015). Why do the lonely stay lonely? Chronically lonely adolescents attributions and emotions in situations of social inclusion and exclusion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 109, 932948.Google Scholar
van Harmelen, A-L (2013). Childhood Emotional Maltreatment: Impact on Cognition and the Brain. Department Clinical Psychology, Faculty of Social and behavioural Sciences, Leiden University: Leiden.Google Scholar
van Harmelen, A-L, De Jong, PJ, Glashouwer, KA, Spinhoven, P, Penninx, BWJH, Elzinga, BM (2010). Child abuse and negative explicit and automatic self-associations: the cognitive scars of emotional maltreatment. Behaviour Research and Therapy 48, 486494.Google Scholar
van Harmelen, A-L, Gibson, JL, St Clair, MC, Owens, M, Brodbeck, J, Dunn, V, Lewis, G, Croudace, T, Jones, PB, Kievit, RA, Goodyer, IM (2016). Friendships and family support reduce subsequent depressive symptoms in at-risk adolescents. PLoS ONE 11, e0153715.Google Scholar
van Wingen, GA, Geuze, E, Vermetten, E, Fernández, G (2011). Perceived threat predicts the neural sequelae of combat stress. Molecular Psychiatry 16, 664671.Google Scholar
Wickham, H (2009). ggplot2: elegant graphics for data analysis.Google Scholar
Wickham, H, Romain, F (2016). Dplyr: a grammar of data manipulation.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Relationship between PSF and Childhood family experiences in N = 1890.

Figure 1

Fig. 2. The relationship between friendships (a) and family support (b) and baseline resilient functioning (N = 1890).

Figure 2

Table 1. Predictors of resilient functioning at baseline (time 1)

Figure 3

Fig. 3. Significant paths in the Structural Equation Model. For reasons of parsimony we only depict significant positive (green) or negative (red) paths (unstandardised Estimates and s.e.). Thicker lines indicate stronger associations.

Figure 4

Table 2. Predictors of later resilient functioning (N = 1093)

Supplementary material: File

Van Harmelen supplementary material

Appendix

Download Van Harmelen supplementary material(File)
File 75 KB