No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Past glories feel good but creative minorities push us forward
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 02 January 2025
Abstract
Historical narratives can satisfy basic individual psychological needs. However, an over-reliance on a group's past can marginalize those who think differently – thus, homogenizing the culture and stifling creativity. By revising narratives to balance the power of collective narratives with the richness of individuality, we foster groups that encourage varied identities.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press
References
Barnett, M. D., Öz, H. C. M., & Marsden, A. D. III. (2018). Economic and social political ideology and homophobia: The mediating role of binding and individualizing moral foundations. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 47, 1183–1194.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bliuc, A.-M., & Chidley, A. (2022). From cooperation to conflict: The role of collective narratives in shaping group behaviour. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 16, e12670.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feldman, G. (2021). Personal values and moral foundations: Examining relations and joint prediction of moral variables. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 12, 676–686.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goff, K., Silver, E., & Iceland, J. (2022). The resonance of repression: Moral intuitions, skepticism toward racial injustice, and public support for Trump's “law and order” response to the 2020 racial justice protests. Socius, 8, 23780231221110277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graham, J., Haidt, J., Koleva, S., Motyl, M., Iyer, R., Wojcik, S. P., & Ditto, P. H. (2013). Moral foundations theory: The pragmatic validity of moral pluralism. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 55–130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hundschell, A., Razinskas, S., Backmann, J., & Hoegl, M. (2022). The effects of diversity on creativity: A literature review and synthesis. Applied Psychology, 71, 1598–1634.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kapoor, H., & Kaufman, J. C. (2022). Unbound: The relationship between creativity, moral foundations, and dark personality. Journal of Creative Behavior, 56, 182–193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kapoor, H., Mahadeshwar, H., Rezaei, S., Reiter-Palmon, R., & Kaufman, J. C. (in press). The ties that bind: Low morals, high deception, and dark creativity. Creativity Research Journal.Google Scholar
Kashdan, T. B. (2022). The art of insubordination: How to dissent and defy effectively. Avery/Penguin.Google Scholar
Kashdan, T. B., Goodman, F. R., McKnight, P. E., Brown, B., & Rum, R. (2024). Purpose in life: A resolution on the definition, conceptual model, and optimal measurement. American Psychologist, 79, 838–853.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaufman, J. C. (2010). Using creativity to reduce ethnic bias in college admissions. Review of General Psychology, 14(3), 189–203.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaufman, J. C., & Glăveanu, V. P. (2022). Making the CASE for shadow creativity. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 16, 44–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lambert, N. M., Stillman, T. F., Hicks, J. A., Kamble, S., Baumeister, R. F., & Fincham, F. D. (2013). To belong is to matter: Sense of belonging enhances meaning in life. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 39, 1418–1427.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lasala Blanco, M. N., Shapiro, R. Y., & Wilke, J. (2021). The nature of partisan conflict in public opinion: Asymmetric or symmetric?. American Politics Research, 49, 46–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Livingstone, A., & Haslam, S. A. (2008). The importance of social identity content in a setting of chronic social conflict: Understanding intergroup relations in Northern Ireland. British Journal of Social Psychology, 47, 1–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luria, S. R., O'Brien, R. L., & Kaufman, J. C. (2016). Creativity in gifted identification: Increasing accuracy and diversity. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1377, 44–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marques, J. M., & Paez, D. (1994). The “black sheep effect”: Social categorization, rejection of ingroup deviates, and perception of group variability. European Review of Social Psychology, 5, 37–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nemeth, C. J. (1986). Differential contributions of field of study and minority influence. Psychological Review, 93, 23–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Dyne, L., & Saavedra, R. (1996). A naturalistic minority influence experiment: Effects on divergent thinking, conflict and originality in work-groups. British Journal of Social Psychology, 35, 151–167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Target article
“Our roots run deep”: Historical myths as culturally evolved technologies for coalitional recruitment
Related commentaries (25)
A terror management theory perspective on the appeal of historical myths
Adaptive lags, illusions and common interest
Beyond our “ancient roots”: Toward a broader understanding of the motivational power of societal meta-narratives
Collective selfhood as a psychically necessary illusion
Group myths can create shared understanding even if they don't act as superstimuli
Historical myths are believed because audiences are socially motivated
Historical myths as commitment devices
Historical myths define group boundaries: A mathematical sketch and evidence from Ukraine
Historical myths promote cooperation through affective states
Homo historicus: History as psychological science
Limited evidence that fitness interdependence produces historical origin myths
Myth as model: Group-level interpretive frameworks
Mythos in the light of evolution
Myths and fitness interdependence: Beyond coalitional longevity
Myths and prestige in Hindu nationalist politics
Myths of trauma and myths of cooperation: Diverse consequences of history for societal cohesion
Past glories feel good but creative minorities push us forward
The influence of stories including myths of origin
The social cognitive evolution of myths: Collective narratives of shared pasts as markers for coalitions' communicative and cooperative prowess
The social identity approach offers a more parsimonious and complete explanation of historical myths’ function and characteristics
The Trojan horse of historical myths: Emotion-driven narratives as a strategy for coalitional recruitment
Uncertainty reduction as an alternative explanation of historical myths
What about language?
Why some coalitions benefit from historical myths more than others
“We are one people”: Group myths also draw cues from self-concept formation
Author response
Coalitional psychology and the evolution of nationalistic cultures