Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jkksz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T06:26:20.991Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Basic symptoms in early psychotic and depressive disorders

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Frauke Schultze-Lutter*
Affiliation:
University of Cologne, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Early Recognition and Intervention Centre for Mental Crises (FETZ), Cologne, Germany
Stephan Ruhrmann
Affiliation:
University of Cologne, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Early Recognition and Intervention Centre for Mental Crises (FETZ), Cologne, Germany
Heinz Picker
Affiliation:
University of Cologne, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Early Recognition and Intervention Centre for Mental Crises (FETZ), Cologne, Germany
Heinrich Graf Von Reventlow
Affiliation:
University of Cologne, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Early Recognition and Intervention Centre for Mental Crises (FETZ), Cologne, Germany
Anke Brockhaus-Dumke
Affiliation:
University of Cologne, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Early Recognition and Intervention Centre for Mental Crises (FETZ), Cologne, Germany
Joachim Klosterkötter
Affiliation:
University of Cologne, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Cologne, Germany
*
Dr Frauke Schultze-Lutter, University of Cologne, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Early Recognition and Intervention Centre for Mental Crises (FETZ), 50924 Cologne, Germany, Tel: +49 221 478 6098; fax: +49 221 478 3624; email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Depression is a frequent condition in early psychosis. Therefore, early detection instruments should distinguish depression from beginning psychosis

Aims

To examine whether basic symptoms, i.e. subtle subjective deficits, differ between participants suffering from a potential prodrome (n = 146), first-episode schizophrenia (n= 153) and non-psychotic depression (n = 115)

Method

Basic symptoms were assessed with the Schizophrenia Proneness Instrument

Results

The prodrome and schizophrenia groups did not differ in level of basic symptoms but both had higher levels than the depression group. DSM – IV depression was frequent in those suffering from a potential prodrome (38%) and first-episode schizophrenia (21%). In both groups, participants with and without depression did not differ in basic symptoms. In multivariate analyses, consideration of current depression generally facilitated correct group classification, except for participants suffering from both a potential prodrome and depression

Conclusions

Cognitive basic symptoms distinguished well between all three groups. However, identification of persons suffering from a potential prodrome might be enhanced by considering current affective status

Type
Papers
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2007 

Clinically relevant depression is frequent in first-episode schizophrenia and its early, prodromal states (for example Reference Koreen, Samuel and SirisKoreen et al, 1993; Reference Rosen, Miller and D'AndreaRosen et al, 2006). Furthermore, depressive symptoms have been suggested to frequently mark the onset of the initial prodrome of psychosis (Reference Häfner, Löffler and MaurerHäfner et al, 1999). Thus, an instrument for the early detection of psychosis has to distinguish potentially prodromal individuals from individuals suffering primarily from a depressive disorder.

Basic symptoms, especially subtle, subjective disturbances of thought, speech and perception, as assessed with the Bonn Scale for the Assessment of Basic Symptoms (BSABS; Reference Gross, Huber and KlosterkötterGross et al, 1987; Reference Huber and GrossHuber & Gross, 1989), were shown to be predictive of later schizophrenia (Reference Klosterkötter, Hellmich and SteinmeyerKlosterkötter et al, 2001) and to distinguish between non-psychotic affective disorders and schizophrenia (Reference Klosterkötter, Ebel and Schultze-LutterKlosterkötter et al, 1996). However, a direct comparison between patients with non-psychotic unipolar depressive disorder, those with potential prodrome and those suffering from a first-episode schizophrenia has not been studied.

METHOD

Inclusion and exclusion criteria

The initial prodrome of psychosis was defined by the presence of at least any one cognitive–perceptive basic symptom found predictive for the development of schizophrenia in the Cologne Early Recognition study (Reference Klosterkötter, Hellmich and SteinmeyerKlosterkötter et al, 2001) with first occurrence at least 12 months earlier and multiple occurrences within 1 of the past 3 months, i.e. a minimum rating of ‘3’ on the Schizophrenia Proneness Instrument. These include thought interferences, perseveration, pressure or blockages; disturbances of receptive language, decreased ability to discriminate between ideas and perception or fantasy and true memories, unstable ideas of reference, derealisation; and visual or acoustic perception disturbances. Participants were between 16 and 40 years. Presence of any ultra-high risk criterion (Reference Phillips, Yung and McGorryPhillips et al, 2000) served neither as an intake nor as an exclusion criterion.

Inclusion criterion for the schizophrenia group was a first episode of schizophrenia according to DSM–IV criteria (American Psychiatric Association, 1994), and for the depression group an episode of major depressive disorder, dysthymic disorder or depressive disorder not otherwise specified according to DSM–IV without psychotic features for which help was sought for the first time.

General exclusion criteria were: (a) diagnosis of delirium, dementia, amnestic or other cognitive disorders, mental retardation, psychiatric disorders due to a somatic factor or related to psychotropic substances according to DSM–IV, (b) alcohol or drug abuse within the past 3 months according to DSM–IV and (c) diseases of the central nervous system (inflammatory, traumatic, epilepsy). In addition, current or past diagnosis of any psychotic disorder according to DSM–IV criteria served as an exclusion criterion in the prodromal and depression group. In the schizophrenia group, the latter was restricted to a past diagnosis of a psychotic disorder. An exception to this in the prodromal and schizophrenia group was psychosis not otherwise specified, when rated due to the presence of brief limited intermittent psychotic symptoms as defined by the ultra-high risk criteria (Reference Phillips, Yung and McGorryPhillips et al, 2000). The presence of attenuated or brief limited intermittent psychotic symptoms according to the ultra-high risk criteria was a further exclusion criterion in the depression group. In the matter of basic symptoms in the depression group, only the 10 cognitive–perceptive basic symptoms relevant for inclusion into the prodromal group were limited to not exceeding an occurrence of once in a month within the past 3 months, i.e. a maximum rating of ‘2’ on the Schizophrenia Proneness Instrument.

Participants

Of the 414 participants included in the study, 146 were suffering from a potential prodrome, 153 from first-episode schizophrenia (70% paranoid, 20% undifferentiated, 9% disorganised and 1% catatonic subtype) and 115 from depression (84% major depressive disorder, with 73% melancholic, 16% atypical and 11% no melancholic, atypical or catatonic features; 2% dysthymic disorders, equally with and without major depressive episode; and 14% depressive disorders not otherwise specified, with 69% recurrent brief depressive disorder and 31% minor depressive disorder). All gave written informed consent to the study between June 2000 and December 2005, with recruitment of participants suffering from a potential prodrome ending in December 2003. The majority of them had sought help at the Cologne Early Recognition and Intervention Centre for mental crisis (FETZ); others, especially those with first-episode schizophrenia, were in-patients of the Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy of the University of Cologne. In addition to the inclusion criteria, 80% of participants suffering from a potential prodrome reported attenuated psychotic symptoms, and only 1 person (0.7%) supplementary brief limited intermittent psychotic symptoms. Group differences for all socio-demographic characteristics are presented in Table 1.

Table 1 Characteristics of sample

Potential prodrome group (n=146) First-episode schizophrenia group (n=153) Unipolar depression group (n=115) P All groups1 (post-hoc)2
Age, mean (±s.d.) 24.4 (±5.2) 26.7 (±6.5) 27.6 (±7.7) 0.000
    median (range) 24 (16-39) 25 (17-43) 26 (17-52) (0.001/0.000/NS)
Gender, % male 69.2 74.5 47.0 0.000
(NS/0.000/0.000)
Partnership, %
    Single 61.4 77.1 62.7 0.031
    Married/steady partner 37.2 22.2 35.5 (0.013/NS/0.037)
    Separated 1.4 0.7 1.8
Graduation3, %
    None 2.8 3.3 0.9 <0.001
    CSE (10 years) 5.5 23.3 13.2 (0.000/NS/NS)
    O-level (10 years) 14.5 20.5 20.2
    VBD (12 years) 13.1 9.9 9.6
    A-level (13 years) 49.7 39.7 48.2
    Still in school 14.5 3.3 7.9
Vocational education, %
    None 13.1 29.4 15.9 0.001
    Apprenticeship or similar 15.2 28.1 27.4 (0.000/NS/0.036)
    Master craftsman or similar 1.4 2.0 4.4
    College of higher education 2.8 1.3 2.7
    University 9.7 4.6 11.5
    Still in school/training 57.9 34.6 38.1
Current occupation, %
    None 16.2 36.6 20.5 0.001
    Therapeutic/sheltered 1.3 0.9 (0.000/NS/0.036)
    Regular including education 83.1 59.9 75.9
    Other 0.7 1.3 2.7

Age was significantly correlated with graduation (Spearman's r S=0.316, P=0.000), vocational education (r S=0.456, P=0.000) and partnership (r S=0.232, P=0.000), which was also positively correlated with female gender (r S=0.397, P=0.000). Graduation and vocational education were weakly associated to each other (r S=0.197, P=0.000) and to current occupation (r S=0.208/0.188, each P=0.000).

Instruments

The subtle, self-experienced, self-reported deficits that often remain solely in the self-perception of the patient and do not show in behaviour, i.e. basic symptoms, were assessed with the Schizophrenia Proneness Instrument, Adult version (SPI–A; Reference Schultze-Lutter, Ruhrmann and PickerSchultze-Lutter et al, 2007). The 34-item SPI–A was derived from BSABS data on 79 individuals with a true prodrome and 346 with remitted schizophrenia, by cluster and confirmatory facet analyses for use by experienced and trained clinicians. Being a semi-structured interview, the SPI–A was designed to be applied together with the Structured Interview for Prodromal Syndromes (SIPS; Reference Miller, McGlashan and Lifshey RosenMiller et al, 2002), assessing attenuated and brief limited intermittent psychotic symptoms, and the Positive And Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS; Reference Kay, Fiszbein and OplerKay et al, 1987) to cover the whole range from early prodromal basic symptoms via attenuated and brief limited intermittent to frank psychotic symptoms. The SPI–A comprises 6 sub-scales of 5 to 6 items each:

  1. (1) affective-dynamic disturbances, including an impaired tolerance to certain stressors, a change in general mood and a decrease in emotional responsiveness in general as well as towards significant others or special events

  2. (2) cognitive–attentional impediments, including some of the less specific cognitive basic symptoms (Reference Klosterkötter, Hellmich and SteinmeyerKlosterkötter et al, 2001), i.e. an inability to divide attention between tasks relying on different senses, for example between talking and preparing a sandwich, feeling overly distracted by all kinds of stimuli, difficulties with short-term memory and concentration as well as slowed-down thinking and lack of purposive thoughts

  3. (3) cognitive disturbances comprising the more specific cognitive basic symptoms (Reference Klosterkötter, Hellmich and SteinmeyerKlosterkötter et al, 2001), i.e. an increased indecisiveness with regard to making minor decisions or choices, disturbances of immediate recall within seconds, interference of emotionally and otherwise insignificant thoughts, thought blockages and disturbances of receptive as well as expressive speech

  4. (4) disturbances in experiencing self and surroundings, including self-reported pressure of unrelated thoughts, unstable ideas of reference, a self-recognised disturbance in the visual perception of others, a decreased capacity to distinguish between different kinds of emotions and an increased emotional reactivity in response to routine social interactions

  5. (5) body perception disturbances comprising various kinds of coenesthetic phenomena

  6. (6) perception disturbances consisting of hypersensitivity to light/optic stimuli and/or to sounds/noise, photopsia, micro-/macropsia, changes in the perception of the intensity/quality of acoustic stimuli and somatopsychic bodily depersonalisation, all of these clearly distinct from psychotic experiences by being related to a malfunction of the individual's own senses or mind.

For the quantitative rating, a 7-point severity scale was introduced with maximum frequency of occurrence within the past 3 months as the guiding criterion; i.e. from ‘0’=‘symptom absent’ to ‘6’=‘present daily’. The overall concordance rate of the four SPI–A interviewers with an expert rating (F.S.-L.) was 91%.

The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM–IV (SKID–I, German version; Reference Wittchen, Wunderlich and GruschwitzWittchen et al, 1997) was conducted to rule out past or present psychotic disorders and confirm diagnoses in the schizophrenia and depression group as well as to assess comorbidity with affective disorders in the schizophrenia and potential prodrome group.

Data analysis

SPI–A sub-scale totals at baseline were compared between groups by two-tailed Mann–Whitney tests and adjusted for multiple testing across both, the pairwise group comparisons and sub-scales (18 pairwise tests in all), according to the sequential method for ordinal data by Holm (Reference Holm1979).

Despite the socio-demographic group differences, no covariance analyses were carried out, as nonparametric correlation analyses of the whole sample had only revealed weak, negligible associations between the socio-demographic and psychopathological data, these being highest for age and affective–dynamic disturbances (r S=0.142, P=0.004) followed by associations between age and perception disturbances (r S=0.108, P=0.028) and gender and affective–dynamic disturbances (r S=0.108, P=0.029).

For evaluation of the combined ability of the SPI–A sub-scales to discriminate participants with depression from those suffering from a potential prodrome as well as from first-episode schizophrenia and the influence of the main socio-demographic characteristics, age and gender, as well as the presence and kind of current depressive episode, stepwise logistic models (Wald-method, forward selection, without inclusion of a constant term) were used on the depression and potential prodrome group (a) with intake of current depressive disorder and (b) without its consideration. To assess the predictive accuracy of the derived model in an unbiased manner, data-sets of the depression and potential prodrome group were randomly split into a model generation sample (MG; n=132) and a model validation sample (MV; n=129) with participants with schizophrenia taken into account as a further validation group expected to be classified as ‘prodromal’.

To estimate the impact of a possible selection bias affecting the comparison between participants suffering from a potential prodrome and those with depression, additional subgroup comparisons were made. Such a selection bias might have been introduced by the inclusion criteria, of which about half of the items of cognitive disturbances (3 of 6 items), of disturbances in experiencing self and surroundings (3 of 5 items) and of perception disturbances (3 of 6 items) were – at a severity of at least ‘3’ – part of the inclusion criteria of the prodrome. To assume that such a selection bias had not influenced the results, the following conditions, which centre on the schizophrenia group as the group included independently from any prerequisite regarding basic symptoms had to be met. If participants with schizophrenia but without current depression do not differ from either participants suffering from a potential prodrome without or with depression, and both potential prodrome subgroups do not differ from each other, then it can be concluded that the performance on the SPI–A of participants suffering from a potential prodrome is independent from the presence of current depression and not solely due to inclusion criteria, in which case it would be expected to exceed that of participants with schizophrenia but no depression. Furthermore, if schizophrenia subgroups with and without current depression show equal SPI–A subscale totals and the schizophrenia subgroup with depression outperforms the depression group but not the prodrome subgroup with depression (which again is assumed not to differ from the prodrome subgroup without depression) do not, then higher sub-scale totals of those suffering from a potential prodrome and depression as well as participants suffering from a potential prodrome but no depression in comparison to participants with depression alone can be assumed to reflect not solely the inclusion criteria but also ‘true’ group differences. Comparisons of subgroups were carried out by two-tailed Mann–Whitney tests and adjusted for multiple testing across sub-scales (6 pairwise tests each), according to Holm's method (Reference HolmHolm, 1979).

RESULTS

Main group comparisons

As expected, participants suffering from a potential prodrome generally reported as many and as severe basic symptoms at all levels as participants with schizophrenia (Fig. 1); only with regard to affective–dynamic disturbances and disturbances in experiencing self and surroundings, did the participants with schizophrenia show even higher mean totals than participants suffering from a potential prodrome (20.3±7.6 vs. 17.6±6.8, P=0.006, and 12.3±6.3 v. 10.1±5.3, P=0.009). Compared to participants with depression, significantly higher mean totals in the potential prodrome and schizophrenia groups were found for all SPI–A sub-scales (Fig. 1).

Discrimination of depression group

Fifty-six participants suffering from a potential prodrome (38%) fulfilled criteria for a current depressive disorder: 34% of these had a current major depressive disorder, 11% a dysthymic disorder and 55% a depressive disorder not otherwise specified. Out of the major depressive disorders, 47% were of the melancholic, 16% of the atypical and 37% of the no melancholic, atypical or catatonic subtype. Out of the dysthymic disorders, 33% were accompanied by a major depressive episode. 71% of the depressive disorders not otherwise specified were recurrent brief depressive disorders and 29% minor depressive disorders. Participants suffering from a potential prodrome and depression and those suffering from a potential prodrome but no depression (n=90) did not differ in their socio-demographic characteristics, except slightly for partnership, with a tendency of those with additional depression to more frequently have a steady partner/spouse (50% v. 30%, 2×3 χ2-test, d.f.=2, P=0.046). Furthermore, the two subgroups did not differ in the percentage of persons with attenuated or brief limited intermittent psychotic symptoms.

Fig. 1 Comparison of group means of the SPI–A sub-scale totals between participants suffering from a potential prodrome, ; first-episode schizophrenia, ░ and non-psychotic depression patients, . Mann–Whitney Tests; P-values are adjusted for multiple testing across sub-scales and groups according to Holm's (Reference Holm1979) sequential method. *P<0.05; ** P<0.0005; ****P<0.000000001. All differences between participants with schizophrenia and those with depression are highly significant (P<0.0001).

Of the participants with first-episode schizophrenia, 32 (21%) fulfilled criteria of a depressive disorder not otherwise specified: 47% of a recurrent brief depressive disorder, 31% of a major depressive disorder in addition to a psychotic disorder and 22% of a post-psychotic depressive disorder. They showed no difference in mean age to those with no current depression. There was a higher proportion of female subjects in the schizophrenia with depression group (41% v. 22%, 2×2 χ2-test, d.f.=1, P=0.027). Despite no difference in graduation and vocational education, participants with both schizophrenia and depression more frequently had a regular occupation than those with solely schizophrenia (81% v. 54%, 2×4 χ2-test, d.f.=3, P=0.019). The greatest difference between participants with schizophrenia with and without depression was in the distribution of schizophrenia subtypes (2×4 χ2-test, d.f.=3, P=0.014): participants with schizophrenia and depression were more frequently of the undifferentiated (42% v. 14%) and less frequently of the paranoid subtype (52% v. 75%).

Considering all six SPI–A sub-scales, age, gender and current depressive episode (i.e. major depressive, dysthymic or other depressive episode), a model, reg1 (χ2=117.5, d.f.=4, P=0.000), was generated, which included cognitive disturbances (β=0.203, s.e=0.069, exp(β)=1.225 with 95% CI (1.070/1.403)), disturbances in experiencing self and surroundings (β=0.170, s.e.=0.082, exp(β)=1.186 with 95% CI (1.009/1.394)), current major depressive episode (β=–4.579, s.e.=0.782, exp(β)=0.010 with 95% CI (0.002/0.048)) and current episode of depression not otherwise specified (β=–2.003, s.e.=0.714, exp(β)=0.135 with 95% CI (0.003/0.547)), classifying 89.4% of the participants with depression and those suffering from a potential prodrome of the model generation (MG) group, 93.8% of the depression–potential prodrome model validation (MV) group and 96.1% of the participants with schizophrenia correctly (Table 2). In both, the potential prodrome and schizophrenia groups, misclassifications were due solely to subjects with current depression, thus leading to lower correct classification rates (76.8 and 81.3%) in these respective subgroups (Table 2).

Table 2 Classification results in groups of stepwise logistic regression equations generated with (reg1) or without (reg2) consideration of current depressive disorders

reg1 classification reg2 classification
Participants with: Depression n (%) Prodrome n (%) Depression n (%) Prodrome n (%)
Unipolar depression (MG, n=59) 53 (89.9) 6 (10.1) 48 (81.4) 11 (18.6)
Potential prodrome (MG; n=73) 8 (11.0) 65 (89.0) 15 (20.5) 58 (79.5)
Unipolar depression (MV, n=56) 53 (94.6) 3 (5.4) 47 (83.9) 9 (16.1)
Potential prodrome (MV; n=73) 5 (6.8) 68 (93.2) 12 (16.4) 61 (83.6)
Potential prodrome with depression (n=56) 13 (23.2) 43 (76.8) 10 (17.9) 46 (82.1)
First-episode schizophrenia (n=153)1 6 (3.9) 147 (96.1) 33 (21.6) 120 (78.4)
First-episode schizophrenia with depression (n=32)1 6 (18.8) 26 (81.3) 8 (25.0) 24 (75.0)

Taking current depression from the equation, a model, reg2 (χ2=84.9, d.f.=5, P=0.000), including five parameters was developed: cognitive–attentional impediments (β=–0.134, s.e.=0.057, exp(β)=0.875 with 95% CI (0.782/0.979)), cognitive disturbances (β=0.322, s.e.=0.075, exp(β)=1.381 with 95% CI (1.191/1.600)), disturbances in experiencing self and surroundings (β=0.233, s.e.=0.076, exp(β)=1.263 with 95% CI (1.089/1.465)), age (β=–0.069, s.e.=0.023, exp(β)=0.933 with 95% CI (0.893/0.975)) and gender (β=–1.411, s.e.=0.549, exp(β)=0.244 with 95% CI (0.083/0.715)), correctly classifying 80.3% of the MG sample, 83.7% of the MV sample and 78.4% of participants with schizophrenia as well as 82.1% of the potential prodrome subgroup with depression and 75.0% of the schizophrenia subgroup with depression (Table 2).

Group comparisons, evaluation of the impact of a selection bias

Participants with schizophrenia but no depression did not differ from those suffering from a potential prodrome and depression but did differ from those suffering from a potential prodrome but no depression on affective–dynamic disturbances (P adjusted=0.0009) and disturbances in experiencing self and surroundings (P adjusted=0.0033; Fig. 2). Thereby, participants with schizophrenia but no depression had higher mean totals than those suffering from a potential prodrome but no depression, which was mainly due to higher mean ratings on the 3 items of a reduced stress tolerance in affective-dynamic disturbances (all P adjusted<0.0001) and on unstable ideas of reference (P adjusted <0.00001) and an increased emotional reactivity in response to routine social interactions (P adjusted<0.05).

There was no difference either in the potential prodrome or in the schizophrenia group, between participants with current depressive disorder and those without on any SPI–A sub-scale (Fig. 2). No significant difference between participants with schizophrenia and depression and those suffering from a potential prodrome and depression showed, however, participants wtih schizophrenia and additional depression exhibited highly significant higher mean totals than participants with depression alone on all SPI–A sub-scales (all P adjust≤0.014; Fig. 2). Furthermore, participants suffering from a potential prodrome and depression as well as those not additionally suffering from depression had highly significant higher means on each SPI–A sub-scale (P adjusted≤0.0056), except on affective–dynamic disturbances that did not differ between participants suffering from a potential prodrome but not depression and those with depression alone (P adjusted=0.179). Here, only the impaired tolerance to certain everyday stressors was more severe in participants suffering from a potential prodrome but not depression (P adjusted<0.05); all the other four items did not differ between the prodrome subgroup without depression and the unipolar depression group.

Fig. 2 Group means of the SPI–A sub-scale totals in subgroups. , schizophrenia, no depression(n=121); ░, schizophrenia+depression (n=32); , potential prodrome, no depression (n=90);▪, potential prodrome+depression (n=56); , non-psychotic depression (n=115)

DISCUSSION

Depression is a frequent comorbid condition in psychosis, especially schizophrenia: depressive mood in the early state of schizophrenia was reported for up to 81% of individuals with first-episode, with clinically relevant depression in about 22% of those with first-episode schizophrenia (Reference Koreen, Samuel and SirisKoreen et al, 1993; Reference Häfner, Löffler and MaurerHäfner et al, 1999). Furthermore, depression according to DSM–IV criteria (i.e. major depressive, dysthymic or depressive disorder not otherwise specified) – at 28% point prevalence – occurred frequently in individuals at ultra-high risk of psychosis (Reference Rosen, Miller and D'AndreaRosen et al, 2006; Reference Simon, Dvorsky and BoeschSimon et al, 2006). In line with these findings, in our study, 38% of the participants fulfilling psychopathological research criteria of an early or late initial prodromal state (Reference Ruhrmann, Schultze-Lutter and KlosterkötterRuhrmann et al, 2003) and 21% of those with first-episode schizophrenia met criteria for a current depressive disorder according to DSM–IV. This emphasises the need to develop criteria for an early detection of psychosis that, among other properties, clearly discriminate between individuals with potential prodrome and those with depression.

Conceptualised originally as primary psychopathological disturbances underlying psychotic symptoms in schizophrenia (Reference Huber and GrossHuber & Gross, 1989; Reference Gross and HuberGross & Huber, 2005), basic symptoms have already been studied in affective disorders. These studies found that especially the cognitive–perceptive disturbances distinguish schizophrenia from non-psychotic affective disorder (Reference Klosterkötter, Ebel and Schultze-LutterKlosterkötter et al, 1996; Reference Bechdolf, Schultze-Lutter and KlosterkötterBechdolf et al, 2002) and possibly even psychotic bipolar illness (Reference Parnas, Handest and SaebyeParnas et al, 2003). Furthermore, basic symptoms, especially self-experienced cognitive disturbances, have been shown to be highly promising predictors of subsequent first-episode psychosis (Reference Klosterkötter, Hellmich and SteinmeyerKlosterkötter et al, 2001; Reference Lam, Hung and ChenLam et al, 2004; Reference Yung, PanYuen and McGorryYung et al, 2005) and have reportedly aggregated significantly in schizophrenia (Reference Parnas, Handest and SaebyeParnas et al, 2003). These cognitive disturbances, in addition, support a more sensitive and narrow definition of a homogeneous group of at-risk subjects compared to other approaches (Reference Simon, Dvorsky and BoeschSimon et al, 2006). We found comparable expressions of basic symptoms in participants with first-episode schizophrenia and those suffering from a potential prodrome for all sub-scales of the SPI–A, which were decidedly more severe than in non-psychotic depressive subjects, even as regards the more depressive-like complaints subsumed in affective–dynamic disturbances. This gives support to the notion that basic symptoms are indeed a phenomenology specific to the schizophrenia spectrum. This notion is further delineated by the lack of differences within the potential prodrome and schizophrenia groups whether or not they have a diagnosis of a current depressive episode according to DSM–IV. Although groups differed significantly in socio-demographic data, the association between socio-demographic data and basic symptoms was negligibly low in correlation analyses, thus not indicating presence of a socio-demographic sample bias.

As in previous studies (Reference Klosterkötter, Ebel and Schultze-LutterKlosterkötter et al, 1996; Reference Bechdolf, Schultze-Lutter and KlosterkötterBechdolf et al, 2002; Reference Parnas, Handest and SaebyeParnas et al, 2003) and at highly significant adjusted P-levels, the differences between the potential prodrome and schizophrenia, respectively, and the depression group were most pronounced in the SPI–A sub-scales cognitive disturbances and disturbances in experiencing self and surroundings. The fact that about half of the items of cognitive disturbances (3 of 6 items) and disturbances in experiencing self and surroundings (3 of 5 items) were (at a severity of at least ‘3’) part of the inclusion criteria of the prodrome is certainly prone to introduce a selection bias into the findings, which might lead to a tautology regarding the comparison of potential prodrome and non-psychotic depressive disorders. While the definition of the putatively prodromal as well as of the participants with nonpsychotic depression was at least partly dependent on SPI–A items, the schizophrenia subjects were a group included completely independently from their performance on the SPI–A. Thus, certain conditions focusing on the performance of these participants with schizophrenia were formulated and tested to estimate the amount to which the results might be influenced by the selection bias. Only three single comparisons did not follow the hypothesised directions: affective–dynamic disturbances and disturbances in self and surroundings were significantly more frequent in participants with schizophrenia but no depression than in those suffering from a potential prodrome but not depression, a result that gives even more support to the view that the high performance of those who are putatively prodromal is not only induced by the inclusion criterion for the prodrome than the hypothesised absence of a group difference. The third comparison that did not show the expected group difference between participants suffering from a nondepressive prodrome and participants with depression alone involved affective–dynamic disturbances was a sub-scale with none of its items being part of the prodromal inclusion criterion. Thus, with only one single comparison truly differing from the initial hypotheses, it seems fair to deduce that the selection bias had no major impact on the results. However, only follow-up of subjects will conclusively show whether the higher totals of cognitive disturbances and of disturbances in experiencing self and surroundings are truly associated with a conversion to psychosis in the comorbid potential prodrome group.

The prior notion of the importance of cognitive disturbances in the identification of persons symptomatically at an increased risk of psychosis is also supported by the results of the stepwise logistic regression analyses, which at a psychopathological level included mainly cognitive disturbances. This led to high rates of correct classifications of around 80% in the model validation groups including individuals with schizophrenia. When age and gender as the main socio-demographic variables accounting for group differences were considered along with current depressive disorders, classification rates improved by about a further 10%, thereby selecting current major depressive disorder and current depressive disorder not otherwise specified over age and gender. With a positive impact of psychopathology and a negative impact of current depression, the resulting equation led to a perfect classification of potential prodrome and schizophrenia subgroups without depression and to only few misclassifications of those participants with depression. Yet, the misclassification rates of prodrome and schizophrenia subgroups with current depression were around 20%. Future studies on larger samples and additional assessment of variables that are potentially relevant to depression, such as dysfunctional beliefs, will have to show if the introduction of a second-step classification algorithm especially generated to distinguish depressive subjects with regard to a potential prodrome of psychosis might significantly increase the number of right classifications in this group. Studies on other samples with different (comorbid) psychiatric conditions are needed to decide if such a two-step procedure might be reasonable for other potentially comorbid conditions such as anxiety disorders within the psychotic prodrome.

Thus, basic symptoms, and especially cognitive basic symptoms, seem to be a good tool to distinguish (pre-)psychotic from affective disorders. Furthermore, especially in those individuals suffering from a potential prodrome and a comorbid depressive disorder, a reliable early identification might require a second-step assessment to account for the comorbid condition.

Acknowledgements

The study was supported by a grant from the German Research Foundation (DFG; grant ID KL970/3–1 and KL970/3–2) to J.K., F.S.-L. and E.M. Steinmeyer.

Footnotes

Declaration of interest

None.

Funding detailed in Acknowledgements.

References

American Psychiatric Association (1994) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th edn) (DSM-IV). APA.Google Scholar
Bechdolf, A. Schultze-Lutter, F. & Klosterkötter, J. (2002) Self-experienced vulnerability, prodromal symptoms and coping strategies preceding schizophrenic and depressive relapses. European Psychiatry, 17, 110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gross, G. & Huber, G. (2005) Basic symptoms and prodromal phase of schizophrenia. Neurology, Psychiatry and Brain Research, 12, 185198.Google Scholar
Gross, G. Huber, G. Klosterkötter, J. et al (1987) Bonner Skala für die Beurteilung von Basissymptomen (BSABS; Bonn Scale for the Assessment of Basic Symptoms). Springer.Google Scholar
Häfner, H. Löffler, W. Maurer, K. et al (1999) Depression, negative symptoms, social stagnation and social decline in the early course of schizophrenia. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 100, 105118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holm, S. (1979) A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scandinavian Journal of Statistics, 6, 6570.Google Scholar
Huber, G. & Gross, G. (1989) The concept of basic symptoms in schizophrenic and schizoaffective psychoses. Recenti Progressi in Medicina, 80, 646652.Google ScholarPubMed
Kay, S. R. Fiszbein, A. & Opler, L. A. (1987) The Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 13, 261276.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klosterkötter, J. Ebel, H. Schultze-Lutter, F. et al (1996) Diagnostic validity of basic symptoms. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 246, 147154.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klosterkötter, J. Hellmich, M. Steinmeyer, E. M. et al (2001) Diagnosing schizophrenia in the initial prodromal phase. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58, 158164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koreen, A. R. Samuel, M. D. Siris, S. G. et al (1993) Depression in first-episode schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 16431648.Google ScholarPubMed
Lam, M. Hung, S. F. & Chen, E. Y. H. (2004) Transition to psychosis in Chinese high risk population. Schizophrenia Research, 70, 21.Google Scholar
Miller, T. J. McGlashan, T. H. Lifshey Rosen, J. et al (2002) Prospective diagnosis of the initial prodrome for schizophrenia based on the Structured Interview for Prodromal Syndromes: preliminary evidence of interrater reliability and predictive validity. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 863865.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parnas, J. Handest, P. Saebye, D. et al (2003) Anomalies of subjective experience in schizophrenia and psychotic bipolar disorder. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 108, 126133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Phillips, L. J. Yung, A. R. McGorry, P. D. (2000) Identification of young people at risk of psychosis: validation of Personal Assessment and Crisis Evaluation Clinic intake criteria. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 34 (suppl.), s164s169.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosen, J. L. Miller, T. J. D'Andrea, J. T. et al (2006) Comorbid diagnoses in patients meeting criteria for the schizophrenia prodrome. Schizophrenia Research, in press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruhrmann, S. Schultze-Lutter, F. & Klosterkötter, J. (2003) Early detection and intervention in the initial prodromal phase of schizophrenia. Pharmacopsychiatry, 36 (suppl.3), 162167.Google ScholarPubMed
Schultze-Lutter, F. Ruhrmann, S. Picker, H. et al (2007) The Schizophrenia Proneness Instrument (SPI–A) – a tool for the assessment of basic symptoms. European Psychiatry, 21 (suppl.1), s27.Google Scholar
Simon, A. Dvorsky, D. N. Boesch, J. et al (2006) Defining subjects at risk for psychosis: A comparison of two approaches. Schizophrenia Research, 81, 8390.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wittchen, H. U. Wunderlich, U. Gruschwitz, S. et al (1997) Strukturiertes Klinisches Interview für DSM-IV. Dt. Bearbeitung. Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Yung, A. R. PanYuen, H. McGorry, P. D. et al (2005) Mapping the onset of psychosis: the Comprehensive Asssessment of At-Risk Mental States Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 39, 964971.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Characteristics of sample

Figure 1

Fig. 1 Comparison of group means of the SPI–A sub-scale totals between participants suffering from a potential prodrome, ; first-episode schizophrenia, ░ and non-psychotic depression patients, . Mann–Whitney Tests; P-values are adjusted for multiple testing across sub-scales and groups according to Holm's (1979) sequential method. *P<0.05; ** P<0.0005; ****P<0.000000001. All differences between participants with schizophrenia and those with depression are highly significant (P<0.0001).

Figure 2

Table 2 Classification results in groups of stepwise logistic regression equations generated with (reg1) or without (reg2) consideration of current depressive disorders

Figure 3

Fig. 2 Group means of the SPI–A sub-scale totals in subgroups. , schizophrenia, no depression(n=121); ░, schizophrenia+depression (n=32); , potential prodrome, no depression (n=90);▪, potential prodrome+depression (n=56); , non-psychotic depression (n=115)

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.