Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dlnhk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T19:58:22.046Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cultural evolution of emotional expression in 50 years of song lyrics

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 November 2019

Charlotte O. Brand*
Affiliation:
Human Behaviour and Cultural Evolution Group, Biosciences, College of Life and Environmental Sciences, University of Exeter, Penryn, UK
Alberto Acerbi
Affiliation:
Faculty of Science, Department for Early Prehistory and Quaternary Ecology, University of Tübingen, Germany
Alex Mesoudi
Affiliation:
Human Behaviour and Cultural Evolution Group, Biosciences, College of Life and Environmental Sciences, University of Exeter, Penryn, UK
*
*Corresponding Author: Human Behaviour and Cultural Evolution Group, Biosciences, College of Life and Environmental Sciences, University of Exeter, Penryn TR10 9FE, UK. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Popular music offers a rich source of data that provides insights into long-term cultural evolutionary dynamics. One major trend in popular music, as well as other cultural products such as literary fiction, is an increase over time in negatively valenced emotional content, and a decrease in positively valenced emotional content. Here we use two large datasets containing lyrics from n = 4913 and n = 159,015 pop songs respectively and spanning 1965–2015, to test whether cultural transmission biases derived from the cultural evolution literature can explain this trend towards emotional negativity. We find some evidence of content bias (negative lyrics do better in the charts), prestige bias (best-selling artists are copied) and success bias (best-selling songs are copied) in the proliferation of negative lyrics. However, the effects of prestige and success bias largely disappear when unbiased transmission is included in the models, which assumes that the occurrence of negative lyrics is predicted by their past frequency. We conclude that the proliferation of negative song lyrics may be explained partly by content bias, and partly by undirected, unbiased cultural transmission.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s) 2019

Media summary: Increase in negative song lyrics and decrease in positive in 50 years of pop music probably due to cultural drift.

Introduction

Are the lyrics of contemporary pop songs happier or sadder than the lyrics of the popular songs of previous generations? Do current songs mention love more or less than they used to? Are the pop charts angrier now than in the past, or have they mellowed over time? Millions of people buy and listen to popular music every day (by ‘popular music’ or ‘pop music’, we mean music with wide appeal that is typically distributed to large audiences through the music industry, and not just the specific ‘pop’ genre), and their song choices offer a window into their emotional states and psychological preferences. Changing trends in pop music over time may therefore offer a means of measuring large-scale societal changes. In recent years, the availability of large datasets in electronic format has allowed long-term, population-level cultural dynamics to be identified in an increasingly precise, quantitative way (Michel et al. Reference Michel, Shen, Aiden, Veres and Gray2011). This, in turn, permits researchers to test hypotheses about cultural trends that previously could only be assessed informally by focusing on a small number of (potentially cherry-picked) cases.

A fruitful area of investigation concerns the analysis of emotions in human cultural expressions. Several tools have been developed to extract the emotional content of texts, known as ‘sentiment analysis’. Some of these provide a classification of how words score on ‘positive’ and ‘negative’ content (Baccianella et al. Reference Baccianella, Esuli and Sebastiani2010), and others provide additional scores for specific emotions (e.g. how ‘angry’ or ‘sad’ is a text; Pennebaker et al. Reference Pennebaker, Chung, Ireland, Gonzales and Booth2007). In most cases, sentiment analysis has been applied on a short-term time scale, such as social media interactions (Lansdall-Welfare et al. Reference Lansdall-Welfare, Lampos and Cristianini2012). However, some researchers have explored a longer time scale, analysing the expression of emotions in several decades of song lyrics (Dodds and Danforth Reference Dodds and Danforth2010), newspaper articles (Iliev et al. Reference Iliev, Hoover, Dehghani and Axelrod2016), in Grimm's folktales (Mohammad Reference Mohammad2013) or in centuries of literary works (Acerbi et al. Reference Acerbi, Lampos, Garnett and Bentley2013).

The quantitative description of trends is fundamental, but a further necessary step is to understand what drives these trends. Cultural evolution theory (Boyd and Richerson, Reference Boyd and Richerson1985; Cavalli-Sforza and Feldman, Reference Cavalli-Sforza and Feldman1981; Mesoudi Reference Mesoudi2011) provides a series of concepts that allows such an endeavour. Drawing a parallel with genetic evolution, this field focuses on how cultural variation is transmitted from person to person via social learning processes such as imitation, and the processes that change that transmitted variation over time. In particular, cultural evolutionists have focused on transmission or learning biases as key drivers of cultural evolutionary dynamics (R. L. Kendal et al. Reference Kendal, Boogert, Rendell, Laland, Webster and Jones2018; Rendell et al. Reference Rendell, Boyd, Cownden, Enquist, Eriksson, Feldman and eldmann2010). Transmission biases are heuristics that individuals use to decide what, when and from whom to copy. They are rule-of-thumb principles such as ‘copy the majority’, ‘copy the successful’ or ‘copy the prestigious’ that allow individuals to adaptively learn from others (Laland Reference Laland2004). Importantly, different transmission biases give rise to different population-level cultural dynamics. A cultural trait introduced in a population in which, for example, individuals copy mostly from their parents will spread slower than the same cultural trait introduced in a population in which individuals copy mostly from a few successful or prestigious individuals (Cavalli-Sforza and Feldman, Reference Cavalli-Sforza and Feldman1981).

‘Model-based’ biases describe from whom people learn: for example, a success bias describes a tendency to learn from successful others, and a prestige bias a tendency to learn from prestigious (high status, respected) others. ‘Content-based’ biases describe what kind of information people learn best, owing to its salience or memorability. For example, a bias to transmit emotionally salient content, or negative content, has been found in several laboratory experiments (Bebbington et al. Reference Bebbington, MacLeod, Ellison and Fay2017; Fessler et al. Reference Fessler, Pisor and Navarrete2014; Stubbersfield et al. Reference Stubbersfield, Tehrani and Flynn2015, Reference Stubbersfield, Tehrani and Flynn2017). These biases can be compared with unbiased transmission, in which cultural variants are transmitted in equal proportion to their existing frequency in the population. While there are many theoretical models that examine the conditions under which different transmission biases are adaptive and their expected population-level consequences (J. Kendal et al. Reference Kendal, Giraldeau and Laland2009; Rendell et al. Reference Rendell, Boyd, Cownden, Enquist, Eriksson, Feldman and eldmann2010), and experiments which have tested the predictions of these models in controlled laboratory set-ups (Caldwell and Millen Reference Caldwell and Millen2008; Mesoudi Reference Mesoudi2008; Mesoudi and O'Brien Reference Mesoudi and Ondudii2008; Morgan et al. Reference Morgan, Rendell, Ehn, Hoppitt and Laland2012), less research has explored how cultural transmission biases may impact real-life cultural dynamics (although see Acerbi and Bentley Reference Acerbi and Bentley2014; Beheim et al. Reference Beheim, Thigpen and Mcelreath2014; Miu et al. Reference Miu, Gulley, Laland and Rendell2018).

In this study, we test the extent to which transmission biases can explain trends in the emotional content of two datasets of English language song lyrics. The first dataset (‘billboard’) contains the lyrics of the songs included in the annual Billboard Hot 100 from 1965 to 2015, a widely known US chart (n = 4913 songs). The second dataset (‘mxm’) contains the lyrics of the English language songs present in the musixmatch.com website, the world's largest lyrics platform where users can search and share lyrics, from 1965 to 2010 (n = 159,015 songs).

Preliminary analyses found a substantial decrease in the use of positive emotion-related words coupled with an increase in the use of negative emotion-related words in both datasets. Specific emotion-related words show considerable change in use during the time frame considered. For example, use of the term ‘love’ more than halved in both datasets, whereas the term ‘hate’ increased in frequency substantially (see Figure 1 for the ‘billboard’ dataset. The trends are qualitatively similar in the mxm dataset). These results are broadly consistent with previous analyses of song lyrics (DeWall et al. Reference DeWall, Pond, Campbell and Twenge2011; Dodds and Danforth Reference Dodds and Danforth2010) and literary fiction (Morin and Acerbi Reference Morin and Acerbi2017), suggesting a general cultural or artistic trend for increasingly negatively valenced emotional expressions.

Figure 1. Proportion of the term love (left panel) and hate (right panel) in all song lyrics by year for the dataset billboard which contains the lyrics of the songs included in the annual US Billboard Hot 100 (n = 4913 songs). The proportions here are small as we are reporting the proportion of the word out of the total number of words in 100 songs each year (on average 30,000 words, i.e. 300 words/song) and on different scales (the frequency of positive emotion words is usually higher than the frequency of negative emotion words). To have an intuitive idea of the change, from 1965 to 1990, in the top-100 billboard songs, the word hate was used each year around four or five times overall (30,000*0.00015), whereas now the average is around 24 (30,000 × 0.0008).

The main goal of this study is to test hypotheses about possible drivers of these two trends. Before testing for the aforementioned transmission biases, we first checked whether linguistic effects could explain the patterns. We considered (a) a possible increase in slang words, (b) an asymmetric semantic change (for which words denoting negative emotions had acquired positive or neutral connotation, e.g. ‘wicked’, but not vice versa) and (c) a general increase in lyric complexity (although note the latter would predict that both negative- and positive-emotion words would decrease in frequency). After finding that the trend persisted after controlling for these linguistic effects (see Supplementary Material), we then examined whether cultural transmission biases might explain the patterns.

We considered, in a fully preregistered study, three hypotheses derived from cultural evolution theory as outlined above:

(H1) Success bias: the emotional trends result from artists copying the best-selling songs from the preceding years.

(H2) Prestige bias: the emotional trends result from artists copying the songs of ‘prestigious’ artists (independently of the success of the songs) from the preceding years.

(H3) Content bias: there is a general psychological preference for lyrics that reflect negative emotions in songs, thus songs with more negative content rank higher in the charts.

Note that we applied H1 and H2 to both positive and negative content, while H3 was applied only to negative content. This was because, although there is ample evidence for a content bias towards negative emotion, as referenced above, we are not aware of any evidence or theory that would predict a content bias for positive emotions.

After running our preregistered analyses to test these three preregistered hypotheses and depositing a preprint, we received the useful suggestion to also include an additional variable to control for unbiased transmission. Including this additional variable affected the interpretation of our results. Hence below we report both the preregistered analyses without unbiased transmission and the revised analyses including unbiased transmission. The use of unbiased transmission here assumes that the emotional trends of lyrics result from artists randomly copying the lyrics of any of the available songs in the preceding years, without taking into account chart rank or artist prestige.

Our results are mixed. We found a small effect of success bias on the likelihood of a word being positive in the billboard dataset, as well as a small effect of both prestige and content bias on the likelihood of a word being negative in the billboard dataset. However, these effects vastly reduced or became non-existent in the mxm dataset. Moreover, when controlling for unbiased transmission, the effects of success and prestige largely disappeared in both datasets. We therefore conclude that content bias may play a role in the likelihood of using negative emotion words in song lyrics, but that success and prestige biases (as we have implemented them) are not strong enough to explain the trends compared with unbiased transmission. We discuss possible explanations for the apparent content bias effect, as well as our interpretation and implementation of success, prestige and unbiased transmission in relation to the content of song lyrics.

Methods

Data preparation

Dataset ‘mxm’

We downloaded (in August 2017) the bag-of-words data (n = 237,662 songs) from https://labrosa.ee.columbia.edu/millionsong/musixmatch. We used the mxm track ID to retrieve from musixmatch.com the name of the artist(s), the year of first release and the genre for each song.

From this dataset we eliminated songs for which it was not possible to retrieve the year or the artist, as well as songs that did not appear to be in English. To assess if a song was in English we checked whether the word ‘the’ was present at least once. The new dataset contained n = 163,551 songs. We additionally filtered the dataset by retaining only the years for which more than 500 songs were present, thus obtaining our final sample of n = 159,015 songs, ranging from 1965 to 2010, both included.

Artists’ names were further processed using the cluster function in R library refinr (Muir Reference Muir2018) to cluster and merge similar names (e.g. ‘madonna’, ‘Madonna’, ‘MADONNA’). To disambiguate collaborations we looked for standard separators in artist names (e.g. ‘featuring’, ‘feat.’, ‘feat’, ‘and’, ‘AND’, ‘and’, ‘with’, ‘,’). We considered artists where no separators were found as single artists. We then ran the strings where separators were found (e.g. X and Y) through the list of single artists. If both X and Y were found in this list, then X and Y were considered as single artists occasionally collaborating (e.g. Eminem and Dr. Dre), if not, they were considered a stable collaboration, and a single data point in our models (e.g. Simon and Garfunkel).

Dataset ‘billboard’

We downloaded (in May 2017) the data from https://github.com/walkerkq/musiclyrics (n = 4913 songs). As the lyrics in the mxm dataset are stemmed (a common practice in digital text analysis: words are reduced to their stems, roughly analogous to their morphological roots, e.g. ‘happily’, ‘happy’, and ‘happiness’ are all reduced to the stemmed form – ‘happi’), we processed the ‘billboard’ dataset in the same way, re-adapting the script used to process the mxm dataset: https://github.com/tbertinmahieux/MSongsDB/blob/master/Tasks_Demos/Lyrics/lyrics_to_bow.py. Henceforth when we use the term ‘word’ or ‘lyric’ we are referring to the stemmed version.

Artists were further processed in the same way as the mxm dataset. Notice the ‘genre’ entry is not present in this dataset as this information was not provided by the Billboard data source, but, importantly, chart position is, which we coded as ‘rank’. Rank is the song's chart position, from 1 to 100, in the yearly Billboard list, where 1 indicates the best-selling and 100 indicates the least-selling song.

Sentiment analysis

We used the ‘positive emotions’ and ‘negative emotions’ categories of the text analysis application Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count (Pennebaker et al. Reference Pennebaker, Chung, Ireland, Gonzales and Booth2007). These categories are ‘virtually unchanged’ compared with the most recent (2015) version of LIWC. The words were stemmed as described above, and we analysed the lyrics with n = 267 negative emotion stems and n = 169 positive emotion stems. Thus each word of each song was classified as positive, negative or neither. As noted in the introduction, the practice of analysing the emotional valence of texts using single words as units is common and prevalent in natural language processing studies (a recent review is Ribeiro et al. Reference Ribeiro, Araújo, Gonçalves, Gonçalves and Benevenuto2016).

Data analysis

Statistical approach

We used Bayesian, aggregated binomial, multilevel models, to examine the effect of prestige, success and content bias on the likelihood of any given word being either positive or not, and negative or not. We conducted all analyses in R version 3.6.0 using the Rethinking package (R Core Team 2019; McElreath Reference McElreath2016). We compared WAICs (‘Widely Applicable Information Criteria’, see McElreath Reference McElreath2016) in a model comparison approach as we stated in our preregistration. WAICs can be thought of as a type of information criteria, such as the better-known AIC, but one that uses the entire posterior distribution for estimation rather than a single point estimate. However, it is important to note that we interpreted the parameter estimates of the full models in all cases, based on advice regarding the instability of WAIC estimates for our time series data. Model parameters were said to have an effect on the model outcome if their 89% credible interval did not cross zero (NB. 89% is the default value in the rethinking package). Priors were chosen to be weakly regularising, in order to control for both under- and overfitting of the model to the data. Owing to the large number of data points that each model processed (each model of the mxm dataset processed roughly 29 million words), and skewed samples within genre and artist categories, we had to reparameterise and adapt the models accordingly to ensure sufficient and appropriate convergence. Trace plots, effective sample sizes and Rhat values were used to check for appropriate model convergence throughout.

Full analysis scripts and data are available at www.github.com/lottybrand22/song_lyrics, and were preregistered at https://osf.io/94ubt/

Level of analysis

Each word of a song was coded, according to LIWC, as ‘positive’ (when present in the list of positive stems), ‘negative’ (when present in the list of negative stems) or neither. Thus, we implemented separate models for coding positive and negative lyrics (see Tables 1 and 2). Our models are aggregated binomial models, in that the words are aggregated within songs, but each word of a song is modelled as the binomial probability of being positive (or not). The negative models model the likelihood that each word in a song is negative (or not). This takes into account the fact that each song has a different number of words, and negates any need for averaging over words and songs. Each song is not an independent data point, as the data are clustered on artists, genre and year of release. Thus, we implemented varying effects models, allowing adaptively regularising priors for the intercepts of artist, genre and year of release.

Table 1. Details of model comparison results for the billboard dataset models. The model with the lowest WAIC and highest proportion of the WAIC weight from each set is in bold. Please note that we always used the full models for inference, as the WAIC standard errors contain a lot of overlap, see Results section for more information

Table 2. Details of model comparison results for the mxm dataset models. The model with the lowest WAIC and highest proportion of the WAIC weight from each set is in bold. Please note that we always used the full models for inference, as the WAIC standard errors contain a lot of overlap, see Results section for more information

Hypotheses

See Table 1 for specifications of all models. The success bias models assume that the probability that any given word in a song is positive (or negative) can be predicted by the average number of positive (or negative) words of the top-10 songs in the preceding three years of the billboard list. Thus our variable ‘success’ for both mxm and billboard datasets consisted of the average number of positive (or negative) words from the top-10 songs of the billboard dataset, for the preceding three years of the song of interest.

The prestige bias models assume that the probability that any given word in a song is positive (or negative) can be predicted by the average number of positive (or negative) words of the songs of prestigious artists in the preceding three years. We define ‘prestigious’ artists as those that appeared more than 10 times in the Billboard top-100. This results in 86 ‘prestigious’ artists (less than 4% of the total in the billboard dataset).

The content bias models assume that the probability that a word of a song will be negative is predicted by the chart positions of the song in the billboard charts, which we assume reflects a psychological bias, or preference for, negative content in songs.

A control for unbiased transmission is implemented by including an effect for the average number of positive (or negative) words of all songs in the preceding three years. This assumes that the probability that any given word in a song is positive (or negative) increases as the frequency of positive (or negative) words in the population of recent songs increases.

Note that many of these choices are necessarily subjective (e.g. using the top-10 songs for success bias, or more than 10 appearances in the charts for prestige). We invite readers to explore alternative choices by adapting our analysis scripts for use on the openly released data.

Results

Model comparison

Our model comparison results suggested that, although some models hold the majority of the weight for the billboard models, it is important to note that their WAIC values are not very different from one another, and the standard errors of the differences show that there is a lot of uncertainty in the model comparison. Furthermore, model comparison is less robust when modelling time-series data. Therefore, we are reporting the results of the full models that include all of the effects of interest, while noting that the model comparison results should be interpreted with caution. We first report the full models without a parameter for unbiased transmission as was originally preregistered. We then report how the results change when controlling for unbiased transmission.

Models of positive lyrics using the billboard dataset

When modelling the likelihood that any word in the lyrics of a song is positive, the full model (Fig. 2a) suggested that success had a small positive effect (mean coefficient estimate: 0.04, 89% confidence interval (CI) [0.00, 0.07], however prestige did not have a detectable effect (0.02, CI [−0.01, 0.06]). These effects are on the log-odds scale, and thus can be interpreted as the odds that a given word is positive increases by 4% as the number of positive words in the previous three years of top-10 songs increases. The model comparison suggests support for H1, suggesting that our hypothesis that success bias contributes to the proportion of positive lyrics was best supported, although there is a lot of uncertainty around the models’ WAIC estimates. However, when controlling for unbiased transmission, the updated full model (Fig. 2b) suggested that neither success nor prestige had a detectable effect (−0.03, CI [−0.07, −0.01], −0.01 CI [−0.04, 0.02]), but that our implementation of unbiased transmission predicted the likelihood of a given word in the lyrics of a song being positive (0.12, CI [0.07, 0.16]).

Figure 2. (a) Parameter estimates from the full positive billboard model. Estimates include 89% confidence intervals. Estimates that cross zero are interpreted as not having a strong effect on the probability of a lyric being positive. (b) Parameter estimates from the full positive billboard model with unbiased transmission included. (c) Parameter estimates from the full negative billboard model. (d) Parameter estimates from the full negative billboard mode with unbiased transmission included.

Models of negative lyrics using the billboard dataset

When modelling the likelihood that any word in the lyrics of a song is negative, the full model (Fig. 2c) suggested that prestige had a positive effect (mean coefficient estimate: 0.08, 89% CI [0.01, 0.14]; however success did not have a strong effect (0.01, CI [−0.05, 0.07]). Content bias had a negative effect, meaning that songs that had a numerically lower chart position (i.e. closer to the top of the charts at 1 rather than 100) increased the log odds of a lyric being negative (−0.02, CI [−0.04, −0.01]). When controlling for unbiased transmission (Fig. 2d), the model suggested that neither success nor prestige had a detectable effect on the likelihood of a lyric being negative (0.01, CI [−0.05, 0.06], 0.04 CI [−0.04, 0.12]). However content bias retained its effect, in that a lower chart position increased the likelihood of a lyric being negative (−0.02, CI [−0.04, −0.01]). There was not strong evidence of unbiased transmission (0.06, CI [−0.02, 0.13]).

Models of positive lyrics using the mxm dataset

When modelling the likelihood that any word in the lyrics of a song in the mxm dataset is positive (Fig. 3a), the full model suggested that prestige had a small positive effect (mean coefficient estimate 0.01, 89% CI [0.00, 0.02]; however success did not have an effect (0.00, CI [−0.01, 0.01]). The prestige effect is much smaller than those from the billboard models. The model comparison suggested that the best model was the null model, suggesting that our parameters for success and prestige did not improve the model fit more than simply including the varying effects for artist, genre and year of release. Furthermore, when controlling for unbiased transmission (Fig. 3b) we found no effect of prestige and success (0.00, CI [0.00, 0.01], −0.01 CI [−0.02, 0.00]) and weak evidence of unbiased transmission (0.01, CI [0.00, 0.03]).

Figure 3. (a) Parameter estimates from the full positive mxm model. Estimates include 89% confidence intervals. Estimates that cross zero are interpreted as not having a strong effect on the probability of a lyric being positive. (b) Parameter estimates from the full positive mxm model with unbiased transmission included. (c) Parameter estimates from the full negative mxm model. (d) Parameter estimates from the full negative mxm model with unbiased transmission included.

Models of negative lyrics using the mxm dataset

When modelling the likelihood that any word in the lyrics of a song is negative, the full model (Fig. 3c) suggested that neither prestige nor success had an effect (prestige 0.00, 89% CI [−0.02, 0.01]; success 0.00, CI [−0.01, 0.01]). Furthermore, the model comparison indicated that the best model was the null model, suggesting that our parameters for success and prestige did not improve the model fit more than simply including the varying effects for year, artist, genre. Similarly, when controlling for unbiased transmission (Fig. 3d) we found no effect of prestige and success, (−0.02, CI [−0.03, 0.01], 0.00 CI [−0.01, 0.01]) and weak evidence of unbiased transmission (0.02, CI [0.00, 0.03]).

Discussion

We analysed the emotional content of song lyrics in over 160,000 songs spanning the years 1965–2015. We found that the frequency of negative words increased over time, whilst the frequency of positive words decreased over time, and asked whether these patterns could be attributed to cultural transmission biases such as success bias, prestige bias, content bias or unbiased transmission. In the billboard dataset, containing top-100 songs from 1965 to 2015, we found an effect of unbiased transmission on positive lyrics, and an effect of content bias on negative lyrics. For the larger mxm databases we only found weak effects of unbiased transmission for both negative and positive lyrics.

The effects we found in all models are extremely small. This is partly because we analysed the data on the scale of each word, negating any need for averaging over lyrics and songs. Thus, the relative increase or decrease in the log odds is understandably small. Furthermore, our implementation of transmission biases is necessarily indirect and simplified given that we lack direct observations of song lyrics being copied. It is therefore unsurprising that the effects vastly reduced or disappeared when controlling for unbiased transmission, given how many other factors must be at play in the generation of song lyrics, both directional biases such as those we explored here and random processes (Bentley et al. Reference Bentley, Lipo, Herzog and Hahn2007). For example, prestige can be realised in myriad ways (Jiménez and Mesoudi Reference Jiménez and Mesoudi2019), particularly in the music industry. The effect of various recording companies, the extent of media attention outside of the charts and the amount of money spent on music promotion may all play a significant role in an artist's apparent prestige, and is not necessarily restricted to the content of their music. Our implementation of ‘prestige’ as predominance in the charts therefore only captures one specific aspect of musical prestige.

The effect of unbiased transmission is, however, the largest and most consistent in all of our models. This result suggests there may be an effect of random drift, or random copying, in the emotional content of song lyrics over time. This is consistent with previous work showing that random copying can explain changes in the popularity of dog breeds, baby names and popular music (Bentley et al. Reference Bentley, Lipo, Herzog and Hahn2007; Hahn and Bentley Reference Hahn and Bentley2003), as well as archaeological pottery and technological patents (Bentley et al. Reference Bentley, Hahn and Shennan2004). Thus, rather than song-writers being influenced by the most prestigious or successful artists, they may simply be influenced by the emotional content of any of the available song lyrics in the previous timestep, which may happen to increase in negativity or decrease in positivity owing to small fluctuations. As in previous work, our results do not provide evidence of literal random copying by individuals as we do not have direct access to individual's copying decisions. Instead, random drift is posed as a baseline against which to compare evidence of other copying biases. It is possible that the population-wide patterns are not a result of unanimous random copying, but owing to a multitude of idiosyncratic causes that collectively cancel each other out to create the appearance of random copying (Hoppitt and Laland Reference Hoppitt and Laland2013). In this sense, any small fluctuation in negative words owing to a particular historical event, or owing to the emergence of a more negatively biased genre, may have caused an initial increase in negative lyrics, which became exacerbated by random drift.

The presence of a content bias in the likelihood of negative lyrics occurring in the billboard songs is noteworthy. This result suggests that songs with more negative lyrics are more successful in general, perhaps reflecting either a general negativity bias (Bebbington et al. Reference Bebbington, MacLeod, Ellison and Fay2017; Fessler et al. Reference Fessler, Pisor and Navarrete2014) or an art-specific, or music-specific, negativity bias. Similar trends favouring negative emotions vs positive ones in other artistic domains support our finding. As mentioned above, Dodds and Danforth (Reference Dodds and Danforth2010) documented a decrease in frequency of positively valenced words, and an increase in negatively valenced ones in pop song lyrics (a similar result was found in DeWall et al. Reference DeWall, Pond, Campbell and Twenge2011). Morin and Acerbi (Reference Morin and Acerbi2017) found a similar pattern in centuries of literary fiction, with a general decrease in the frequency of words denoting emotions, explained by a decrease in words denoting positive emotions, whereas the frequency of negative words remained constant. It is worth noting that we were unable to look for content bias (with our implementation) in the mxm data as there was no ranking system. One possible way of determining the popularity or use of a song could be to look at how many times, or how often, its lyrics are searched for, and whether this correlates with negative content.

In general, the idea that negative emotions would be privileged in art is consistent with the hypothesis that artistic expressions may have an adaptive function, in particular as simulation of social interactions (Mar and Oatley Reference Mar and Oatley2008). According to this view, developed with literary fiction in mind but potentially generalisable to other expressive forms, art would provide hypothetical scenarios where we can test and train, with no risk, our cognitive and emotional reactions. From this perspective, simulating negative events is more useful than simulating positive ones (Clasen Reference Clasen2017; Gottschall Reference Gottschall2012). Art expressing negative emotions, in addition, may hold more value for audiences seeking comfort from the knowledge that others also experience negative emotions. Indeed, studies have shown that people underestimate the prevalence of others’ negative emotions, and this underestimation exacerbates loneliness and decreases life satisfaction (Jordan et al. Reference Jordan, Monin, Dweck, Lovett, John and Gross2011). Furthermore, suppressing rather than reappraising negative emotions decreases self-esteem and increases sadness (Nezlek and Kuppens Reference Nezlek and Kuppens2008)(Nezlek & Kuppens, Reference Nezlek and Kuppens2008). This hypothesis is worth investigating in future research.

Our varying effects models suggested that most of the variation lay between artists. However, genre also showed considerable variation. We were unable to control for genre in the billboard data as genre information was not available with this dataset. This could provide a partial explanation for the differing results between the billboard and mxm datasets; indeed, Dodds and Danforth (Reference Dodds and Danforth2010) attributed the decrease in emotional valence within pop song lyrics to the emergence of more negative genres such as heavy metal and punk. Future work investigating the variation of emotional expression between different genres of music would be valuable. A further limitation of this study is that we restricted our analysis to comparing the content of each song with that of the songs from the previous three years of songs. Mechanistically this suggests that songs that are currently in the charts influence song-writers who are writing within three years of chart success, assuming that the time it takes to get from the song-writing process to chart success is three years or less. It is possible that these effects are stronger or weaker at different time points, such as within one or five years of chart success. Furthermore, although we controlled for artist, many songs in the billboard charts are in fact written by specially designated song-writers, such as Max Martin.

Overall this research contributes to the growing body of work attempting to quantitatively study trends in the domain of music (Youngblood Reference Youngblood2019; Savage Reference Savage2019; Mauch et al. Reference Mauch Matthias, Levy and Leroi Armand2015; Ravignani et al. Reference Ravignani, Honing and Kotz2017). Our starting result of an increase in negative emotions and decrease in positive ones in song lyrics is paired with similar findings regarding acoustic qualities. Using the same Billboard top-100 songs that we analysed, Schellenberg and von Scheve (Reference Schellenberg and von Scheve2012) found an increase in minor mode and a decrease in the average tempo, which indicates that the songs become more sad-sounding through time. This seems to be part of a longer trend in Western classical music, where the use of the minor mode increased over a 150-year period from 1750 to 1900 (Horn and Huron Reference Horn and Huron2015). The relationship between minor tone and negative valence of lyrics has been also studied, and confirmed, quantitatively (Kolchinsky et al. Reference Kolchinsky, Dhande, Park and Ahn2017). Analogously, studying more than 500,000 songs released in the UK between 1985 and 2015, Interiano et al. (Reference Interiano, Kazemi, Wang, Yang, Yu and Komarova2018) found a similar decrease in ‘happiness’ and ‘brightness’, coupled with a slight increase in ‘sadness’ (these high-level features result from algorithms analysing low-level acoustic features, such as the tempo, the tonality, etc.). They also found the puzzling result that, despite a general trend towards sadder songs, the successful hits are, on average, happier than the rest of the songs. In the same way, whereas we found that the higher the position in the billboard chart the more negative a song is, billboard songs are as a whole more positive than the songs in the mxm dataset, which contains more (and less successful) songs.

In this study we used cultural evolutionary theory to try to explain patterns in one of the most pervasive of human cultural practices, music production. More specifically, we tried to detect whether any particular transmission bias best explained the changing patterns of emotional expression over time. We conclude that, although we found weak evidence of success and prestige biases, these were overwhelmed by an effect for unbiased transmission. The presence of a content bias for negative lyrics remained, and this may be a contributing factor to the increasing in negative lyrics over time. A potential explanation for these results is that a multitude of transmission biases and other causes are at play. It is likely that small shifts, for example owing to historical events or the emergence of particular genres, may have nudged the production and transmission of negative and positive lyrics in opposite directions, and random copying exacerbated this trajectory. These possibilities should be explored more in future work. Overall, the exercise of precisely analysing large datasets to explain cultural change, if refined on relatively benign cultural trends such as pop music, could eventually be more expertly applied to areas of greater societal importance and impact, such as shifts in political beliefs or moral preferences.

Supplementary Material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/ehs.2019.11

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank the following people who contributed valuable advice for this project. Dr Sally Street provided feedback on our preprint and suggested controlling for unbiased transmission in our full models; we are very grateful for this suggestion as it was a valuable addition to our final analyses. We are grateful to Professor James Pennebaker who answered our questions regarding the latest version of the LIWC programme. We are also grateful to Professor Richard McElreath who advised us on many aspects of our analyses.

Author contributions

AA retrieved the data and completed the initial exploration. AA, AM and CB designed the project and preregistration together. CB and AA wrote the analysis scripts. CB ran the analyses. CB AA and AM interpreted the results and wrote the manuscript together.

Funding

This research was supported by The Leverhulme Trust (grant no. RPG-2016-122 awarded to AM).

Publishing ethics

The authors declare that this work is in accordance with Cambridge University Press's publishing ethics guidelines, and is original and has not been published elsewhere, although it is available as a preprint on SocArXiv at https://osf.io/preprints/socarxiv/3j6wx/ and was fully preregistered at https://osf.io/wr3qm/registrations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

Acerbi, A and Bentley, RA (2014) Biases in cultural transmission shape the turnover of popular traits. Evolution and Human Behavior 35(3), 228236. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2014.02.003Google Scholar
Acerbi, A, Lampos, V, Garnett, P and Bentley, RA (2013) The expression of emotions in 20th century books. PLOS One 8(3), e59030. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0059030Google Scholar
Baccianella, S, Esuli, A and Sebastiani, F (2010) SENTIWORDNET 3.0: An Enhanced Lexical Resource for Sentiment Analysis and Opinion Mining, 5. https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Fabrizio_Sebastiani/publication/220746537_SentiWordNet_30_An_Enhanced_Lexical_Resource_for_Sentiment_Analysis_and_Opinion_Mining/links/545fbcc40cf27487b450aa21.pdf.Google Scholar
Bebbington, K, MacLeod, C, Ellison, TM and Fay, N (2017) The sky is falling: evidence of a negativity bias in the social transmission of information. Evolution and Human Behavior 38(1), 92101. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.07.004Google Scholar
Beheim, BA, Thigpen, C and Mcelreath, R (2014) Strategic social learning and the population dynamics of human behavior: the game of Go. Evolution and Human Behavior 35(5), 351357. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2014.04.001Google Scholar
Bentley, RA, Hahn, MW and Shennan, SJ (2004) Random drift and culture change. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences 271(1547), 14431450.Google Scholar
Bentley, RA, Lipo, CP, Herzog, HA and Hahn, MW (2007) Regular rates of popular culture change reflect random copying. Evolution and Human Behavior 28(3), 151158. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2006.10.002Google Scholar
Boyd, R and Richerson, PJ (1985) Culture and the Evolutionary Process. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Caldwell, CA and Millen, AE (2008) Experimental models for testing hypotheses about cumulative cultural evolution. Evolution and Human Behavior 29(3), 165171. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.12.001Google Scholar
Cavalli-Sforza, LL and Feldman, MW (1981) Cultural Transmission and Evolution: A Quantitative Approach. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Clasen, M (2017) Why Horror Seduces. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
DeWall, CN, Pond, RS Jr, Campbell, WK and Twenge, JM (2011) Tuning in to psychological change: Linguistic markers of psychological traits and emotions over time in popular U.S. song lyrics. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts 5(3), 200207. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023195Google Scholar
Dodds, PS and Danforth, CM (2010) Measuring the happiness of large-scale written expression: songs, blogs, and presidents. Journal of Happiness Studies 11(4), 441456. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-009-9150-9Google Scholar
Fessler, DMT, Pisor, AC and Navarrete, CD (2014) Negatively-biased credulity and the cultural evolution of beliefs. PLoS One 9(4), e95167. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0095167Google Scholar
Gottschall, J (2012) The Storytelling Animal: How Stories Make Us Human. London: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.Google Scholar
Hahn, MW and Bentley, RA(2003) Drift as a mechanism for cultural change: an example from baby names. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences 270(suppl_1), S120S123.Google Scholar
Hoppitt, W and Laland, KN (2013) Social Learning: An Introduction to Mechanisms, Methods, and Models. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Horn, K and Huron, D (2015) On the changing use of the major and minor modes 1750ng use Music Theory Online 21(1). https://doi.org/10.30535/mto.21.1.4Google Scholar
Iliev, R, Hoover, J, Dehghani, M and Axelrod, R (2016) Linguistic positivity in historical texts reflects dynamic environmental and psychological factors. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 113(49), E7871E7879. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1612058113Google Scholar
Interiano, M, Kazemi, K, Wang, L, Yang, J, Yu, Z and Komarova, NL (2018) Musical trends and predictability of success in contemporary songs in and out of the top charts. Royal Society Open Science 5(5), 171274.Google Scholar
Jiménez, ÁV and Mesoudi, A (2019) Prestige-biased social learning: current evidence and outstanding questions. Palgrave Communications 5(1), 20.Google Scholar
Jordan, AH, Monin, B, Dweck, CS, Lovett, BJ, John, OP and Gross, JJ (2011) Misery has more company than people think: underestimating the prevalence of others’ negative emotions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 37(1), 120135. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167210390822Google Scholar
Kendal, J, Giraldeau, L-A and Laland, K(2009) The evolution of social learning rules: payoff-biased and frequency-dependent biased transmission. Journal of Theoretical Biology 260(2), 210219. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jtbi.2009.05.029Google Scholar
Kendal, RL, Boogert, NJ, Rendell, L, Laland, KN, Webster, M and Jones, PL (2018) Social learning strategies: bridge-building between fields. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 22(7), 651665. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2018.04.003Google Scholar
Kolchinsky, A, Dhande, N, Park, K and Ahn, YY (2017) The minor fall, the major lift: inferring emotional valence of musical chords through lyrics. Royal Society Open Science 4(11), 170952.Google Scholar
Laland, KN(2004) Social learning strategies. Animal Learning & Behavior 32(1), 414.Google Scholar
Lansdall-Welfare, T, Lampos, V and Cristianini, N (2012) Effects of the recession on public mood in the UK. In Proceedings of the 21st international conference companion on World Wide Web – www'12 Companion (p. 1221) Lyon: ACM Press. https://doi.org/10.1145/2187980.2188264Google Scholar
Mar, RA and Oatley, K (2008) The function of fiction is the abstraction and simulation of social experience. Perspectives on Psychological Science 3(3), 173192. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00073.xGoogle Scholar
Mauch Matthias, MacCallum Robert M, Levy, Mark and Leroi Armand, M (2015) The evolution of popular music: USA 1960–2010. Royal Society Open Science 2(5), 150081. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.150081Google Scholar
McElreath, R (2016) Statistical Rethinking. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.Google Scholar
Mesoudi, A (2008) An experimental simulation of the ‘copy-successful-individuals’ cultural learning strategy: adaptive landscapes, producer–scrounger dynamics, and informational access costs. Evolution and Human Behavior 29(5), 350363. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2008.04.005Google Scholar
Mesoudi, A (2011) Cultural Evolution: How Darwinian Theory Can Explain Human Culture and Synthesize the Social Sciences. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Mesoudi, A and Ondudii, MJ (2008) The cultural transmission of great basin projectile-point technology I: An experimental simulation. American Antiquity 73(1), 328.Google Scholar
Michel, J-B, Shen, YK, Aiden, AP, Veres, A, Gray, MK, Team TGB, eamsa2 EL (2011) Quantitative analysis of culture using millions of digitized books. Science 331(6014), 176182. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1199644Google Scholar
Miu, E, Gulley, N, Laland, KN and Rendell, L (2018) Innovation and cumulative culture through tweaks and leaps in online programming contests. Nature Communications 9. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-018-04494-0Google Scholar
Mohammad, S (2013) From once upon a time to happily ever after: Tracking emotions in novels and fairy tales. ArXiv:1309.5909 [Cs]. Retrieved from http://arxiv.org/abs/1309.5909Google Scholar
Morgan, TJH, Rendell, LE, Ehn, M, Hoppitt, W and Laland, KN (2012) The evolutionary basis of human social learning. Proceedings of the Royal Society B 279(1729), 653662. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2011.1172Google Scholar
Morin, O and Acerbi, A (2017) Birth of the cool: a two-centuries decline in emotional expression in Anglophone fiction. Cognition and Emotion 31(8), 16631675. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2016.1260528Google Scholar
Muir, C (2018) Refinr: cluster and merge similar values within a character vector. R Package version 0.3.1. https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/refinr/refinr.pdfGoogle Scholar
Nezlek, JB and Kuppens, P (2008) Regulating positive and negative emotions in daily life. Journal of Personality 76(3), 561580. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2008.00496.xGoogle Scholar
Pennebaker, JW, Chung, CK, Ireland, M, Gonzales, A and Booth, RJ (2007) The development and psychometric properties of LIWC2007, 22. https://repositories.lib.utexas.edu/handle/2152/31333Google Scholar
Ravignani, A, Honing, H and Kotz, SA (2017) Editorial: the evolution of rhythm cognition: timing in music and speech. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2017.00303Google Scholar
R Core Team (2019) R: a Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Available at https://www.R-project.org/Google Scholar
Rendell, L, Boyd, R, Cownden, D, Enquist, M, Eriksson, K, Feldman, MW, eldmann, KN (2010) Why copy others? Insights from the social learning strategies tournament. Science, 328(5975), 208213. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1184719Google Scholar
Ribeiro, FN, Araújo, M, Gonçalves, P, Gonçalves, MA and Benevenuto, F (2016) Sentibench – a benchmark comparison of state-of-the-practice sentiment analysis methods. EPJ Data Science 5(1), 23.Google Scholar
Savage, PE (2019) Cultural evolution of music. Palgrave Communications 5(1), 16.Google Scholar
Schellenberg, EG and von Scheve, C (2012) Emotional cues in American popular music: five decades of the Top 40. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts 6(3), 196.Google Scholar
Stubbersfield, JM, Tehrani, JJ and Flynn, EG(2015) Serial killers, spiders and cybersex: social and survival information bias in the transmission of urban legends. British Journal of Psychology 106(2), 288307. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12073Google Scholar
Stubbersfield, JM, Tehrani, JJ and Flynn, EG (2017) Chicken tumours and a fishy revenge: evidence for emotional content bias in the cumulative recall of urban legends. Journal of Cognition and Culture 17(1–2), 1226. https://doi.org/10.1163/15685373-12342189Google Scholar
Youngblood, M (2019) Cultural transmission modes of music sampling traditions remain stable despite delocalization in the digital age. PloS One 14(2), e0211860.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Proportion of the term love (left panel) and hate (right panel) in all song lyrics by year for the dataset billboard which contains the lyrics of the songs included in the annual US Billboard Hot 100 (n = 4913 songs). The proportions here are small as we are reporting the proportion of the word out of the total number of words in 100 songs each year (on average 30,000 words, i.e. 300 words/song) and on different scales (the frequency of positive emotion words is usually higher than the frequency of negative emotion words). To have an intuitive idea of the change, from 1965 to 1990, in the top-100 billboard songs, the word hate was used each year around four or five times overall (30,000*0.00015), whereas now the average is around 24 (30,000 × 0.0008).

Figure 1

Table 1. Details of model comparison results for the billboard dataset models. The model with the lowest WAIC and highest proportion of the WAIC weight from each set is in bold. Please note that we always used the full models for inference, as the WAIC standard errors contain a lot of overlap, see Results section for more information

Figure 2

Table 2. Details of model comparison results for the mxm dataset models. The model with the lowest WAIC and highest proportion of the WAIC weight from each set is in bold. Please note that we always used the full models for inference, as the WAIC standard errors contain a lot of overlap, see Results section for more information

Figure 3

Figure 2. (a) Parameter estimates from the full positive billboard model. Estimates include 89% confidence intervals. Estimates that cross zero are interpreted as not having a strong effect on the probability of a lyric being positive. (b) Parameter estimates from the full positive billboard model with unbiased transmission included. (c) Parameter estimates from the full negative billboard model. (d) Parameter estimates from the full negative billboard mode with unbiased transmission included.

Figure 4

Figure 3. (a) Parameter estimates from the full positive mxm model. Estimates include 89% confidence intervals. Estimates that cross zero are interpreted as not having a strong effect on the probability of a lyric being positive. (b) Parameter estimates from the full positive mxm model with unbiased transmission included. (c) Parameter estimates from the full negative mxm model. (d) Parameter estimates from the full negative mxm model with unbiased transmission included.

Supplementary material: File

Brand et al. supplementary material

Brand et al. supplementary material

Download Brand et al. supplementary material(File)
File 1.6 MB