Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t8hqh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T22:35:24.024Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Combatting ageism through virtual embodiment? Using explicit and implicit measures

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 September 2022

Liat Ayalon*
Affiliation:
Louis and Gabi Weisfeld School of Social Work, Bar Ilan University, Ramat Gan, Israel
Ehud Dayan
Affiliation:
Sonarion LTD, Jerusalem, Israel
Sara Freedman
Affiliation:
Louis and Gabi Weisfeld School of Social Work, Bar Ilan University, Ramat Gan, Israel
*
Correspondence should be addressed to: Liat Ayalon, Louis and Gabi Weisfeld School of Social Work, Bar Ilan University, Ramat Gan, 52900, Israel. Phone 035317910. Email: [email protected].
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objectives:

Ageism is defined as stereotypes, prejudice, and discrimination towards people because of their age. Although ageism can be directed towards people of any age group, most research has focused on ageism towards older people. Ageism towards older people is known to have a significant impact on their health and wellbeing and to even result in higher healthcare costs. The present study evaluated the use of virtual embodiment (VE) to reduce self- and other-directed ageism.

Design, setting, and participants:

We randomized 80 individuals between the ages of 18 and 35 years to one of two conditions: VE as an older or a younger avatar.

Results:

No differences were found on explicit measures of ageism. Once multiple comparisons were accounted for, a nonsignificant reduction in implicit age bias following exposure to the older avatar (Cohen’s d = .75, p = .02) also was found.

Conclusions:

Past research has established the effectiveness of VE in relation to implicit measures. However, once both explicit and implicit measures are included and multiple comparisons are accounted for, neither explicit nor implicit measures of ageism show a significant effect. Given the multidimensional nature of ageism, further research is needed to establish the effectiveness of VE once multiple measures of ageism are considered.

Type
Original Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© International Psychogeriatric Association 2022

Introduction

Ageism is defined as stereotypes, prejudice, and discrimination towards people because of their age. Ageism can be reflected in a positive or a negative bias based on age and can be directed towards people of all age groups. However, most research to date has focused on negative aspects of ageism directed towards older people because of their age (Ayalon and Tesch-Römer, Reference Ayalon and Tesch-Römer2017). Another feature of ageism concerns the fact that it can be either explicit, well-acknowledged by those who hold it or implicit, performed with limited or no awareness or acknowledgement (Levy and Banaji, Reference Levy, Banaji and Nelson2002). Moreover, in contrast to the other big “isms,” namely sexism and racism, ageism affects all of us and is considered “the enemy within” (Levy, Reference Levy2001). People might hold ageist attitudes towards others, whom they perceive as different from them (e.g. an out group), but they may also hold ageist views towards themselves given the often-negative value assigned to old age and internalized over the years. This demonstrates a distinction between other-directed and self-directed ageism as two aspects of ageism of relevance to the lives of older people (Ayalon and Tesch-Römer, Reference Ayalon and Tesch-Römer2017). Ageism is considered a major public health threat. It was identified by the World Health Organization (WHO) as a global threat to older people’s health and wellbeing (World Health Organization, 2021). As such, ageism is now one of the four pillars addressed by the United Nation’s Decade of Healthy Ageing with the understanding that ageism is a barrier to healthy aging and healthy longevity (World Health Organization, 2020).

Virtual embodiment as a strategy to reduce ageism

Following the WHO and the UN call to eradicate ageism, the present study evaluates a virtual embodiment (VE) method to reduce ageism. According to the self-perception theory, people form their opinions, feelings, and attitudes by observing their bodily sensations (Bem, Reference Bem1972). This theory has been elaborated to account for exposure to the behaviors of an avatar (Bailey et al., Reference Bailey, Bailenson and Casasanto2016). When the self-representation is modified in a substantial way compared with the physical self, the situation is called proteus effect. The person is thought to be inferring about his/her own internal beliefs and attitudes by observing one’s behaviors in a virtual form (Fox et al., Reference Fox, Bailenson and Tricase2013).

VE has been used as a tool to reduce ageism. A study conducted in Italy has found a reduction in implicit age bias after exposure to an old age virtual arm, unrelated to the position of the arm (La Rocca et al., Reference La Rocca, Andrea, Tosi and Daini2020). A different study conducted in the United Kingdom has exposed students to different conditions including VE. Based on qualitative interviews, the authors concluded that students acknowledged the importance of the simulation for contextualizing course materials relevant to old age and that identifying with the experiences of older people fostered frustration and distress among students (Hudson et al., Reference Hudson2018). A different study has found that a VE task is comparable to embodied perspective taking, and both are insufficient to foster positive attitudes towards older people, when intergroup threat is direct and concrete, but when intergroup threat is abstract, VE is preferred to embodied perspective taking (Oh et al., Reference Oh, Bailenson, Weisz and Zaki2016). Experiencing an older avatar resulted in a slower walking pace compared with those who experienced a younger avatar and those in the control group. However, this effect lasted only for the first half of the walking phase (Reinhard et al., Reference Reinhard, Shah, Faust-Christmann and Lachmann2020). Considering these mixed findings, clearly there is a need to further assess the utility of VE as a tool to reduce ageism.

Assessing the impact of a VE intervention on ageism

A recent review has concluded that currently, there are no psychometrically adequate measures to assess all dimensions of ageism (e.g. stereotypes, prejudice, and discrimination) (Ayalon et al., Reference Ayalon2019). Moreover, the fact that people might be reluctant to admit to being ageist makes the use of explicit measures questionable. Implicit measures, in contrast, are seen as superior because they can capture attitudes that people are unwilling to disclose or unaware of. The idea behind implicit measures is that performance indicators are inferred from behaviors such as response time or performance accuracy (Gawronski and Brannon, Reference Gawronski, Brannon, Albarracin and Johnson2018).

According to the principles of a dual process theory, implicit biases are automatic and unconscious, and unaffected by respondent’s motivation. Explicit bias on the other hand represents a more conscious and deliberate processes (Forscher et al., Reference Forscher2019). However, the evidence to support this assumption is equivocal. It has been argued that the predictive value of implicit measures is weak, and their incremental validity over self-report measures is small (Meissner et al., Reference Meissner, Grigutsch, Koranyi, Müller and Rothermund2019). Nevertheless, despite the limited correlation between implicit and explicit measures and the limited test–retest reliability of implicit measures at the individual level, at the aggregate regional-level implicit–explicit correlations and test–retest reliability are high (Hehman et al., Reference Hehman, Calanchini, Flake and Leitner2019). In addition, a different meta-analysis has concluded that attitudes, stereotypes, and identity measured either implicitly or explicitly are related to intergroup behaviors (Kurdi et al., Reference Kurdi2019)

Research concerning the impact of varied interventions on implicit measures is largely inconclusive. A recent systematic review has found that many interventions have no effect or even result in increased implicit bias, rather than reduced bias (FitzGerald et al., Reference FitzGerald, Martin, Berner and Hurst2019). A different meta-analysis had found weak short-term effects on implicit measures following brief single-session interventions. The authors also found that procedures changed explicit measures less and to a lesser degree compared with implicit measures. Changes in implicit measures did not account for changes in explicit measures, and changes in implicit bias did not result in comparable behavioral changes (Forscher et al., Reference Forscher2019). Thus, further research is needed to better understand the unique contributions of implicit measures over and above explicit measures.

The present study

This study responded to a direct call put forth by the WHO and the UN to eradicate ageism because of its detrimental impact on the health and wellbeing of older people (World Health Organization, 2020, 2021). VE has shown promise in increasing empathy and reducing implicit negative attitudes in various domains (Banakou et al., Reference Banakou, Hanumanthu and Slater2016; Hamilton-Giachritsis et al., Reference Hamilton-Giachritsis, Banakou, Garcia Quiroga, Giachritsis and Slater2018; Li and Kyung Kim, Reference Li and Kyung Kim2021). In the case of ageism, results have been somewhat equivocal as VE as an older avatar also resulted in slower walking pace (Reinhard et al., Reference Reinhard, Shah, Faust-Christmann and Lachmann2020) and greater frustration and distress (Hudson et al., Reference Hudson2018). VE also was found ineffective in the face of a concrete explicit threat (Oh et al., Reference Oh, Bailenson, Weisz and Zaki2016). Moreover, to date, the entire spectrum of ageism, which addresses both others- and self-directed ageism and can be measured either explicitly or implicitly (Ayalon and Tesch-Römer, Reference Ayalon and Tesch-Römer2017), has not been adequately addressed in the context of VE interventions. As such, the study aimed to examine VE as a tool to reduce ageism towards older people. We conducted a comprehensive assessment of ageism targeting both other- and self-directed ageism, relying on both explicit and implicit measures.

Methods

Procedure and sample

The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the School of Social Work at Bar Ilan University (#061904/2). We recruited participants between the ages of 18 and 35 years from a university setting. Exclusion criteria were being pregnant, past negative experiences with virtual reality (VR), and history of epilepsy or seizures. All participants signed informed consent and received detailed explanations about the study prior to their participation. Participants were not reimbursed financially for their time. Recruitment took place within a public area of the university premises. Although most people who frequent the facility are students, we did not restrict the recruitment of study participants to university students. Hence, the sample represents the general public but consists mainly of university students. Recruitment occurred during two time periods: June 2021 and March–April 2022. A total of 80 participants were recruited. The average age of the sample was 23.4 (SD = 5.83; range = 18–33), 59% were men, and the average number of years of education was 13.81(SD = 1.70; 11–20).

VE procedure

The VE was hosted on a DELL G5 5587 laptop with Oculus Rift and touch controllers, and the software was written by Sonarion LTD using Unity. The participant entered the VE environment (Figure 1) and was oriented to the contents of the room. Participants were instructed to look at "themselves" in the mirror and to move different body parts (e.g. to raise their arms). Participants were then asked to move different objects around the room. After that brief introduction, the participant was asked to step forward to the mirror in front of him/her and to look closely at "his/her" face, that is, the avatar face, and move his/her head from side to side while touching it with both hands, then step forward and backward a bit with his/her whole body and then use the hand-held touch device’s thumb joystick to travel around the counter and push away objects to the floor. Lastly, the participant was asked to look again in the mirror and wave his/her hands. Participants were randomized to one of two conditions: VE of an older avatar (Figure 1) or VE of a younger avatar (Figure 2). Randomization relied on a pre-assigned random number list. The sex of the avatar was congruent with the sex of the respondent.

Figure 1. An image of an older avatar.

Figure 2. An image of a younger avatar.

Measures

Except for the VE questionnaire which was completed while participants engaged in the VE tasks, all other measures were completed immediately after the VE task was completed.

VE questionnaire

This is a 12-item measure that assesses perception of embodiment as an indicator of the experiences of participants (Roth and Latoschik, Reference Roth and Latoschik2019). The measure evaluates the ownership of the virtual body (“I felt like the virtual body was my body”), agency over the virtual body (“I felt like I was controlling the movement of the virtual body”), and changes in perceived body scheme (“I felt like the weight of my own body has changed”). These questions were ranked on a 1 (not at all) to 7 (completely agree) scale. A composite mean score was calculated. Cronbach’s alpha was .79 in the present study. This questionnaire was administered to 41 participants who were enrolled in the VE task.

Self-perceptions of aging (SPA)

This eight-item measure represents self-directed ageism. The measure was developed based on five items from the Philadelphia Geriatric Center Morale Scale (Lawton, Reference Lawton1975) and three items from the Berlin Aging Study. Items range on a scale between 1 (not at all) and 6 (completely agree). A composite score was calculated to reflect overall ageism, composed of positive self-perceptions of aging (SPA) (“I am satisfied with the way I am aging”) and negative SPA (“things keep getting worse as I get older”). Cronbach’s alpha was .60 and .70, respectively.

Succession, Identity, and Consumption Scale (SIC)

This measure represents other-directed ageism. The 20-item measure taps prescriptive beliefs concerning intergenerational conflicts related to the expectations surrounding older people’s succession from envied positions/resources (N = 7 items; “It is unfair that older people get to vote on issues that will impact younger people much more”), the maintenance of an “old” identity, which does not cross over to the younger people’s domain (N = 5 items; “Older people probably shouldn't use Facebook”) and limited consumption/unfair depletion of shared resources (N = 8 items; “Older people don't really need to get the best seats on buses and trains”) (North and Fiske, Reference North and Fiske2013). A higher score represents more negative ageist attitudes. In the present study, Cronbach’s alpha was .88.

Brief Implicit Association Test (BIAT)

The implicit association test is the most prominent measure of implicit attitudes. Response time is measured regarding congruently matched vs. non-congruently matched pictures of young/old faces and positive and negative words in terms of their valence (Hummert et al., Reference Hummert, Garstka, O'Brien, Greenwald and Mellott2002). Compared with the IAT, the Brief Implicit Association Test (BIAT) has a fewer number of trials. However, it has shown to be psychometrically equivalent to the original IAT (Sriram and Greenwald, Reference Sriram and Greenwald2009). A higher score represents a more negative bias towards older people compared with younger people. This was administered only during the second administration period and was completed by 41 participants.

Analysis

We first ran descriptive statistics to characterize the sample. Next, we examined between group differences (e.g. young vs. old VE) on VE questionnaire. To assess group differences on explicit outcome variables (e.g. SPA, SIC, and ERA), we conducted multivariate analysis of variance. Because the implicit measure (BIAT) was administered to only 41 participants, differences on this measure were assessed using t-test for independent samples. Differences across the two conditions regarding demographic variables (e.g. age, gender, and education) also were examined.

Results

The VE experience suggested comparable moderate embodiment experiences in the two groups. Multivariate analysis of variance was nonsignificant (Hotteling’s test), indicating that there was no significant differences on the explicit measures of ageism (e.g. SPA, SIC). Independent samples t-test was conducted to assess the implicit measure (BIAT), which was administered to 41 participants. This measure did not meet the Bonferroni correction cutoff (.01). Respondents exposed to an older avatar showed a reduced negative bias towards older people compared with those exposed to a young avatar, which matched their sex. This intervention resulted in a large effect size of .75 but was nonsignificant once multiple comparisons were considered and the cutoff for statistical significance was adjusted for. As noted, the explicit ageism scales did not show a difference between the two experimental conditions. There also were no differences across the two conditions in the VE experiences reported by participants nor in any of the demographic variables examined (see Table 1).

Table 1. Differences in sample characteristics between the old vs. young VE tasks (N = 80)

VE = virtual embodiment; SPA = self-perceptions of aging; SIC = Succession, Identity, and Consumption Scale; BIAT = Brief Implicit Association Test.

21 people participated in the VE old and 20 in the VE young for the completion of the BIAT measure.

Discussion

The present study was inspired by the recent call put forth by the WHO and the UN to address ageism given its detrimental impact on older people’s health and wellbeing (World Health Organization, 2020, 2021). The study also was guided by the understanding that the assessment of ageism should be multimodal and incorporate both implicit and explicit measures (Ayalon et al., Reference Ayalon2019). Given equivocal findings concerning the role of VE in addressing ageism and the varied impact of different interventions on implicit vs. explicit measures, both types of measures were included. Moreover, because ageism can be directed both towards others as well as towards one-self (Ayalon and Tesch-Römer, Reference Ayalon and Tesch-Römer2017), we examined the possible impact of the VE task on both forms of ageism. This approach is innovative as past research has looked only at implicit measures in the context of VE.

Our findings suggest that if only a single implicit measure were used, young people who are virtually embodied via an older avatar’s image would have been less likely to show a negative bias towards older people. However, although the effect size of the VE task on the implicit measure was moderate to large, it was nonsignificant once multiple comparisons were considered. This finding should be viewed considering past VE research which had failed to report findings concerning explicit measures. The present study, in contrast, examined both implicit and explicit outcomes and found no differences between respondents who experienced VE with a young vs. an older avatar once multiple comparisons were considered. In contrast to the implicit measure, which would have been significant had it been used as a single outcome measure, there were no differences on the explicit measures of ageism, which captured both self- and other-directed ageism, regardless of the number of comparisons.

To date, VE interventions that have shown effectiveness in reducing negative attitudes towards out groups have relied on implicit measures (Banakou et al., Reference Banakou, Hanumanthu and Slater2016; La Rocca et al., Reference La Rocca, Andrea, Tosi and Daini2020). Our study adds by highlighting the fact that none of the explicit measures showed a significant effect. Moreover, once both implicit and explicit measures are taken into account, the effects of the VE task become nonsignificant even for the implicit measure, which had a moderate to large effect size.

Conclusions and limitations

The VE experiment used in this study was quite simple. It required participants to perform varied tasks, such as taking items off the counter or lifting their hands following the instructions of the person administering the study. Supposedly, the only difference experienced by participants assigned to one of the two groups was the age of the avatar staring back at them in the mirror and representing their own reflection. Future research will benefit from a more detailed elaboration of differences between avatars. For instance, pace and steadiness of movement could differ between the two avatars, with the older avatar being slower or even more shaky than the younger avatar. Moreover, a task which includes other people and their reactions to the avatar could also be informative and perhaps even educate participants about exposure to ageism. For instance, the older avatar might experience people trying to help him or her cross the street or lift the groceries without even asking or might experience social exclusion and social invisibility as a means to educate participants about the negative experiences of many older people (Scharf et al., Reference Scharf, Phillipson and Smith2005). In reflecting on the experiment, it also is important to note that although the two avatars are supposed to differ only based on their age, it is possible that the older avatar was deemed less physically attractive compared with the younger avatar. This difference, however, reflects differences in attitudes towards younger and older people’s attractiveness in general (He et al., Reference He, Workman, Kenett, He and Chatterjee2021).

Another limitation of the study concerns using a randomized procedure, which relied on a single time point to measure the immediate effects of the intervention. In addition, as already noted, there is no clear consensus concerning the best way to measure ageism (Ayalon et al., Reference Ayalon2019). Moreover, the value of implicit measures is still questionable (Meissner et al., Reference Meissner, Grigutsch, Koranyi, Müller and Rothermund2019). As such, the present findings should be reviewed within an emerging field, which is still looking for an answer concerning the best ways to capture undesirable, multidimensional constructs, such as ageism. Our findings question the value of VE as a possible way to reduce ageism towards older people, once multiple outcomes are taken into account. As ageism is a major threat to the health and wellbeing of older people worldwide, it is our duty to continue and identify future interventions that may reduce ageism as well as measures that capture changes brought by such interventions. Our findings point to the questionable beneficial effects of VE on implicit age bias towards older people, once explicit measures of ageism also are considered. Further research is needed to explore the long-term effects of this intervention as well as to better understand the multidimensional nature of ageism as captured by the different measures in the context of a VE intervention.

Acknowledgements

We wish to thank Dr. Assaf Suberry for his tremendous assistance with the implementation and analysis of the implicit measure.

This work was supported by a grant from the Impact Center for the Study of Ageism and Old Age provided by Gabi Weisfeld.

Conflicts of interest

None.

Source of funding

Impact Center for the Study of Ageism and Old Age.

Description of authors’ roles

LA: concept development, analysis, and write-up.

ED: concept development and intervention design.

SF: concept development and critical revisions.

References

Ayalon, L. et al. (2019). A systematic review of existing ageism scales. Ageing Research Reviews, 54, 100919.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ayalon, L. and Tesch-Römer, C. (2017). Taking a closer look at ageism: self-and other-directed ageist attitudes and discrimination. European Journal of Ageing, 14, 14.Google Scholar
Bailey, J. O., Bailenson, J. N. and Casasanto, D. (2016). When does virtual embodiment change our minds? Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments, 25, 222233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Banakou, D., Hanumanthu, P. D. and Slater, M. (2016). Virtual embodiment of white people in a black virtual body leads to a sustained reduction in their implicit racial bias. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10, 601. DOI 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00601.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bem, D. J. (1972). Self-perception theory. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 6, 162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
FitzGerald, C., Martin, A., Berner, D. and Hurst, S. (2019). Interventions designed to reduce implicit prejudices and implicit stereotypes in real world contexts: a systematic review. BMC Psychology, 7, 112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forscher, P. S. et al. (2019). A meta-analysis of procedures to change implicit measures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 117, 522559.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fox, J., Bailenson, J. N. and Tricase, L. (2013). The embodiment of sexualized virtual selves: the Proteus effect and experiences of self-objectification via avatars. Computers in Human Behavior, 29, 930938.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gawronski, B. and Brannon, S. M. (2018). Attitudes and the implicit-explicit dualism. In: Albarracin, D. and Johnson, B. T. (Eds.), The Handbook of Attitudes. Volume 1: Basic Principles (pp 158196). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Hamilton-Giachritsis, C., Banakou, D., Garcia Quiroga, M., Giachritsis, C. and Slater, M. (2018). Reducing risk and improving maternal perspective-taking and empathy using virtual embodiment. Scientific Reports, 8, 110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
He, D., Workman, C. I., Kenett, Y. N., He, X. and Chatterjee, A. (2021). The effect of aging on facial attractiveness: an empirical and computational investigation. Acta Psychologica, 219, 103385.Google ScholarPubMed
Hehman, E., Calanchini, J., Flake, J. K. and Leitner, J. B. (2019). Establishing construct validity evidence for regional measures of explicit and implicit racial bias. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 148, 10221040.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hudson, J. et al. (2018). Using virtual experiences of older age: exploring pedagogical and psychological experiences of students. In: Proceedings of the Virtual and Augmented Reality to Enhance Learning and Teaching in Higher Education Conference 2018 (pp. 6172). VR/AR Conference 2018, 12 September 2018, Swansea, UK.Google Scholar
Hummert, M. L., Garstka, T. A., O'Brien, L. T., Greenwald, A. G. and Mellott, D. S. (2002). Using the implicit association test to measure age differences in implicit social cognitions. Psychology and Aging, 17, 482495.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kurdi, B. et al. (2019). Relationship between the Implicit Association Test and intergroup behavior: a meta-analysis. American Psychologist, 74, 569586.Google ScholarPubMed
La Rocca, S., Andrea, B., Tosi, G. and Daini, R. (2020). No country for old men: reducing age bias through virtual reality embodiment. Annual Review of CyberTherapy and Telemedicine, 18/2020, 127.Google Scholar
Lawton, M. P. (1975). The Philadelphia geriatric center morale scale: a revision. Journal of Gerontology, 30, 8589.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levy, B. R. (2001). Eradication of ageism requires addressing the enemy within. The Gerontologist, 41, 578579.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levy, B. R. and Banaji, M. R. (2002). Implicit ageism. In: Nelson, T. (Ed.), Ageism: Stereotyping and Prejudice Aagainst Oolder Persons (pp 4975). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Li, B. J. and Kyung Kim, H. (2021). Experiencing organ failure in virtual reality: effects of self- versus other-embodied perspective taking on empathy and prosocial outcomes. New Media & Society, 23, 21442166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meissner, F., Grigutsch, L. A., Koranyi, N., Müller, F. and Rothermund, K. (2019). Predicting behavior with implicit measures: disillusioning findings, reasonable explanations, and sophisticated solutions. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 179. DOI 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02483.Google ScholarPubMed
North, M. and Fiske, S. (2013). A prescriptive intergenerational-tension ageism scale: succession, identity, and consumption (SIC). Psychological Assessment, 25, 706713.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oh, S. Y., Bailenson, J., Weisz, E. and Zaki, J. (2016). Virtually old: embodied perspective taking and the reduction of ageism under threat. Computers in Human Behavior, 60, 398410.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reinhard, R., Shah, K. G., Faust-Christmann, C. A. and Lachmann, T. (2020). Acting your avatar’s age: effects of virtual reality avatar embodiment on real life walking speed. Media Psychology, 23, 293315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roth, D. and Latoschik, M. E. (2019). Construction of a validated virtual embodiment questionnaire. Preprint, arXiv: 1911.10176.Google Scholar
Scharf, T., Phillipson, C. and Smith, A. E. (2005). Social exclusion of older people in deprived urban communities of England. European Journal of Ageing, 2, 7687.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sriram, N. and Greenwald, A. G. (2009). The brief implicit association test. Experimental Psychology, 56, 283294.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
World Health Organization. (2020). UN Decade of Healthy Ageing: plan of action. Available at: https://www.who.int/publications/m/item/decade-of-healthy-ageing-plan-of-action Google Scholar
World Health Organization. (2021). Global report on ageism: executive summary. Available at: https://www.who.int/publications/i/item/9789240020504 Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. An image of an older avatar.

Figure 1

Figure 2. An image of a younger avatar.

Figure 2

Table 1. Differences in sample characteristics between the old vs. young VE tasks (N = 80)