Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-p9bg8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T01:01:45.505Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Contribution of home availability, parental child-feeding practices and health beliefs on children’s sweets and salty snacks consumption in Europe: Feel4Diabetes-Study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 October 2021

Maria Michelle Papamichael
Affiliation:
Department of Dietetics, Nutrition and Sport, School of Allied Health, Human Services and Sport, La Trobe University, Melbourne, VIC 3086, Australia Department of Nutrition and Dietetics, Harokopio University of Athens, Kallithea, Athens, 17671, Greece
Eva Karaglani
Affiliation:
Department of Nutrition and Dietetics, Harokopio University of Athens, Kallithea, Athens, 17671, Greece
Kalliopi Karatzi
Affiliation:
Department of Food Science and Human Nutrition, Agricultural University of Athens, Athens 11855, Greece
Violeta Iotova
Affiliation:
Department of Pediatrics, Medical University of Varna, Varna, Bulgaria
Jemina Kivelä
Affiliation:
Population Health Unit, Finnish Institute for Health and Welfare, Helsinki, Finland
Greet Cardon
Affiliation:
Department of Movement and Sports Sciences, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
Flore De Vylder
Affiliation:
Department of Movement and Sports Sciences, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
Yuliya Bazdarska
Affiliation:
Department of Pediatrics, Medical University of Varna, Varna, Bulgaria
Roumyana Dimova
Affiliation:
Department of Diabetology, Clinical Centre of Endocrinology, Medical University of Sofia, Sofia, Bulgaria
Esther González-Gil
Affiliation:
Department of Biochemistry and Molecular Biology II, Instituto de Nutrición y Tecnología de los Alimentos, Center of Biomedical Research (CIBM), Universidad de Granada, Granada, Spain Growth, Exercise, Nutrition and Development (GENUD) Research Group, University of Zaragoza, Zaragoza, Spain CIBER Fisiopatología de la Obesidad y Nutrición, Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Madrid, Spain
Paloma Flores-Barrantes
Affiliation:
Growth, Exercise, Nutrition and Development (GENUD) Research Group, University of Zaragoza, Zaragoza, Spain CIBER Fisiopatología de la Obesidad y Nutrición, Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Madrid, Spain
Stavros Liatis
Affiliation:
National and Kapodistrian University of Athens Medical School, First Department of Propaedeutic Medicine, Laiko Gnenral Hospital, Athens, Greece
Luis Moreno
Affiliation:
Growth, Exercise, Nutrition and Development (GENUD) Research Group, University of Zaragoza, Zaragoza, Spain
Konstantinos Makrilakis
Affiliation:
National and Kapodistrian University of Athens Medical School, First Department of Propaedeutic Medicine, Laiko Gnenral Hospital, Athens, Greece
Yannis Manios*
Affiliation:
Department of Nutrition and Dietetics, Harokopio University of Athens, Kallithea, Athens, 17671, Greece Institute of Agri-food and Life Sciences, Hellenic Mediterranean University Research Centre, Heraklion, Greece
*
*Corresponding author: Professor Yannis Manios, fax +30 210 9549141, email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Adoption of healthy dietary and snacking habits could support optimum physical and mental development in children as they define health in adulthood. This study assessed parameters associated with children’s snacking such as food home availability, parenting practices, and parents’ health beliefs. In this cross-sectional study 12 039 children, 49·4% boys 5–12 years, participating in the European Feel4Diabetes-Study were included. Children’s weekly consumption of sweets and salty snacks, home availability of snacks, food parenting practices, and health beliefs were assessed via questionnaires. Logistic regression was applied to explore associations of a) home availability of snacks, b) food parenting practices (permissiveness and rewarding with snacks) and c) parent’s opinions on deterministic health beliefs with children’s consumption of sweets and salty snacks. Results showed that home availability (sweets: ORadj: 4·76, 95 % CI: 4·32, 5·23; salty snacks: ORadj: 6·56, 95 % CI: 5·64, 7·61), allowing to consume (sweets: ORadj: 3·29, 95 % CI: 2·95, 3·67; salty snacks: ORadj: 3·41, 95 % CI: 2·98, 3·90) and rewarding with sweets/salty snacks (sweets: ORadj: 2·69, 95 % CI: 2·23, 3·24; salty snacks: ORadj: 4·34, 95 % CI: 3·57, 5·28) ‘sometimes/or less frequently’ compared to ‘always/or often’ were associated with lower weekly consumption of sweets and snacks. Parents’ disagreement compared to agreement with deterministic health beliefs and inattentive eating were associated with lower consumption of salty snacks and sweets in children. Overall, the findings of this study indicate that attempts to promote healthy snacking habits in children should aim to improve parental dietary habits, food parenting practices, health beliefs, and reducing home availability of unhealthy foods and snacks.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Nutrition Society

During childhood, it is well known that dietary habits are important for children’s development(Reference Echouffo-Tcheugui and Ahima1). Over the last few decades, global modernisation has resulted in the abandonment of wholesome traditional dietary patterns to the adoption of a Westernised diet, including overconsumption of energy-dense, nutrient-poor foods such as sweets, salty snacks, sugar-sweetened beverages and fast foods, characterised for being high in sugars, fats, processed meats and salt(Reference Popkin2). Data derived from round 4 (2015–2017) of the WHO European Childhood Obesity Surveillance Initiative that involved 132 489 children from twenty-three European countries showed that 10 % of children consumed sweet snacks or soft drinks daily and less than 50 % fruits and vegetables(Reference Williams, Buoncristiano and Nardone3). These findings signify the urgent need to create healthier food and beverage environments within the family circle. Furthermore, adherence to this type of dietary pattern promotes inflammation triggering the premature onset of adult chronic diseases such as CVD, diabetes and obesity(Reference Echouffo-Tcheugui and Ahima1). Contrastingly, a healthy diet rich in fruits, vegetables, wholegrain cereals, nuts and fish diminishes inflammation and lowers the risk for future chronic diseases(Reference Echouffo-Tcheugui and Ahima1). Therefore, to ensure optimum health, it is crucial that children adopt good dietary and snacking habits early in life to track into adulthood.

The home is the most widely studied setting for influences on children’s dietary habits(Reference Vaughn, Ward and Fisher4). Parents play a primary role in influencing children’s dietary habits(Reference Vaughn, Ward and Fisher4). Types of foods purchased, made available and accessible in the home including during family meals as well as their own dietary practices, health attitudes and beliefs are correlated with children’s food intake(Reference Al-Shookri, Al-Shukaily and Hassan5,Reference Scaglioni, Arrizza and Vecchi6) . Parents’ role as food providers can impact children’s intake of healthy foods such as fruits and vegetables or intake of unhealthy foods sweets and salty snacks through the foods they provide as well as the social environment they create(Reference Johnson, van Jaarsveld and Wardle7Reference Blaine, Kachurak and Davison10). Furthermore, feeding practices employed by parents to influence children’s food intake represent a large component of parental behaviours(Reference Vaughn, Ward and Fisher4). Child-feeding practice constructs including controlling and restricting/or forbidding sweets, snacks and junk foods can be counterproductive and inadvertently enhance children’s intake of these foods, cause dysregulation of food intake and increase adiposity(Reference Birch and Fisher11Reference Rodenburg, Kremers and Oenema14). Alternatively, fewer family rules concerning the type of foods eaten and rewarding using poor quality foods can contribute to a higher intake of fat and sugars, snacks along with fewer fruits and vegetables(Reference Blaine, Kachurak and Davison10,Reference Rodenburg, Kremers and Oenema14) . Given that school-aged children consume two-thirds of their meals at home, environmental exposures external to the home, namely the school setting, via availability of foods served in canteens, and peers, appear to play a minor role. Understanding the factors that shape food preferences in childhood is critical in identifying aspects that promote dietary habits beneficial to health and deter unhealthy ones. Furthermore, the impact of family on children’s fruit and vegetable intake has been extensively studied(Reference Pearson, Biddle and Gorely15,Reference Rasmussen, Krølner and Klepp16) ; however, less attention has been given to food parenting practices that are associated with snacking in European children(Reference Blaine, Kachurak and Davison10). Currently, there are no universal definitions for snacks, snacking or quantitative recommendations for weekly intake(Reference Potter, Vlassopoulos and Lehmann17). Most nutritional guidelines define snacking as energy-dense foods high in sugar and fat with minimal nutritional content consumed in between habitual meals, providing fewer calories than in typical meals(Reference Potter, Vlassopoulos and Lehmann17) and recommend ‘limiting’ intake of sweet and savoury snacks(Reference Potter, Vlassopoulos and Lehmann17). On the other hand, foods such as fruit, vegetables, nuts, milk and yogurt can be considered healthy snacks.

Therefore, the scope of this study was to explore parameters associated with children’s intake of sweets and salty snacks, such as food home availability, food parenting practices and beliefs. We hypothesised that home availability of snacks and certain parental practices, such as permissiveness and the use of food as reward, and beliefs are positively associated with sweets and salty snacks intake in European school children. In terms of public health significance, this research suggests parenting practices and beliefs that could be discouraged when targeting to improve children’s dietary behaviours as well as provide direction for health professionals working with families.

Methods

Study design

This study is a cross-sectional analysis of baseline data of all families (parents-children dyads) participating in the Feel4Diabetes-Study. This multi-national population study was a 2-year school and community-based intervention designed to prevent type 2 diabetes and promote healthy eating and physical activity in vulnerable families across six European countries: Bulgaria, Hungary, Belgium, Finland, Greece and Spain. In short, the Feel4Diabetes intervention promoted healthy eating and exercise by creating a supportive environment at three levels that included the home/family, school and municipalities. Recruitment was based on a standardised multi-sampling procedure and was undertaken in selected provinces of the six European countries. Elementary schools were randomly selected and recruited within each area. The population of interest was 5–12-year-old children attending the first three grades of elementary school in the selected municipalities of each country. The participating families were either randomised to active intervention or the control group. Details of the screening procedure and study methodology have been previously described(Reference Manios, Androutsos and Lambrinou18). For the purpose of the current study, only baseline data involving all families have been analysed. The study protocol was approved by the Institutional Ethics committees in each of the six European countries, and informed consent was obtained from all participating families. The work described has been conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki guidelines for experiments involving humans.

Study sample

In the Feel4Diabetes-Study, 12 041 families (parent-dyads) were enrolled at baseline, and data was assessed for 12 039 children (age range 5–12 years old). Two children (4 and 16 years old) were excluded because their ages were not within the specified limits. Given that some families consisted of more than one child and to avoid duplication of parental information, one child per family was randomly selected. Data on children’s sweets intake were complete for 11 356 children and on salty snacks for 9928 children.

In the present study, we postulated that parenting is one of the main influential components of the home food and social environment that defines children’s snacking patterns. Based on previous literature(Reference Petrauskienė, Žaltauskė and Albavičiūtė19), we defined snacking as ready-to-eat, energy-dense, nutrient-poor foods consumed in between meals and less than four times per week as the recommended intake given the health benefits of reduced snacking in the prevention of obesity and dental caries in children(Reference Potter, Vlassopoulos and Lehmann17). According to food composition tables, snack foods were categorised as ‘sweets’ (such as chocolate bars, cookies or ice-cream) or ‘salty snacks’ (hamburgers, chips and pizza) based on raw materials used in their production (high content of sugars, fat and Na)(Reference Hess, Jonnalagadda and Slavin20) and manufacturing process (frying, drying, baking and roasting)(Reference Saldivar, Caballero, Finglas and Toldrá21).

Demographic information

Demographic information that included country of residence, children’s sex and age along with maternal educational level as an indicator of socio-economic status(Reference De Lepeleere, De Bourdeaudhuij and Van Stappen22) was collected via a printed version of a standardised self-administered questionnaire that was distributed to children during school hours and completed by one parent at home. Parents were instructed to seal completed questionnaires in an envelope that was returned by children to the school and collected by researchers on a weekly basis. Parents were provided with the contact details of researchers in order to clarify any queries that they might have during the study period.

Children’s snacking habits

Children’s snacking was evaluated using a self-administered validated FFQ developed for the Feel4Diabetes-Study(Reference Anastasiou, Fappa and Zachari23). Data regarding children’s snack intake were collected from one parent per family. Respondents were instructed to report children’s usual frequency of sweets and salty snacks in terms of specified serving size. Conventional household measures were used to represent one standard portion size for each food item (1 cup, ½ cup) as well as commercial units (1 small hamburger, 1 small bag of chips, 1 slice of pizza, 1 small chocolate bar, ½ cup of ice-cream, cookies or sweets). Frequency of sweets and salty snacks intake was recorded as weekly or daily consumption of food items which were categorised as less than 1 time per week, 1 or 2 times per week, 3 or 4 times per week, 5 or 6 times per week, 1 or 2 times per day, 3 or 4 times per day, 5 or 6 times per day and more than 6 times per day. Parents’ intake of snacks was evaluated using the same FFQ.

Home availability of salty snacks/and sweets, food parenting practices and health beliefs

Home availability of snacks, parental practices and beliefs were evaluated by the following questions.

Q1a) On a weekly basis, how often are sweets available at your home?

Q1b) On a weekly basis, how often are salty snacks available at your home?

Q2a) On a weekly basis, how often do you allow your child to eat sweets and/or salty snacks?

Q2b) On a weekly basis, how often do you reward your child with sweets or salty snacks?

Q3a) I believe that my health and well-being are determined by my destiny.

Q3b) I believe that people have little power on preventing disease

Q3c) I choose to eat the food that I like without thinking too much about it.

For Q1, response options were ‘always, often, sometimes, rarely and never’, for Q2 options were ‘very often, often, sometimes, rarely and never and for Q3 options were ‘strongly disagree, disagree, agree or strongly agree’. Parents were instructed to tick one of the responses. To maintain comparability across countries, all questionnaires were translated into six languages.

Anthropometry

During school hours, basic anthropometric measurements were conducted on a weekly basis in children by trained personnel. Bodyweight was recorded to the nearest 0·1 kg using digital scales (SECA, 813) with children standing without shoes in minimal clothing. Height was measured to the nearest 0·1 cm using a stadiometer (SECA, 217) with children standing without shoes, their shoulders relaxed, arms hanging freely and head aligned in the Frankfort horizontal plane. Then, BMI was calculated using Quetelet’s equation (weight (kg)/height2 (m2)) and z-scores estimated as defined by the International Obesity Task Force sex- and age-specific BMI cut-offs(Reference Cole and Lobstein24).

Sample size calculation/randomisation

Screen time is one of the most important energy balance related behaviours in children(Reference Shang, Wang and O’Loughlin25) and also one of the main objectives of the Feel4Diabetes-Study was to reduce sedentary behaviour in school children(Reference Manios, Androutsos and Lambrinou18). In this context, a power calculation was performed using G × Power analysis(Reference Faul, Erdfelder and Lang26) and estimated based on reducing sedentary behaviour in school children. It was estimated that a minimum sample of 600 families per treatment arm (i.e 1200 families in total) was required to achieve statistical power >80 % at a two-sided 5 % significance level for reducing screen time by 0·2 h/d in children within 8 months(Reference Manios, Androutsos and Lambrinou18). After including an attrition rate of 20 %, a total sample of 9000 families would be required to be recruited in order to detect a statistically significant difference between the arms. Schools and families were randomised to the intervention and control arms within each municipality with a 1:1 allocation ratio after the completion of baseline assessments.

Statistical analysis

Continuous variables were assessed for normality by applying the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test and the histogram plot. Demographic data are presented as means and standard deviations (sd) or as frequencies (n) and percentages (%) in the case of skewness. Response options for frequencies of sweets and salty snacks were dichotomised to ≤ and > 4 times per week(Reference Petrauskienė, Žaltauskė and Albavičiūtė19). Home availability responses were also recoded into two categories ‘always/often’ and ‘sometimes/rarely/never’. Similarly, food parenting practices were merged to ‘very often/often’ and ‘sometimes/rarely/never’ and health beliefs ‘strongly agree/agree’ and ‘strongly disagree/disagree’. In the univariate analysis, group differences were examined using Mann–Whitney or Pearson’s χ 2 test. Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient rho (r) was used to determine correlations between parents and children’s intake of sweets and salty snacks, where values of rho ranging from 0·10 to 0·39 indicate weak correlations and 0·40 to 0·69 moderate(Reference Schober, Boer and Schwarte27). The associations between home availability, parental practices/beliefs and children’s intake of sweets and salty snacks were explored applying multivariate logistic regression. In analyses, children’s sweets intake and salty snacks were defined as the dependent variables and food parenting practices and health beliefs as the independent variable. Given that children’s age, sex, BMI z-score, country and maternal education are factors influencing children’s food choice, these were entered as covariates in the adjusted regression model. How well the theoretical model fitted the data was measured using the Nagelkerke coefficient R2. The degree of association is expressed as OR and 95 % CI. Exploratory to this study, we repeated the regression analysis stratified by age group (5·0–9·0 years v. 10·0–12·5 years) according to the WHO definition for teenagers (10–19 years old)(28), given that this can be an influencing factor in children’s eating habits(29). All P-values reported are two-tailed, and statistical significance was set at alpha < 0·05. SPSS version 20 (IBM) was employed for all statistical analyses.

Results

Demographic details of the population are presented in Table 1. From the initial sample at baseline, mean age of children was 8·20 (sd 0·99) years with 49·35 % (5942/12 039) boys; 85·20 % (10 255/12 039) of respondents were mothers and 10·17 % fathers (1224/12 039) with 66·56 % (8014/12 039) of mothers completing tertiary education and 52·30 % (6295/12 039) of fathers.

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of population per total sample of families and by children’s sex

P-value significant at 5 %.

§Data for mothers and fathers are not shown but described in text.

* P-value estimated using χ 2 test.

Data are presented as mean (sd).

Mann–Whitney test.

|| Statistically significant P-values.

Regarding intake of sweets and salty snacks, frequency (%) of children and parents consuming these foods ≤ 4 times per week is presented in Table 2. Sex differences in sweets intake were observed in children, with more girls consuming sweets ≤ 4 times per week than boys (girls v. boys: 56·41 % v. 53·85 %; P = 0·01). Comparison of parents’ v. children’s sweets and salty snacks intake showed moderate correlations (sweets: r = 0·451, P < 0·001; salty snacks r = 0·531, P < 0·001).

Table 2. Percentage of children and parents consuming sweets and salty snacks ≤ 4 times per week

Data shown for frequency of sweets and salty snacks intake ≤ 4×/week only.

Key: ≤ 4×/week – less than or equal to four times per week.

* P-value derived from χ 2 test comparing differences in children’s frequency of snacks and sweets intake ≤ 4×/week and > 4×/week.

Statistically significant P-values.

The relationship between home availability, food parenting practices and health beliefs and children’s intake of sweets and salty snacks less/equal to or more than four times/week is shown in Table 3. The univariate analysis revealed that for children consuming sweets and salty snacks ≤ 4 times/week there were substantial differences in home availability (P < 0·001), food parental practices (P < 0·001) and beliefs (P < 0·001). It appears that when sweets and salty snacks were available in the home ‘sometimes/rarely’, children were more likely to consume these foods ≤ 4 times/week as compared with these foods being available ‘always/often’. The same trend was observed for food parenting practices such as ‘allowing to eat these foods’ and ‘rewarding with sweets and salty snacks. With respect to health beliefs, parents who ‘strongly disagree/disagree’ that ‘health was determined by destiny’, ‘I have little control on preventing disease’ and ‘I choose to eat food I like without thinking’, more children consumed sweets and salty snacks ≤ 4 times/week as compared with those whose parents ‘strongly agree/agree’ to these questions.

Table 3. Relationship between home availability, food parenting practices and health beliefs and children’s intake of sweets and salty snacks ≤ 4 times per week

Data shown for frequency of sweets and snack intake ≤ 4×/week only.

Key: ≤ 4×/week – less than or equal to four times per week.

* P-value derived from χ 2 test comparing differences in children’s frequency of snacks and sweets intake ≤ 4×/week and > 4×/week.

Statistically significant P-values.

Applying logistic regression to explore the association between home availability, food parenting practices and health beliefs, and children’s intake of sweets and salty snacks yielded statistical significance for all factors (Table 4). In the crude analysis, a positive association was observed between home availability of sweets (OR: 5·82, 95 % CI: 5·33, 6·35), allowing to eat sweets (OR: 3·73, 95 % CI: 3·38, 4·11) and being rewarded with sweets (OR: 3·30, 95 % CI: 2·78, 3·92) ‘sometimes/rarely’ and children’s intake of sweets ≤ 4 times/week, as compared with ‘always/often’. After adjusting for children’s age, sex, BMI, country of residence and maternal education level, home availability of sweets at a frequency of ‘sometimes/rarely’ was associated with children being 4·76 times more likely to consume sweets ≤ 4 times/week than those when sweets were available ‘always/often’ (ORadj 4·76, 95 % CI: 4·32, 5·23), whereas 3·29 times more likely when allowing to eat sweets ‘sometimes/rarely’ (ORadj: 3·29, 95 % CI: 2·95, 3·67) and 2·69 times more likely when rewarding with sweets at the same frequency (ORadj: 2·69, 95 % CI: 2·23, 3·24). The degree of association as reflected by the Nagelkerke coefficient R2 indicated substantial influence of home availability, food parenting practices including allowing to consume and rewarding with sweets (29·4, 23·1, and 19·1 %, respectively). Regarding parental health beliefs, in the adjusted analysis, no significant associations were observed for parents who ‘strongly disagree/disagree’ that their ‘health is determined by destiny’, ‘I have little power preventing disease’ and children’s sweets intake compared with parents who ‘strongly agree/agree’ to these statements. In contrast, for parents who ‘strongly disagree/disagree’ with ‘I choose to eat food I like without thinking’, children were 1·61 times more likely to consume sweets ≤ 4 times/week than those parents who ‘strongly agree/agree’ (ORadj: 1·61, 95 % CI: 1·45, 1·80). Comparable findings were observed in the crude and adjusted regression analysis for children’s intake of salty snacks, although to a greater extent than sweets intake as reflected by the size of the odds ratio. Home availability of salty snacks, allowing to consume and rewarding with salty snacks ‘sometimes/rarely’ was associated with children consuming these foods ≤ 4 times/week compared with ‘always/often’ ((ORadj: 6·56, 95 % CI: 5·64, 7·61); (ORadj: 3·41, 95 % CI: 2·98, 3·90); (ORadj: 4·34, 95 % CI: 3·57, 5·28), respectively). With respect to parental health beliefs, children from parents who ‘strongly disagree/disagree’ that ‘my health depends on destiny’, ‘I have little power preventing disease’ and ‘I choose to eat food I like without thinking’ were twice as likely to consume salty snacks ≤ 4 times/week compared with children whose parents ‘strongly agree/agree’ with the above statements ((ORadj: 2·08, 95 % CI: 1·74, 2·49); (ORadj: 1·86, 95 % CI: 1·58, 2·20); (ORadj: 2·65, 95 % CI: 2·26, 3·11), respectively).

Table 4. Association between home availability, food parenting practices, beliefs and children’s intake of sweets and salty snacks ≤ 4 times per week from the crude and adjusted logistic regression model

R2 – Nagelkerke coefficient R2.

Dependent variables children’s frequency of intake of sweets and salty snacks as the dichotomous variables (0 = > 4 times/week, 1 = ≤ 4 times/week).

PP-value derived from the crude logistic regression analysis.

P adjP-value from the regression model adjusted for children’s age, sex, BMI (z-score), country and maternal education.

* Reference category.

Statistically significant P-values.

Furthermore, the same trend was observed for children’s sweet and salty snacks intake v. home availability and parenting practices in the regression analysis stratified by age group (5·0–9·0 years v. 10·0–12·5 years) (online Supplementary Table S1). However, regarding health belief ‘parents disagreeing with my health is determined by destiny’ which was borderline significant in the original adjusted analysis (P adj = 0·05), became significant for sweet intake < 4 times/week in the 5·0–9·0 year olds (P adj = 0·03) and nonsignificant for salty snack intake < 4 times/week in 10·0–12·5 year olds (P adj = 0·35). Likewise, for ‘I have little power preventing disease’ was non-significant in the original analysis for sweet intake (P adj = 0·80) but significant in the 10–12·5 years age group P adj = 0·01). On the other hand, results remained significant in both age groups for ‘I choose to eat food I like without thinking’ which is consistent with the original analysis.

Discussion

The present study aimed to determine factors of the home environment, food parenting practices and health beliefs that associate with sweets and salty snack intake in children. Understanding how children’s food consumption choices are developed will aid in the adoption of good dietary habits in children which have potential lifetime health benefits. The findings of this study highlight that home availability of sweets and salty snacks, ‘sometimes or less frequently’ compared with ‘always or often’, was associated with lower weekly intake of these foods in children. The same trend was observed when parents ‘allowed’ children to consume sweets and salty snacks ‘sometimes or rarely’ and ‘rewarded’ them with these foods at the same frequency as compared with ‘very often’. Interestingly, stratification of data according to age group did not alter associations between parenting practices, home availability and children’s weekly intake of sweets and salty snacks. In reference to health beliefs, parents who ‘disagree’ that ‘my health is determined by destiny’, ‘I have little power preventing disease’ and ‘I choose to eat the food I like without thinking’ were related to lower intake of sweets and salty snacks in children as compared with those whose parents ‘agree’ to the above statements. Notably, differences were observed in associations between health beliefs and snacking in the 10–12·5 year age group. These data support our primary hypothesis and is promising because it suggests that by modifying parental behaviour and rectifying false health beliefs it is possible to discourage snacking in children. Therefore, our study is important and useful as a practical guide for health professionals on strategies that could hinder unhealthy snacking in European children and closes the gaps in the literature(Reference Johnson, van Jaarsveld and Wardle7,Reference Couch, Glanz and Zhou30Reference Gibson, Androutsos and Moreno32) .

Various factors might explain children’s reduced intake of sweets and salty snacks when snacks were available in the home ‘sometimes/rarely’ as compared with always/often. Aspects of the home environment can contribute to children’s dietary behaviour especially in children within the 5–12 years age group. The family is the major provider of food and therefore parents influence availability, accessibility, foods purchased and served during family mealtimes and function as important role models(Reference Scaglioni, Arrizza and Vecchi6). Furthermore, parents provide experiences with food and children imitate parental dietary behaviours, food-related attitudes including preferences. In addition, we found moderate correlations between parents’ and children’s intake of sweets and salty snacks which reflect a significant modelling effect via direct observation of parental behaviour and through increased taste exposure(Reference Wardle and Cooke33). This is in line with prior studies documenting resemblance between parents’ and children’s intake of sweets, salty snacks and fat across Europe(Reference Johnson, van Jaarsveld and Wardle7,Reference Hebestreit, Intemann and Siani31,Reference Gibson, Androutsos and Moreno32,Reference Wang, Beydoun and Li34) . Therefore, in the home setting, early repeated exposure of children to foods high in energy, sugar and fat might enhance children’s liking and prioritise preference and selection for them.(Reference Birch and Fisher11,Reference Wardle and Cooke33,Reference Anzman-Frasca, Savage and Marini35) . In this context, it is plausible that by decreasing frequency of exposure to poor quality foods in the home would inevitably result in decreased consumption by children, as was demonstrated in our study. Overall, our observations highlight the need for improving parents’ food preferences to enhance positive changes in children.

Another intriguing observation we noted was that parents ‘disagreeing’ that ‘my health is related to destiny’, ‘I have little power over disease’ and ‘I choose to eat food I like without thinking’, was associated with children consuming fewer sweets and salty snacks. It appears that parents’ health-related beliefs were transferred to offspring. Previous research has demonstrated that a positive, health-conscious family environment can establish and enhance adoption of beneficial health behaviours through role modelling, provision of healthy foods, along with encouragement and support for practising healthy dietary behaviours by children(Reference Vaughn, Ward and Fisher4). Hoffman et al. in a study of 7–11-year-old schoolchildren found that apart from parental feeding practices, parents’ health-related attitudes, goals and motivations influenced their children’s food intake(Reference Hoffmann, Marx and Kiefner-Burmeister36). In children whose mothers emphasised health-related goals, children consumed more healthy foods and less unhealthy(Reference Hoffmann, Marx and Kiefner-Burmeister36). Similarly, low levels of nutrition knowledge and food-related health attitudes in mothers along with less knowledge regarding snack recommendations were related to poor diet quality and increased snacking in pre-schoolers(Reference Al-Shookri, Al-Shukaily and Hassan5,Reference Gibson, Androutsos and Moreno32) . On the other hand, intriguingly, we noted age differences in associations between health beliefs and children’s snacking, predominately in the 10–12·5 year age group, most likely related to cognitive development, health and nutrition literacy in adolescents(Reference Koca and Arkan37).

These observations posit that educating parents the importance of diet in relation to health and addressing unhealthy parental beliefs might be a cost-effective, feasible and practical means of improving dietary habits of the entire family and ultimately overall health of the population. Nutrition education would provide parents with the skills to make informed choices about foods that their family consume and support autonomy because such information would provide direction for dietary behaviours.

With regard to food parenting practices such as ‘permissiveness’ and rewarding with snacks, we showed that rewarding or allowing children to snack sometimes or rarely resulted in lower weekly intake of sweets and salty snacks which is supported by the literature(Reference Hennessy, Hughes and Goldberg38Reference Wang, van de Gaar and Jansen40). Wang et al. demonstrated that using food as a reward was associated with higher odds of children snacking more than once daily (OR, 1·43; 95 % CI 1·01 to 2·04)(Reference Wang, van de Gaar and Jansen40). Sleddens et al. reported that instrumental feeding (or rewarding) practised by parents’ increased snacking behaviour in 6–7-year-old children(Reference Sleddens, Kremers and De Vries39). On the same note, Hennessy et al. showed that a permissive feeding style (lack of control and indulging to children’s requests) was associated with increased intake of low nutrient dense foods including sweets and salty snacks in 9-year-old children(Reference Hennessy, Hughes and Goldberg38). Although WHO dietary recommendations advocate reduction in intake of energy-dense high fat and sugary foods(41), using highly palatable foods (for example sweets) as a reward, this is common practice by parents that can promote children’s overconsumption of low-nutrient energy dense foods by diminishing the extent that children rely on their own hunger and satiety cues to initiate and terminate eating(Reference Birch, Fisher and Davison13). Collectively, the aforementioned studies reinforce our observations that allowing or rewarding children by sweets and salty snacks ‘sometimes’ by parents might decrease the consumption of these foods. Thus, suggesting that health professionals should be aware of the different parental child-feeding practices and styles when evaluating children’s diet quality and perhaps focus on interventions modifying parental behaviour that will encourage healthy eating habits in children and discourage unhealthy.

Strengths and limitations

The findings of the current study should be interpreted in light of several considerations. Novel to this study was the exploration of associations between health beliefs and snacking patterns in children. To our knowledge, most studies have focused on children’s intake of healthy foods such as fruits and vegetables, and more research is needed to determine factors that reduce intake of unhealthy foods and snacking(Reference Yee, Lwin and Ho42). Hence, the development of intervention strategies to improve children’s dietary patterns is likely to be more successful if supported by an understanding not only of healthy but also unhealthy food intake. Furthermore, given the lack of a uniform definition for snacking as well as snack-specific dietary recommendations for children available in the WHO European region(Reference Potter, Vlassopoulos and Lehmann17), our study could be useful in setting the foundations for guidelines on how parents should incorporate snacks into children’s healthy diet, and parenting practices that could effectively modify the intake of unhealthy snacks. Additional strong points of the present work are the standard methods and procedures used by all participating countries to record dietary intake, food parenting practices and home availability along with the large sample size and homogeneity among children with respect to age.

A possible limitation is the cross-sectional design that does not allow conclusions to be drawn about causal relationships. The use of parental reports to capture children’s sweet and salty snacks intake may evoke a source of bias due to recall error(Reference Pérez-Rodrigo, Artiach Escauriaza and Artiach Escauriaza43). Moreover, parents may not be aware of foods purchased and consumed by children outside of the home and during school hours(Reference Pérez-Rodrigo, Artiach Escauriaza and Artiach Escauriaza43). Although the use of FFQ in collecting dietary data is common practice in nutritional epidemiology, they are subject to over-reporting of healthy foods and under-reporting of non-healthy due to social desirability(Reference Pérez Rodrigo, Aranceta and Salvador44). Nevertheless, FFQ are cost effective and appropriate for large epidemiological cohort studies to assess habitual intake of populations(Reference Pérez Rodrigo, Aranceta and Salvador44), and we employed a concise, simple FFQ of low-respondent burden. Another drawback, questionnaire response options ‘always, often, sometimes, rarely and never’ were not defined to respondents which could lead to inaccuracy in results. One more domain worth further investigation, sex differences in parenting styles were not explored in the present study. Previous research has identified fathers as predictors of children’s unhealthy food intake(Reference Hebestreit, Intemann and Siani31,Reference Hall, Collins and Morgan45) .

Conclusion

The family unit is an important social context where children learn and adopt dietary behaviours that persist throughout the lifespan. During childhood, parents play a prime role as health promoters, role models and educators in the lives of their children, influencing food perceptions and choices including snacks. This study demonstrated that home availability of sweets and salty snacks, permissiveness and rewarding with these foods ‘sometimes or less frequently’ compared with ‘always/or often’ were associated with lower weekly intake of snacks in children. These findings indicate that attempts to promote healthy snacking habits in school-aged children should target improving parental dietary habits, food parenting practices, health beliefs and reducing home availability of unhealthy foods such as sweets and salty snacks which could bring about adoption of healthy eating practices in children that track into adulthood.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank the members of the Feel4Diabetes-Study Group: Coordinator: Yannis Manios, Steering Committee: Yannis Manios, Greet Cardon, Jaana Lindström, Peter Schwarz, Konstantinos Makrilakis, Lieven Annemans, Winne Ko, Harokopio University (Greece): Yannis Manios, Kalliopi Karatzi, Odysseas Androutsos, George Moschonis, Spyridon Kanellakis, Christina Mavrogianni, Konstantina Tsoutsoulopoulou, Christina Katsarou, Eva Karaglani, Irini Qira, Efstathios Skoufas, Konstantina Maragkopoulou, Antigone Tsiafitsa, Irini Sotiropoulou, Michalis Tsolakos, Effie Argyri, Mary Nikolaou, Eleni-Anna Vampouli, Christina Filippou, Kyriaki Apergi, Amalia Filippou, Gatsiou Katerina, Efstratios Dimitriadis, Finnish Institute for Health and Welfare (Finland): Jaana Lindström, Tiina Laatikainen, Katja Wikström, Jemina Kivelä, Päivi Valve, Esko Levälahti, Eeva Virtanen, Tiina Pennanen, Seija Olli, Karoliina Nelimarkka, Ghent University (Belgium), Department of Movement and Sports Sciences: Greet Cardon, Vicky Van Stappen, Nele Huys, Department of Public Health: Lieven Annemans, Ruben Willems, Department of Endocrinology and Metabolic Diseases: Samyah Shadid, Technische Universität Dresden (Germany): Peter Schwarz, Patrick Timpel, University of Athens (Greece), Konstantinos Makrilakis, Stavros Liatis, George Dafoulas, Christina-Paulina Lambrinou, Angeliki Giannopoulou, International Diabetes Federation European Region (Belgium): Winne Ko, Ernest Karuranga, Universidad De Zaragoza (Spain): Luis Moreno, Fernando Civeira, Gloria Bueno, Pilar De Miguel-Etayo, Esther Mª Gonzalez-Gil, María L. Miguel-Berges, Natalia Giménez-Legarre; Paloma Flores-Barrantes, Alelí M. Ayala-Marín, Miguel Seral-Cortés, Lucia Baila-Rueda, Ana Cenarro, Estíbaliz Jarauta, Rocío Mateo-Gallego, Medical University of Varna (Bulgaria): Violeta Iotova, Tsvetalina Tankova, Natalia Usheva, Kaloyan Tsochev, Nevena Chakarova, Sonya Galcheva, Rumyana Dimova, Yana Bocheva, Zhaneta Radkova, Vanya Marinova, Yuliya Bazdarska, Tanya Stefanova, University of Debrecen (Hungary): Imre Rurik, Timea Ungvari, Zoltán Jancsó, Anna Nánási, László Kolozsvári, Csilla Semánova, Éva Bíró, Emese Antal, Sándorné Radó: Extensive Life Oy (Finland): Remberto Martinez, Marcos Tong.

The Feel4Diabetes-Study has received funding from the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation program (Grant Agreement: n° 643708). The content of this article reflects only the authors’ views, and the European Community is not liable for any use that may be made of the information contained therein. The funding body had no role in the design of this study and collection, analysis and interpretation of the data and in writing this manuscript.

Conceptualisation: M. M. P., Y. M. and K. K. Methodology: M. M. P., Y. M. and K. K. Formal analysis: M. M. P. Conceived the concept for the analysis, conducted the statistical analysis and is the principle author of the first and final draft of the manuscript. Investigation and data collection: M. M. P., K. K. and E. K. Data curation: M. M. P., K. K. and Y. M. Writing-original and final draft preparation: M. M. P. and K. K. Review and editing: Y. M., K. K., E. K., G. C., F. D. V., V. I., E. G-G., P. F-B., J. K., L. M., K. M., S. L. and Y. M. Supervision and project administration: Y. M. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

The authors have no conflict of interests to declare.

Supplementary material

For supplementary material/s referred to in this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007114521004190

References

Echouffo-Tcheugui, JB & Ahima, RS (2019) Does diet quality or nutrient quantity contribute more to health? J Clin Invest 129, 39693970.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Popkin, BM (2006) Global nutrition dynamics: the world is shifting rapidly toward a diet linked with noncommunicable diseases. Am J Clin Nutr 84, 289298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, J, Buoncristiano, M, Nardone, P, et al. (2020) A snapshot of European Children’s eating habits: results from the fourth round of the WHO European Childhood Obesity Surveillance Initiative (COSI). Nutrients 12, 2481.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vaughn, AE, Ward, DS, Fisher, JO, et al. (2016) Fundamental constructs in food parenting practices: a content map to guide future research. Nutr Rev 74, 98117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Al-Shookri, A, Al-Shukaily, L, Hassan, F, et al. (2011) Effect of mothers nutritional knowledge and attitudes on Omani children’s dietary intake. Oman Med J 26, 253257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scaglioni, S, Arrizza, C, Vecchi, F, et al. (2011) Determinants of children’s eating behavior. Am J Clin Nutr 94, 2006s2011s.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, L, van Jaarsveld, CH & Wardle, J (2011) Individual and family environment correlates differ for consumption of core and non-core foods in children. Br J Nutr 105, 950959.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trofholz, AC, Tate, AD, Draxten, ML, et al. (2016) Home food environment factors associated with the presence of fruit and vegetables at dinner: a direct observational study. Appetite 96, 526532.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christian, MS, Evans, CE, Hancock, N, et al. (2013) Family meals can help children reach their 5 a day: a cross-sectional survey of children’s dietary intake from London primary schools. J Epidemiol Community Health 67, 332338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blaine, RE, Kachurak, A, Davison, KK, et al. (2017) Food parenting and child snacking: a systematic review. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 14, 146.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Birch, LL & Fisher, JO (1998) Development of eating behaviors among children and adolescents. Pediatrics 101, 539549.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aparício, G, Cunha, M, Duarte, J, et al. (2015) Parental attitudes, beliefs, and practices about child feeding: relationship with preschooler’s weight status. Atención Primaria 47, 2428.Google Scholar
Birch, LL, Fisher, JO & Davison, KK (2003) Learning to overeat: maternal use of restrictive feeding practices promotes girls’ eating in the absence of hunger. Am J Clin Nutr 78, 215220.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodenburg, G, Kremers, SP, Oenema, A, et al. (2014) Associations of parental feeding styles with child snacking behaviour and weight in the context of general parenting. Public Health Nutr 17, 960969.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pearson, N, Biddle, SJ & Gorely, T (2009) Family correlates of fruit and vegetable consumption in children and adolescents: a systematic review. Public Health Nutr 12, 267283.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rasmussen, M, Krølner, R, Klepp, K-I, et al. (2006) Determinants of fruit and vegetable consumption among children and adolescents: a review of the literature. Part I: quantitative studies. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 3, 2222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Potter, M, Vlassopoulos, A & Lehmann, U (2018) Snacking recommendations worldwide: a scoping review. Adv Nutr 9, 8698.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manios, Y, Androutsos, O, Lambrinou, CP, et al. (2018) A school- and community-based intervention to promote healthy lifestyle and prevent type 2 diabetes in vulnerable families across Europe: design and implementation of the Feel4Diabetes-study. Public Health Nutr 21, 32813290.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petrauskienė, A, Žaltauskė, V & Albavičiūtė, E (2015) Family socioeconomic status and nutrition habits of 7–8 year old children: cross-sectional Lithuanian COSI study. Ital J Pediatr 41, 34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hess, JM, Jonnalagadda, SS & Slavin, JL (2016) What is a snack, why do we snack, and how can we choose better snacks? A review of the definitions of snacking, motivations to snack, contributions to dietary intake, and recommendations for improvement. Adv Nutr 7, 466475.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saldivar, SOS (2016) Snack foods: types and composition. In Encyclopedia of Food and Health, pp. 1318 [Caballero, B, Finglas, PM & Toldrá, F, editors]. Oxford: Academic Press. https://www.elsevier.com/books/encyclopedia-of-food-and-health/caballero/978-0-12-384947-2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Lepeleere, S, De Bourdeaudhuij, I, Van Stappen, V, et al. (2018) Parenting practices as a mediator in the association between family socio-economic status and screen-time in primary schoolchildren: a Feel4Diabetes study. Int J Environ Res Public Health 15, 2553.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anastasiou, CA, Fappa, E, Zachari, K, et al. (2020) Development and reliability of questionnaires for the assessment of diet and physical activity behaviors in a multi-country sample in Europe the Feel4Diabetes Study. BMC Endocr Disord 20, 135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cole, TJ & Lobstein, T (2012) Extended international (IOTF) body mass index cut-offs for thinness, overweight and obesity. Pediatr Obes 7, 284294.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shang, L, Wang, J, O’Loughlin, J, et al. (2015) Screen time is associated with dietary intake in overweight Canadian children. Prev Med Rep 2, 265269.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Faul, F, Erdfelder, E, Lang, AG, et al. (2007) G × Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods 39, 175191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schober, P, Boer, C & Schwarte, LA (2018) Correlation coefficients: appropriate use and interpretation. Anesth Analg 126, 17631768.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
WHO (2021) Adolescent Health. https://www.who.int/health-topics/adolescent-health#tab=tab_1 (accessed September 2021).Google Scholar
WHO (2016) Fact Sheet: Adolescents’ Dietary Habits. https://www.euro.who.int/__data/assets/pdf_file/0006/303477/HBSC-No.7_factsheet_Diet.pdf%3Fua%3D1 (accessed September 2021).Google Scholar
Couch, SC, Glanz, K, Zhou, C, et al. (2014) Home food environment in relation to children’s diet quality and weight status. J Acad Nutr Diet 114, 15691579.e1561.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hebestreit, A, Intemann, T, Siani, A, et al. (2017) Dietary patterns of European children and their parents in association with family food environment: results from the I. Family Study. Nutrients 9, 126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibson, EL, Androutsos, O, Moreno, L, et al. (2020) Influences of parental snacking-related attitudes, behaviours and nutritional knowledge on young children’s healthy and unhealthy snacking: the ToyBox study. Nutrients 12, 432.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wardle, J & Cooke, L (2008) Genetic and environmental determinants of children’s food preferences. Br J Nutr 99, S15S21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, Y, Beydoun, MA, Li, J, et al. (2011) Do children and their parents eat a similar diet? Resemblance in child and parental dietary intake: systematic review and meta-analysis. J Epidemiol Community Health 65, 177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anzman-Frasca, S, Savage, JS, Marini, ME, et al. (2012) Repeated exposure and associative conditioning promote preschool children’s liking of vegetables. Appetite 58, 543553.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoffmann, DA, Marx, JM, Kiefner-Burmeister, A, et al. (2016) Influence of maternal feeding goals and practices on children’s eating behaviors. Appetite 107, 2127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koca, B & Arkan, G (2021) The relationship between adolescents’ nutrition literacy and food habits, and affecting factors. Public Health Nutr 24, 717728.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hennessy, E, Hughes, SO, Goldberg, JP, et al. (2012) Permissive parental feeding behavior is associated with an increase in intake of low-nutrient-dense foods among American children living in rural communities. J Acad Nutr Diet 112, 142148.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sleddens, EF, Kremers, SP, De Vries, NK, et al. (2010) Relationship between parental feeding styles and eating behaviours of Dutch children aged 6–7. Appetite 54, 3036.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, L, van de Gaar, VM, Jansen, W, et al. (2017) Feeding styles, parenting styles and snacking behaviour in children attending primary schools in multiethnic neighbourhoods: a cross-sectional study. BMJ Open 7, e015495.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
WHO (2003) Diet, Nutrition and the Prevention of Chronic Diseases. Geneva: World Health Organization.Google Scholar
Yee, AZ, Lwin, MO & Ho, SS (2017) The influence of parental practices on child promotive and preventive food consumption behaviors: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 14, 47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pérez-Rodrigo, C, Artiach Escauriaza, B, Artiach Escauriaza, J, et al. (2015) Dietary assessment in children and adolescents: issues and recommendations. Nutr Hosp 31, 7683.Google ScholarPubMed
Pérez Rodrigo, C, Aranceta, J, Salvador, G, et al. (2015) Food frequency questionnaires. Nutr Hosp 31, 4956.Google ScholarPubMed
Hall, L, Collins, CE, Morgan, PJ, et al. (2011) Children’s intake of fruit and selected energy-dense nutrient-poor foods is associated with fathers’ intake. J Am Diet Assoc 111, 10391044.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of population per total sample of families and by children’s sex

Figure 1

Table 2. Percentage of children and parents consuming sweets and salty snacks ≤ 4 times per week

Figure 2

Table 3. Relationship between home availability, food parenting practices and health beliefs and children’s intake of sweets and salty snacks ≤ 4 times per week

Figure 3

Table 4. Association between home availability, food parenting practices, beliefs and children’s intake of sweets and salty snacks ≤ 4 times per week from the crude and adjusted logistic regression model

Supplementary material: File

Papamichael et al. supplementary material

Table S1

Download Papamichael et al. supplementary material(File)
File 23.2 KB