Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-94fs2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-15T17:15:04.320Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The association between maternal postnatal depressive symptoms and offspring sleep problems in adolescence

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 October 2016

A. K. Taylor
Affiliation:
Centre for Child and Adolescent Health, School of Social and Community Medicine, University of Bristol, Oakfield House, Oakfield Grove, Bristol BS8 2BN, UK
E. Netsi
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Warneford Lane, Oxford OX3 7JX, UK
H. O'Mahen
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, College of Life and Environmental Sciences, University of Exeter, Exeter EX4 4QG, UK
A. Stein
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Warneford Lane, Oxford OX3 7JX, UK
J. Evans
Affiliation:
Centre for Child and Adolescent Health, School of Social and Community Medicine, University of Bristol, Oakfield House, Oakfield Grove, Bristol BS8 2BN, UK
R. M. Pearson*
Affiliation:
Centre for Child and Adolescent Health, School of Social and Community Medicine, University of Bristol, Oakfield House, Oakfield Grove, Bristol BS8 2BN, UK
*
*Address for correspondence: R. M. Pearson, Centre for Child and Adolescent Health, School of Social and Community Medicine, University of Bristol, Oakfield House, Oakfield Grove, Bristol BS8 2BN, UK. (Email: [email protected])
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Sleep problems are associated with increased risk of physical and mental illness. Identifying risk factors is an important method of reducing public health impact. We examined the association between maternal postnatal depression (PND) and offspring adolescent sleep problems.

Method

The sample was derived from Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) participants. A sample with complete data across all variables was used, with four outcome variables. A sensitivity analysis imputing for missing data was conducted (n = 9633).

Results

PND was associated with increased risk of sleep problems in offspring at ages 16 and 18 years. The most robust effects were sleep problems at 18 years [adjusted odds ratio (OR) for a 1 s.d. increase in PND, 1.26, 95% confidence interval (CI) 1.15–1.39, p < 0.001] and waking more often (adjusted OR 1.14, 95% CI 1.05–1.25, p = 0.003). This remained after controlling for confounding variables including antenatal depression and early sleep problems in infancy.

Conclusions

PND is associated with adolescent offspring sleep problems. Maternal interventions should consider the child's increased risk. Early sleep screening and interventions could be introduced within this group.

Type
Original Articles
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2016

Introduction

Sleep disturbances and disorders are very common and affect 10–56% of the global population (Stoller, Reference Stoller1994; Chilcott & Shapiro, Reference Chilcott and Shapiro1996; Partinen & Hublin, Reference Partinen, Hublin, Kryger, Roth and Dement2005; Leger et al. Reference Leger, Poursain, Neubauer and Uchiyama2008). In adults, experiencing lack of sleep or poor-quality sleep affects concentration, memory and daily functioning in the short and long term (Wolfson & Carskadon, Reference Wolfson and Carskadon1998; Gregory et al. Reference Gregory, Caspi, Moffitt and Poulton2009; Ram et al. Reference Ram, Seirawan, Kumar and Clark2010; Shochat et al. Reference Shochat, Cohen-Zion and Tzischinsky2014), and fatigue commonly occurs alongside insomnia (Hossain & Shapiro, Reference Hossain and Shapiro2002). Sleep problems also affect adolescent school performance as measured by grade point average (Wolfson & Carskadon, Reference Wolfson and Carskadon2003; Pagel et al. Reference Pagel, Forister and Kwiatkowki2007). Shorter sleep duration is associated with common illness, and increases the risk of high blood pressure and other cardiometabolic risk factors in adolescents (Azadbakht et al. Reference Azadbakht, Kelishadi, Khodarahmi, Qorbani, Heshmat, Motlagh, Taslimi and Ardalan2013; Panciencia et al. Reference Panciencia, Barros, Araujo and Ramos2013; Orzech et al. Reference Orzech, Acebo, Seifer, Barker and Carskadon2014). This may be linked with evidence showing that sleep-deprived adolescents make less-healthy food choices (Kruger et al. Reference Kruger, Reither, Peppard, Krueger and Hale2014). Poor sleep also affects mental health; reduced hours of sleep are associated with potentially severe mental health problems such as depression, anxiety and suicidal ideation (Settineri et al. Reference Settineri, Gitto, Conte, Fanara, Mallamace, Mento, Silvestri, Tati, Zoccali, Cordici, Grugno, Polimeni, Vitetta and Bramanti2012; Wong & Brower, Reference Wong and Brower2012; Sarchiapone et al. Reference Sarchiapone, Mandelli, Carli, Iosue, Wasserman, Hadlaczky, Hoven, Apter, Balazs, Bobes, Brunner, Corcoran, Cosman, Haring, Kaess, Keeley, Kereszteny, Kahn, Postuvan, Mars, Saiz, Varnik, Sisask and Wasserman2014). Poor sleep quality may co-occur with these disorders or may precede them and be a contributing factor (Wong & Brower, Reference Wong and Brower2012; Sarchiapone et al. Reference Sarchiapone, Mandelli, Carli, Iosue, Wasserman, Hadlaczky, Hoven, Apter, Balazs, Bobes, Brunner, Corcoran, Cosman, Haring, Kaess, Keeley, Kereszteny, Kahn, Postuvan, Mars, Saiz, Varnik, Sisask and Wasserman2014).

Both Australian and American studies have calculated a high economic burden of insomnia in terms of both direct and indirect costs (National Commission on Sleep Disorders Research, 1993; Hillman et al. Reference Hillman, Scott Murphy, Antic and Pezzullo2006). Direct costs included costs of healthcare, pharmaceuticals, diagnostic tests and research. Indirect costs included the cost of absenteeism and lost productivity.

Intervening before the development of sleep problems is an important method of reducing their public health impact. Risk factors may include anxiety or depression, low socio-economic status, bereavement, childhood abuse, irregular sleep schedules, cigarette smoking or drug and alcohol misuse (Shapiro et al. Reference Shapiro, MacFarlane and Hussain1994; Fleming & Shapiro, Reference Fleming, Shapiro and Shapiro1995; Shang et al. Reference Shang, Gau and Soong2006; Greenfield et al. Reference Greenfield, Lee, Friedman and Springer2011; Jarrin et al. Reference Jarrin, McGrath and Quon2014). Early life risk factors may present opportunities for early identification and intervention. Early intervention is important because not only are sleep difficulties a problem in their own right, but they may also lead to other problems that have a high public health impact.

Maternal depression is one potential early-life risk factor. It has already been established that maternal depression is associated with offspring sleep problems in infancy (O'Connor et al. Reference O'Connor, Caprariello, Blackmore, Gregory, Glover and Fleming2007) and the development of depression at age 18 years (Pearson et al. Reference Pearson, Evans, Kounali, Lewis, Heron, Ramchandani, O'Connor and Stein2013). However, no large cohort studies have addressed the possibility of maternal postnatal depression (PND) being a risk factor for adolescent sleep problems. There are several potential mechanisms for a causal association between early maternal depression and offspring sleep problems: shared genes may increase risk, antenatal depression may have a biological effect on the child while in utero, or PND may affect parenting and the development of set sleep/wake routines. However, any observed association may also reflect reverse causality (disturbed sleep patterns in the infants may contribute to depression in their mothers) or confounding by shared vulnerability factors such as depression in the offspring.

Therefore, in the current study we test the hypothesis that maternal postnatal depressive symptoms are associated with an elevated risk of sleep problems in adolescent offspring using Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) data. Investigating a potential link will add to existing findings that maternal depression can have lasting effects on offspring development. Bidirectional effects and reporting bias will be considered; these have been difficult to address in previous studies as they have relied on shorter-term follow-ups where timings of maternal depression and infant sleep problems are likely to co-occur and are based on maternal reporting only. The unique strength of this study is that sleep problems at 16 and 18 years old are reported by the child and occur so long after the PND that it is unlikely to be explained by reverse causality. In addition, infancy sleep problems are measured and can be accounted for.

Method

Data source

The sample was derived from participants from ALSPAC, which recruited 15 247 pregnant mothers in the early 1990s (Fraser et al. Reference Fraser, Macdonald-Wallis, Tilling, Boyd, Golding, Davey Smith, Henderson, Macleod, Molloy, Ness, Ring, Nelson and Lawlor2013) and has followed up the mothers and their offspring since. Ethical approval for the study was obtained from the ALSPAC Law and Ethics Committee and Local Research Ethics Committees, and participants gave informed consent. Further information is available on the ALSPAC study website, which hosts details of all available data through a searchable data dictionary (http://www.bris.ac.uk/alspac/researchers/data-access/data-dictionary/). The current study uses data from children completing questionnaires at ages 16 and 18 years. See Fraser et al. (Reference Fraser, Macdonald-Wallis, Tilling, Boyd, Golding, Davey Smith, Henderson, Macleod, Molloy, Ness, Ring, Nelson and Lawlor2013) for a full description of this sample. Please note that the study website contains details of all the data that is available through a fully searchable data dictionary (University of Bristol, 2015).

Sample

Our starting sample was mothers who completed postal questionnaires for PND (n = 10 317). A sample with complete data across the exposure, outcome, mediating and confounding variables was primarily used for both exposure variables, i.e. n = 2054 for the categorical depression of the first 2 years, and n = 2496 for a continuous score for PND. All analyses were repeated post-imputation for missing data in a sample with at least one measure of sleep in the offspring, which enabled prediction of missing sleep data (n = 9633).

Measures

Exposure

Symptoms of maternal depression were measured using the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS), which is a 10-item self-report questionnaire validated for use in and outside of the perinatal period (Cox et al. Reference Cox, Holden and Sagovsky1987). Postal questionnaires, including EPDS measures, were administered at approximately 8 weeks and 8 months postnatally, and also when the child reached 1.5 and 2.5 years of age.

EPDS scores greater than 12 have a high specificity and sensitivity in predicting clinically diagnosed depressive disorder. Scoring above 12 on the EPDS on at least two occasions suggests depression that is likely to require treatment (Cox et al. Reference Cox, Holden and Sagovsky1987).

Outcome

Four variables were used to indicate potential sleep problems. At age 16 years sleep outcomes were derived from a self-report sleep diary: sleep problems at age 16 years (yes/no), waking in the night at least once per night at age 16 years, and average time to get to sleep in minutes at age 16 years. At age 18 years sleep problems were measured as part of an interview assessing common mental health disorders (Clinical Interview Schedule-Revised, CIS-R) (Lewis et al. Reference Lewis, Pelosi, Araya and Dunn1992) and a binary variable was derived relating to reporting significant sleep problems according to International Classification of Diseases (ICD)-10 criteria or not. Such criteria relate to the duration, frequency and impact of sleep disturbances such as difficulties getting to sleep or waking too often. (See the online Appendix for the full questions.)

Confounding variables

The following variables are thought to be associated with both maternal depression and adolescent sleep problems and are important to explore as potential confounding variables or alternative explanations. We considered the following potential confounding variables to be important for the reasons described below:

  1. (1) Depression at age 16 years using the Development and Well-Being Assessment (DAWBA, for outcomes at age 16 years) and the CIS-R (outcomes at age 18 years). This was in order to account for concurrent depression to the sleep assessment, because sleep problems may simply be reflective of depressed mood rather than any independent effect of PND on sleep.

  2. (2) Sleep problems in infancy (6 months, 18 months and 2.5 years) as reported by the mother in order to account for reverse causality (where sleep problems contribute to maternal PND).

  3. (3) Socio-economic factors (maternal education, maternal age at birth, and parity), which may elevate risks of mental health and sleep problems in both mother and child.

  4. (4) Smoking in pregnancy and antenatal depression, in order to account for fetal programming.

  5. (5) We also later controlled for the number of subsequent maternal depressive episodes (number of times the mother is above threshold on the EPDS across 10 occasions up to age 12 years) in the post-imputation sample, in order to assess whether continued exposure to maternal depressive symptoms is important. We did this only in the imputed sample because this variable was only possible to derive in mothers who had completed all 10 EPDS measures and so contained substantial missing data.

Statistical analysis

Our exposure variable was maternal postnatal depressive symptoms. We conducted analysis using both a continuous score for PND (taken as the average EPDS score from the 8-week and 8-month assessments to give a more stable estimate of symptoms in the first postnatal year) and using a categorical variable indicating persistence of PND. We derived a three-level categorical variable: (1) those never reaching the threshold (non-depressed); (2) those above threshold on one occasion only (transient PND); and (3) those above threshold on the first postnatal EPDS and above threshold at least once in the following three questionnaires (recurrent PND). This is because each analysis provides us with slightly different information. Using the continuous score maximizes statistical power and examines the linear association with population symptoms, whereas the categorical variable allows us to examine clinically relevant groups. This approach led to multiple comparisons and therefore it is important to look at the overall patterns of findings and precise p values rather than taking p thresholds to determine ‘significant’ effects which would be likely to occur by chance with this many comparisons.

These exposure variables were then regressed on to each of the sleep outcomes described above in separate models. For binary outcomes we used logistic regression models and for continuous outcome measures (duration to get to sleep) we used linear regression models. Regression models were first conducted with just the outcome measures and then, in order to explore the role of different areas of confounding outlined above, we adjusted for these variables with additional covariates in a series of further models. This was important to highlight the importance of these different potential alternative explanations for any observed associations. For example, if the association was attenuated following adjustments for child depression it would suggest that the association is driven by child depression, and only children of depressed mothers who go on to have depression themselves also have sleep problems.

Imputation for missing data

We imputed for missing data because a complete case analytical approach can lead to biased results if the data are not missing completely at random (Royston & White, Reference Royston and White2011). For example, in this study the missing data are likely to follow a systematic pattern relevant to our hypothesis. Those mothers who are depressed are more likely to fail to complete surveys. In addition, offspring with mental health or sleep problems are also less likely to attend research assessments. Therefore, our analysis is likely to be biased towards an underestimate of any association between PND and offspring sleep problems (because we are missing those who have both a mother with PND and a child with sleep problems, i.e. the sample with a positive association between these two variables). However, we can correct for this bias by using available data to predict the values of those with missing data, and we are confident in our ability to build an adequate imputation model for these missing data due to the wealth of auxiliary measures that can be employed for this purpose. For example, given that there is substantial information on sociodemographic variables in ALSPAC that predict missingness, missing information can be assumed dependent on observed data (missing at random assumption). We employed a fully conditional specification as implemented in the multiple imputation (MI) chained algorithm in STATA 13 (StataCorp LP, USA) using all variables described in the analyses and additional sociodemographic indicators of missingness (list available on request) to predict missing data across 100 imputed datasets. Monte Carlo errors were less than 10% of the standard error and fraction of missing information (FMI) values were no larger than 0.8 (Royston & White, Reference Royston and White2011). The imputation method is based on regression equations to predict the missing variable. Therefore, the unique associations between each imputed variable and the predictor variables are used and every imputed variable is imputed using a unique set of regression equations. We imputed up to a sample with at least one measure of maternal depression and one infant sleep measure, giving a total of n = 9633.

Results

Sample

The mean maternal EPDS depression score postnatally was 5.5 (s.d. = 4.4, range 0–27). Of mothers, 10% exceeded the EPDS threshold at 8 weeks, and 6% exceeded the threshold on at least one more occasion in the next 2 years (see Pearson et al. Reference Pearson, Evans, Kounali, Lewis, Heron, Ramchandani, O'Connor and Stein2013 for further description of this sample in relation to PND). Frequencies of sleep problems in offspring of mothers with and without PND are displayed in Table 1. A relatively large proportion of offspring report problems with sleeping (approximately 15% at age 16 years and 22% at age 18 years), and over 40% of adolescents report waking in the night. The frequencies of all indices of problematic sleep were raised in the offspring of depressed mothers (see Table 1).

Table 1. Descriptive information (based on all available data)

CI, Confidence interval; PND, postnatal depression.

Main effects of maternal PND symptoms on offspring sleep problems

As shown in Table 2, the unadjusted models showed an association between PND and the following offspring sleep problems: taking longer to get to sleep, waking more than once in the night, and sleep problems at both 16 and 18 years. The majority of these findings remained following adjustments for adolescent depression at the time they reported sleep problems, and adjustments for sleep problems in infancy (reducing the likelihood of reverse causality), socio-economic factors and antenatal depression. However, the associations between PND and self-reported sleep problems at 16 years or time to get to sleep were less consistent across models, with little evidence for an association between PND and sleep problems at age 16 years once concurrent depression was accounted for.

Table 2. Logistic and linear regression models reporting associations between PND and sleep variables

Data are given as odds ratio (95% confidence interval) unless otherwise indicated.

PND, Postnatal depression; CIS-R, Clinical Interview Schedule-Revised; EPDS, Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale.

a Recurrent PND (EPDS score >12 postnatally and again in any following wave) compared with no depression in any of the waves; there were no differences when comparing maternal transient PND with no depression.

b Continuous postnatal depression taken as the average score across the 8-week and 8-month assessments = linear association; odds ratio or coefficient = risk for every five-point increase in EPDS average score.

After imputation for missing data and for the model including all adjustments for confounding variables (shown in Table 3), the associations between PND and offspring sleep problems remained similar, with continued evidence for associations between the two main outcomes found to be associated in complete case data: waking at age 16 years and sleep problems on the CIS-R at age 18 years.

Table 3. Results post-imputation

Data are given as odds ratio (95% confidence interval).

PND, Postnatal depression; CIS-R, Clinical Interview Schedule-Revised.

a See Table 2.

Role of later maternal depression

In a final step only possible in the imputed sample with imputed maternal depression at all time points, we were also able to include a final adjustment for the number of further maternal episodes of depression. There was evidence that the inclusion of this variable attenuated the associations between PND and sleep variables, with no evidence for associations with waking at 16 years once these variables are included [odds ratio (OR) 1.08, 95% confidence interval (CI) 0.94–1.23, p = 0.272] and weakened evidence for an association with sleep problems at age 18 years (OR 1.17, 95% CI 1.00–1.38, p = 0.050). This does not negate the original associations; rather it suggests that some of the association is likely to be explained by repeated exposure.

Discussion

Summary of main findings

There was evidence for an association between PND and increased risk of sleep problems across dimensions and at both ages of 16 and 18 years. The most robust effect was on risk of waking at night and sleep problems at age 18 years. There was some evidence that associations with the more subjective reports of sleep problems were partially explained by concurrent adolescent depression because adjusting for depression at the time of assessment diminished associations. However, there was evidence that the association between PND and waking in the night remained after controlling for later offspring depression, infant sleep problems (to account for reverse causality), socio-economic variables and antenatal depression. Of offspring of re-currently depressed mothers, 36% reported sleep problems at 18 years, compared with only 22% of offspring of non-depressed mothers.

Our findings are in line with evidence from a study of adolescent girls, reporting poorer sleep quality (measured subjectively) in girls whose mothers experienced at least two episodes of depression during their daughter's lifetime compared with controls (Chen et al. Reference Chen, Burley and Gotlib2012).

Mechanisms

The mechanisms to explain the associations are unclear from the current data alone; however, some speculation is possible. There are a number of mechanisms that could contribute to the association between maternal PND and offspring sleep problems. These include genetic vulnerabilities shared between mother and child (Goodman & Gotlib, Reference Goodman and Gotlib1999). These shared genetic factors which affect sleep/wake patterns would probably manifest during infancy and in part could explain this association. We have, however, attempted to address this by controlling for infant sleep problems, and there is still a clear association. During the first years of life the sleep/wake pattern will undergo substantial changes and, therefore, any shared genetic sleep vulnerabilities or influences that PND may have on the sleep/wake cycle may not become apparent until the child is an adolescent.

PND is known to affect parenting capacities and may interrupt the development of regulated sleep/wake patterns, first through compromising parenting consistency and sleeping routine and hygiene, which is needed to establish successful sleep patterns. Second, maternal cognitions and processing of information and any perceptions or expectations she may have relating to infant sleep may be affected. For example, parental cognitions about sleep are a known predictor of infant sleep problems particularly as they related to setting limits to parental night-time involvement (Tikotzky & Shaashua, Reference Tikotzky and Shaashua2012). These in turn could disrupt the infant's development of the ability to self-regulate and thus go back to sleep when they awaken. This may particularly influence sleep as the child develops, as well as the mother–child interaction (Pearson et al. Reference Pearson, Evans, Kounali, Lewis, Heron, Ramchandani, O'Connor and Stein2013). Future studies should investigate the role of potential mediating pathways including such parenting behaviours and cognitions around child sleep. Further mediating pathways could be the child's development of regulation capacities such as executive function or emotional functioning such as depression. Finally, home environment, including noise levels, number of people in the household and changing environments could also mediate associations between maternal depression and offspring sleep (Bartel et al. Reference Bartel, Gradisar and Williamson2015).

A potential explanation of the mechanism for the association between maternal PND and sleep problems may be the chronicity of the offspring's exposure to maternal depression. There was some evidence that that the number of maternal episodes of depression explained at least part of the association between maternal depression.

Bi-directional effects are also important as sleep problems in the early years can act as a major stressor to family life, cause maternal sleep loss and feelings of fatigue and contribute to maternal depressive symptoms. It is therefore possible that early sleep difficulties in the child and maternal depressive symptoms entrain a pattern of bidirectional effects predisposing the child for continued sleep problems and the mother at increased risk of relapses (Stein et al. Reference Stein, Pearson, Goodman, Rapa, Rahman, McCallum, Howard and Pariante2014).

Of particular importance may be the timing of maternal depression. There are two proposed mechanisms through which disrupted sleep can affect child development (Sadeh, Reference Sadeh2007). First, it may cause daytime sleepiness and reduce alertness. This, in turn, has the potential to disturb behavioural regulation and cognitive functioning. Early childhood is a period during which children learn to regulate their emotions and significant disruptions may affect this process. Second, disrupted rapid eye movement (REM) sleep may reduce the activities required for brain maturation, memory consolidation and learning. During the first 2 years of life the sleep/wake rhythm will undergo substantial developmental changes as a result of both brain maturation and development of the central nervous system. Any disruptions to these processes during this stage may have long-lasting consequences (Sadeh, Reference Sadeh2007).

Strengths and limitations

This is the first study to examine the association between maternal postnatal depressive symptoms and offspring sleep problems in adolescence using a large longitudinal cohort. Child-reported sleep at age 18 years was included, and we were able to adjust for sleep problems in infancy, making reverse causality unlikely. Results following imputation were broadly similar, indicating that the findings could not be explained by imputing for missing data.

However, clinical diagnoses of PND or sleep problems were not made formally by doctors, but inferred from the questionnaires that participants completed (the EPDS only allows screening for depressive symptoms but does not diagnose depressive disorders). Additionally there were no measures of maternal sleep problems that could be controlled for. Surrounding causes of waking in the night (such as physical illness, noise or irregular routine) were not known but would in the future provide important insights into potential pathways. Although there were some missing data, imputing for missing data has reduced the risk of bias. Finally, generalizability of the findings to culturally different populations or those with different sleep patterns may be difficult.

Implications

Maternal depressive symptoms are a risk factor for offspring sleep problems. Early identification of maternal depression and treatment (with, for example, antidepressants, cognitive–behavioural therapy or interpersonal therapy) of the mother's symptoms could potentially help prevent offspring problems (Howard et al. Reference Howard, Molyneaux, Dennis, Rochat, Stein and Milgrom2014). Interventions for mothers with depression should also consider the child's experience. For example, depressed mothers are increasingly offered support with parenting and it may be important for such approaches to include management of infant sleep. Looking at sleep patterns in the offspring of depressed mothers, potentially in sleep studies, is also important. For example, Symon et al. (Reference Symon, Bammann, Crichton, Lowings and Tucsok2012) describe an improvement in infant sleep and maternal wellbeing following an infant sleep intervention focusing on behavioural strategies. Early sleep interventions such as this could be introduced within offspring of depressed mothers, who appear to be a high-risk group according to our findings. Sleep studies may also help identify a specific sleep problem that offspring of depressed mothers may develop, which would guide more specific screening and intervention possibilities.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0033291716002427

Acknowledgements

We are extremely grateful to all the families who took part in this study, the midwives for their help in recruiting them, and the whole ALSPAC team, which includes interviewers, computer and laboratory technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers, managers, receptionists and nurses. The UK Medical Research Council and the Wellcome Trust (grant reference 102215/2/13/2) and the University of Bristol provide core support for ALSPAC. The current project was supported by an Elizabeth Blackwell Institute for Health Research Institutional Wellcome Strategic Award supporting an early career fellowship held by R.M.P. This article is the work of the authors, and R.M.P. will serve as guarantor for the contents of this article and the analyses of data.

A.K.T. and R.M.P. devised the study, conducted the analysis and led the analysis and writing. E.N., H.O.M., J.E. and A.S. contributed to analysis and writing.

Declaration of Interest

None.

References

Azadbakht, L, Kelishadi, R, Khodarahmi, M, Qorbani, M, Heshmat, R, Motlagh, ME, Taslimi, M, Ardalan, G (2013). The association of sleep duration and cardiometabolic risk factors in a national sample of children and adolescents: the CASPIAN III study. Nutrition 29, 11331141.Google Scholar
Bartel, KA, Gradisar, M, Williamson, P (2015). Protective and risk factors for adolescent sleep: a meta-analytic review. Sleep Medicine Reviews 21, 7285.Google Scholar
Chen, MC, Burley, HW, Gotlib, IH (2012). Reduced sleep quality in healthy girls at risk for depression. Journal of Sleep Research 21, 6872.Google Scholar
Chilcott, LA, Shapiro, CM (1996). The socioeconomic impact of insomnia. An overview. Pharmacoeconomics 10 (Suppl. 1), 114.Google Scholar
Cox, JL, Holden, JM, Sagovsky, R (1987). Detection of postnatal depression. Development of the 10-item Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale. British Journal of Psychiatry 150, 782786.Google Scholar
Fleming, ZA, Shapiro, CM (1995). Sleep function and insomnia assessment. In Sleep Solutions Manual (ed. Shapiro, CM), pp. 1432. Kommunicom Publications: St Laurent, Quebec, Canada.Google Scholar
Fraser, A, Macdonald-Wallis, C, Tilling, K, Boyd, A, Golding, J, Davey Smith, G, Henderson, J, Macleod, J, Molloy, L, Ness, A, Ring, S, Nelson, SM, Lawlor, DA (2013). Cohort profile: the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children: ALSPAC mothers cohort. International Journal of Epidemiology 42, 97110.Google Scholar
Goodman, SH, Gotlib, IH (1999). Risk for psychopathology in the children of depressed mothers: a developmental model for understanding mechanisms of transmission. Psychological Review 106, 458490.Google Scholar
Greenfield, EA, Lee, C, Friedman, EL, Springer, KW (2011). Childhood abuse as a risk factor for sleep problems in adulthood: evidence from a US national study. Annals of Behavioural Medicine 42, 245256.Google Scholar
Gregory, AM, Caspi, A, Moffitt, TE, Poulton, R (2009). Sleep problems in childhood predict neuropsychological functioning in adolescence. Pediatrics 123, 11711176.Google Scholar
Hillman, DR, Scott Murphy, A, Antic, R, Pezzullo, L (2006). The economic cost of sleep disorders. Sleep 29, 299305.Google Scholar
Hossain, JL, Shapiro, CM (2002). The prevalence, cost implications and management of sleep disorders: an overview. Sleep and Breathing 6, 85102.Google Scholar
Howard, LM, Molyneaux, E, Dennis, CL, Rochat, T, Stein, A, Milgrom, J (2014). Non-psychotic mental disorders in the perinatal period. Lancet 384, 17751788.Google Scholar
Jarrin, DC, McGrath, JJ, Quon, EC (2014). Objective and subjective socioeconomic gradients exist for sleep in children and adolescents. Health Psychology 33, 301305.Google Scholar
Kruger, AK, Reither, EN, Peppard, PE, Krueger, PM, Hale, L (2014). Do sleep-deprived adolescents make less-healthy food choices? British Journal of Nutrition 111, 18981904.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leger, D, Poursain, B, Neubauer, D, Uchiyama, M (2008). An international survey of sleeping problems in the general population. Current Medical Research and Opinion 24, 307317.Google Scholar
Lewis, G, Pelosi, AJ, Araya, R, Dunn, G (1992). Measuring psychiatric disorders in the community: a standardised assessment for use by lay interviewers. Psychological Medicine 22, 465486.Google Scholar
National Commission on Sleep Disorders Research (1993). Wake up America: A National Sleep Alert. Vol. 1. Executive summary and executive report. National Institutes of Health, US Government Printing Office: Washington, DC.Google Scholar
O'Connor, TG, Caprariello, P, Blackmore, ER, Gregory, AM, Glover, V, Fleming, P; ALSPAC Study Team (2007). Prenatal mood disturbance predicts sleep problems in infancy and toddlerhood. Early Human Development 83, 451458.Google Scholar
Orzech, KM, Acebo, C, Seifer, R, Barker, D, Carskadon, MA (2014). Sleep patterns are associated with common illness in adolescents. Journal of Sleep Research 23, 133142.Google Scholar
Pagel, JF, Forister, N, Kwiatkowki, C (2007). Adolescent sleep disturbance and school performance: the confounding variable of socioeconomics. Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine 3, 1923.Google ScholarPubMed
Panciencia, I, Barros, H, Araujo, J, Ramos, E (2013). Association between sleep duration and blood pressure in adolescents. Hypertension Research 36, 747752.Google Scholar
Partinen, M, Hublin, C (2005). Epidemiology of sleep disorders. In Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine (ed. Kryger, MH, Roth, T and Dement, WC), pp. 626647. Elsevier Saunders: Philadelphia, PA.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pearson, RM, Evans, J, Kounali, D, Lewis, G, Heron, J, Ramchandani, PG, O'Connor, TG, Stein, A (2013). Maternal depression during pregnancy and the postnatal period: risks and possible mechanisms for offspring depression at age 18 years. JAMA Psychiatry 70, 13121319.Google Scholar
Ram, S, Seirawan, H, Kumar, SKS, Clark, GT (2010). Prevalence and impact of sleep disorders in the United States. Sleep and Breathing 14, 6370.Google Scholar
Royston, IR, White, P (2011). Multiple imputation by chained equations (MICE): implementation in Stata. Journal of Statistical Software 45, 120. Google Scholar
Sadeh, A (2007). Consequences of sleep loss or sleep disruption in children. Sleep Medicine Clinics 2, 513520.Google Scholar
Sarchiapone, M, Mandelli, L, Carli, V, Iosue, M, Wasserman, C, Hadlaczky, G, Hoven, CW, Apter, A, Balazs, J, Bobes, J, Brunner, R, Corcoran, P, Cosman, D, Haring, C, Kaess, M, Keeley, H, Kereszteny, A, Kahn, JP, Postuvan, V, Mars, U, Saiz, PA, Varnik, P, Sisask, M, Wasserman, D (2014). Hours of sleep in adolescents and its association with anxiety, emotional concerns and suicidal ideation. Sleep Medicine 15, 248254.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Settineri, S, Gitto, L, Conte, F, Fanara, G, Mallamace, D, Mento, C, Silvestri, R, Tati, F, Zoccali, R, Cordici, F, Grugno, R, Polimeni, G, Vitetta, A, Bramanti, P (2012). Mood and sleep problems in adolescents and young adults: an econometric analysis. Journal of Mental Health Policy and Economics 15, 3341.Google Scholar
Shang, C, Gau, SS, Soong, W (2006). Association between childhood sleep problems and perinatal factors, parental mental distress and behavioural problems. Journal of Sleep Research 15, 6373.Google Scholar
Shapiro, CM, MacFarlane, JG, Hussain, MRG (1994). Conquering Insomnia, pp. 1981. Empowering Press: Hamilton, Ontario, Canada.Google Scholar
Shochat, T, Cohen-Zion, M, Tzischinsky, O (2014). Functional consequences of inadequate sleep in adolescents: a systematic review. Sleep Medicine Reviews 18, 7587.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stein, A, Pearson, RM, Goodman, SH, Rapa, E, Rahman, A, McCallum, M, Howard, LM, Pariante, CM (2014). The effects of perinatal mental disorders on the fetus and child. Lancet 384, 18001819.Google Scholar
Stoller, MK (1994). Economic effects of insomnia. Clinical Therapeutics 16, 873897.Google Scholar
Symon, B, Bammann, M, Crichton, G, Lowings, C, Tucsok, J (2012). Reducing postnatal depression, anxiety and stress using an infant sleep intervention. BMJ Open 2, e001662.Google Scholar
Tikotzky, L, Shaashua, L (2012). Infant sleep and early parental sleep-related cognitions predict sleep in pre-school children. Sleep Medicine 13, 185192.Google Scholar
University of Bristol (2015). Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. Accessing the Resource. Data Dictionary (http://www.bris.ac.uk/alspac/researchers/data-access/data-dictionary/). Accessed July 2015.Google Scholar
Wolfson, AR, Carskadon, MA (1998). Sleep schedules and daytime functioning in adolescents. Child Development 69, 875887.Google Scholar
Wolfson, AR, Carskadon, MA (2003). Understanding adolescents’ sleep patterns and school performance: a critical appraisal. Sleep Medicine Reviews 7, 491506.Google Scholar
Wong, MM, Brower, KJ (2012). The prospective relationship between sleep problems and suicidal behavior in the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health. Journal of Psychiatric Research 46, 953959.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptive information (based on all available data)

Figure 1

Table 2. Logistic and linear regression models reporting associations between PND and sleep variables

Figure 2

Table 3. Results post-imputation

Supplementary material: File

Taylor supplementary material

Appendix

Download Taylor supplementary material(File)
File 104.7 KB