Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7czq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T04:03:28.175Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Mate value, intrasexual competition and sociosexual desire drive Brazilian women's well-being

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 March 2021

Anthonieta Looman Mafra*
Affiliation:
Department of Experimental Psychology, University of São Paulo, Avenida Professor Mello de Morais, 1721 Butantã, São Paulo, SP 05508-030, Brazil
Renata Pereira Defelipe
Affiliation:
Department of Experimental Psychology, University of São Paulo, Avenida Professor Mello de Morais, 1721 Butantã, São Paulo, SP 05508-030, Brazil
Marco Antonio Correa Varella
Affiliation:
Department of Experimental Psychology, University of São Paulo, Avenida Professor Mello de Morais, 1721 Butantã, São Paulo, SP 05508-030, Brazil
John M. Townsend
Affiliation:
Department of Anthropology, Syracuse University, Syracuse, New York, USA
Jaroslava Varella Valentova
Affiliation:
Department of Experimental Psychology, University of São Paulo, Avenida Professor Mello de Morais, 1721 Butantã, São Paulo, SP 05508-030, Brazil
*
*Corresponding author. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Well-being (vs. ill-being) might function as an internal guide for approaching (vs. avoiding) situations, strategies, and achievements that ancestrally led to higher (vs. lower) reproductive success. Indeed, coupled individuals report higher well-being than singles, while depressive individuals report lower mate value and higher sociosexuality. Here we investigate associations between well-being, depression and evolutionary reproduction-related aspects (mate value, intrasexual competition, age, and sociosexuality). Overall, 1,173 predominantly heterosexual Brazilian women (mean = 31.89; standard deviation = 11.10) responded to online instruments measuring self-perceived happiness, life-satisfaction, depression, mate value, intrasexual competition, age, and sociosexuality. Multiple regression models indicated that higher well-being was positively predicted by mate value and negatively by intrasexual competition and sociosexual desire, while the opposite was true for depression. Although intrasexual competition and unrestricted sociosexuality can, under some circumstances, increase individual reproductive success, they are risky and suboptimally effective strategies, thus leading to feelings of ill-being. Contrarily, affective long-term bonds, higher mate-value, and lower intrasexual competition might increase feelings of well-being, because this would lead to a safer route towards ancestral reproductive advantages.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Evolutionary Human Sciences

Social media summary: Well-being rises with mate value and decreases with sociosexual desire and intrasexual competition in Brazilian women.

1. Introduction

Feelings of well-being facilitate individuals’ life goals and prompt them to explore new situations and interact with others (Kenrick & Krems, Reference Kenrick, Krems, Diener, Oishi and Tay2018). In contrast, depressive episodes lead individuals to remain quiet and isolated, preventing individuals from adverse social situations and from confronting higher status individuals (Watson & Andrews, Reference Watson and Andrews2002; Workman & Reader, Reference Workman and Reader2014). Such feelings of well- and ill-being might be triggered by external factors, including those concerning the reproductive domain (Troisi, Reference Troisi, Crawford and Krebs2008). When asked about the importance of evolutionary-oriented life goals (self-protection, disease avoidance, affiliation, status seeking, mate seeking, mate retention, parental investment) on their well-being, people indicated goals linked to long-term relationships (mate retention and parental investment) as the most important ones, while they ranked mate seeking as the least important (Ko et al., Reference Ko, Pick, Kwon, Barlev, Krems, Varnum and Kenrick2019). Indeed, adult individuals, on average, invest more in parenting than mating (Valentova et al., Reference Valentova, Junior, Štěrbová, Varella and Fisher2020), although, depending on specific conditions, both short- and long-term relationships can bring reproductive advantages (Buss & Schmitt, Reference Buss and Schmitt2019).

Humans can pursue a mixed mating strategy, shifting between long- and short-term strategies (Gangestad & Simpson, Reference Gangestad and Simpson2000), or they can invest in both mating and parenting at the same time (Valentova et al., Reference Valentova, Junior, Štěrbová, Varella and Fisher2020). By expanding the number of sexual partners, individuals can increase reproductive fitness by securing immediate resources and a genetic diversity of offspring that receive lower parental investment. Contrarily, by maintaining a mate with parental abilities to share child rearing, committed relationships can increase reproductive success by increasing paternal certainty and the survival rate of fewer offspring who receive higher parental investment (Troisi, Reference Troisi, Crawford and Krebs2008).

Short- and long-term relationships are partly influenced by trade-offs between characteristics that define one's value in the mating market and characteristics desired in a partner (Muggleton & Fincher, Reference Muggleton and Fincher2017). Individuals’ mate value and intrasexual competition are two reproductive variables supposed to secure a higher-quality mate, which might enhance one's reproductive success. Although these two variables may improve reproductive success, both work differently in the process of gaining and maintaining a partner. Individuals with higher mate value are mostly targets of desire and competition by potential partners; consequently, they tend to mate with similarly high-mate-value individuals (Kirsner et al., Reference Kirsner, Figueredo and Jacobs2003). People with lower mate value compete more than higher-mate-value individuals to acquire a romantic partner with higher mate value (Fisher & Fernández, Reference Fisher, Fernández and Fisher2017). However, lower-mate-value individuals are less likely to retain long-term relationships with higher value mates and tend to be less selective (Lee et al., Reference Lee, Loewenstein, Ariely, Hong and Young2008). Thus, intrasexual competition would have a more negative impact on individuals with lower mate value. By correctly assessing their own and their rivals’ mate value, individuals with lower mate value are less prone to engage in a competition with a rival who has a much higher mate value – thus avoiding wasted resources and time (Fisher & Fernández, Reference Fisher, Fernández and Fisher2017). Indeed, in men self-rated attractiveness correlates with other-rated attractiveness; however, this does not apply to women (Pereira et al., Reference Pereira, da Silva, Havlíček, Kleisner, Varella, Pavlovič and Valentova2019).

Age is another component that influences attractiveness, particularly in women. Because of their high obligatory initial physiological investment in the offspring and eventual menopause, women have a much shorter window of reproduction than do men (Mafra, Reference Mafra, Shackelford and Weekes-Shackelford2019). Age is thus a potent limiting factor of female fertility (Mafra, Reference Mafra, Shackelford and Weekes-Shackelford2017). The nubility hypothesis states that women with higher residual reproductive value would be preferred as reproductive partners (Marlowe, Reference Marlowe1998). Indeed, older women are rated as less attractive than men and women of various ages (Foos & Clark, Reference Foos and Clark2011; Maestripieri et al., Reference Maestripieri, Klimczuk, Traficonte and Wilson2014).

The adoption of specific mating strategies is flexible and depends on environmental demands, personal qualities and individual developmental pathways (Buss & Schmitt, Reference Buss and Schmitt2019; Gangestad & Simpson, Reference Gangestad and Simpson2000). For example, women, who are on average more sexually restricted than men (Schmitt, Reference Schmitt2005, Varella et al., Reference Varella, Valentova, Pereira and Bussab2014), may become more unrestricted in a society with more gender equality owing to both cultural and biological factors (Lippa, Reference Lippa2009; Schmitt, Reference Schmitt2005). More gender equal societies usually have more progressive social roles and ideologies, as well as better distributed resources, particularly towards women, and lower environmental stress. Consequently, biparental care is less necessary than in less egalitarian societies and unpredictable environments (Lippa, Reference Lippa2009; Schmitt, Reference Schmitt2005). As women's mating strategy shifts according to environmental demands, in an environment with lower stress women invest more in mating effort, short-term relationships, and consequently, possibly gain in offspring's genetic variation (Schmitt, Reference Schmitt2005). Similarly, women engage in more casual sex in societies with a higher proportion of women than men, which applies in particular to women with lower mate value (Millar et al., Reference Millar, Westfall and Walsh2019; Schmitt, Reference Schmitt2005). The less numerous sex becomes the limiting factor, being more valuable and obtaining more bargaining power within the local mating market. These advantages allow the less numerous sex to pursue its favoured strategy. In this case, rare males can avoid or minimise investment in one partner while seeking low-investment copulation with multiple partners (Lippa, Reference Lippa2009; Schmitt, Reference Schmitt2005).

Mixed results have been reported about association among sociosexuality, mate value, intrasexual competition and/or mental health. Some studies pointed out that higher sociosexual behaviour in women was associated with worry-vulnerability (Townsend & Wasserman, Reference Townsend and Wasserman2011), sexual victimisation (Townsend et al., Reference Townsend, Jonason and Wasserman2020), more depressive symptoms (Grello et al., Reference Grello, Welsh and Harper2006) and psychological distress (Fielder & Carey, Reference Fielder and Carey2010). Higher sociosexuality also decreases relationship quality (French et al., Reference French, Altgelt and Meltzer2019). However, the association between casual sex and indicators of well-being can also be positive in both sexes (e.g. Schmitt & Jonason, Reference Schmitt and Jonason2019) and can be moderated by several factors. Vrangalova and Ong (Reference Vrangalova and Ong2014) found that unrestricted men and women presented higher psychological well-being (higher self-esteem and life satisfaction; lower depression and anxiety) after uncommitted sex, while there was no such association among restricted individuals. Further, individuals with extrinsic motivation to casual sex presented lower well-being after casual hook-ups, while no such association appeared in individuals with intrinsic motivations towards casual sex (Vrangalova, Reference Vrangalova2015).

Worry/vulnerability and emotional upset do not necessarily result from women's casual sexual encounters, nor do they appear in the usual measures of well-being. Instead, women experience upset when there is a discrepancy between what they want/expect from a partner and what they are getting. Women who accept that hookups entail little or no investment do not necessarily experience upset. If they have regular sexual intercourse with a specific partner, however, they will tend to bond and then desire control over his level of commitment and investment (Townsend, Reference Townsend1987, Reference Townsend1998; Townsend et al., Reference Townsend, Kline and Wasserman1995). However, women who engage in multiple casual encounters for years tend to exhibit negative psychological symptoms (Furman & Collibee, Reference Furman and Collibee2014).

Previous studies mostly focused on casual sex or sociosexual behaviour. However, sociosexual behaviour can differ from sociosexual desires and attitudes. In particular, the behavioural dimension is an outcome of individual desires, combined with personal and external factors that limit the outcome realisation (Penke & Asendorpf, Reference Penke and Asendorpf2008). Sociosexual attitudes, on the other hand, are evaluations of sexuality, reflecting the sociocultural values of the given population. For example, a person can have a very high sexual desire (fantasies, arousal), a much lower frequency of sociosexual behaviour (e.g. because of unavailability of casual sexual partners) and very restricted attitudes (e.g. because the local religion values sexual chastity). A possible association between well-being and dimensions of sociosexuality other than behaviour (i.e. desire and attitudes) is yet to be tested. For example, Sociosexual Orientation Inventory (SOI) attitudes did not predict female sexual victimisation but SOI behaviour did (Townsend et al., Reference Townsend, Wasserman and Rosenthal2015).

Similarly, women with higher intrasexual competition and lower mate value presented several negative psychological symptoms such as disordered eating behaviours (Abed et al., Reference Abed, Mehta, Figueredo, Aldridge, Balson, Meyer and Palmer2012; Workman & Reader, Reference Workman and Reader2014) and higher depression scores (Kirsner et al., Reference Kirsner, Figueredo and Jacobs2003). It might thus be expected that higher intrasexual competition also decreases individual well-being. Contrarily, women with higher self-perceived physical attractiveness (i.e. one of the components of women's mate value) reported higher happiness (Gupta et al., Reference Gupta, Etcoff and Jaeger2016; Ko & Suh, Reference Ko and Suh2019). However, mate value is a broader concept than physical attractiveness, and its association with well-being is unknown.

Furthermore, depression increases with age in women between 15 and 55 years. Some authors report that this relationship becomes negative when women are over 55 years (usually after menopause), but there are others that report higher rates of depression in older women, even after menopause (for discussion see, Faravelli et al., Reference Faravelli, Scarpato, Castellini and Sauro2013). Some authors suggest that the age-dependent variation in depression can be explained through hormonal variation, pointing out that there is an increased risk for depression associated with changes in androgen and estrogen levels (Solomon & Herman, Reference Solomon and Herman2009; Oldehinkel & Bouma, Reference Oldehinkel and Bouma2011). Nevertheless, socio-cultural influences on women's behaviour during aging can play an important role as well (Oldehinkel & Bouma, Reference Oldehinkel and Bouma2011).

Our aim is to investigate which of the evolutionary reproduction-related variables (i.e., mate value, intrasexual competition, sociosexuality, relationship status and age) would predict women's mental health (i.e., well- and ill-being). We predict that mate value would positively predict well-being, whereas intrasexual competition and sociosexuality would negatively predict well-being. The opposite would be true for depression. The study is mostly exploratory because the above outlined literature did not jointly address the current independent variables in a single model.

Importantly, most previous studies on mating strategies and well-being were conducted in North American and West European populations, i.e. in Western, Educated, Industrialised, Rich and Democratic (WEIRD) societies, thus being far from representative of the human species (Apicella et al., Reference Apicella, Norenzayan and Henrich2020; Henrich et al., Reference Henrich, Heine and Norenzayan2010). Asian, African and Latin American populations are less frequently studied, despite being populous and highly genetically and culturally diverse (Rad et al., Reference Rad, Martingano and Ginges2018). Here we present data on a less-WEIRD population of Brazilian women, who are among the most anxious and depressive in the world (McPhillips, 2016), thus presenting an important opportunity to study women's well-being.

2. Methods

2.1. Participants

A total of 1,547 participants took part in the research. In the final analyses, we included 1,173 cisgender predominantly heterosexual women of 18 years or older (mean, M age = 31.89; standard deviation, SDage = 11.10). The majority of our sample were from southeast Brazil and self-identified as white (73.8%), pardo (or mixed-race, 17.9%), Black (3.6%), Asian (2.9%), Indigenous (0.6%) or belonging to other ethnicities (1.2%). Around one-third of the participants (34.3%) had completed graduate studies, 23.9% were undergraduate students, 21.4% had graduated, 9.9% had finished high school, 9% were graduate students and 1.6% had not completed elementary school. Regarding family income per month, most of the participants (27.4%) earned USD730–1,470 (R$2.811–5.622), 19.5% earned USD 1,470–2,200 (R$5,622–8,433), 14.9% earned above USD3,660 (above R$14,055), 14.4% earned USD 240–730 (R$937–2,811), 11.8% earned USD 2,200–2,930 (R$ 8,433–11,244), 7.9% earned USD2,930–3,660 (R$11,244–14,055) and 3.1% earned less than USD240 (R$937).

2.2. Instruments

The study was part of a larger project focused on makeup usage, well-being, and personality in women (Mafra et al., Reference Mafra, Shackelford and Weekes-Shackelford2019), and only questionnaires relevant for this study are presented below (all instruments are provided in Supplementary Material 1 in Brazilian Portuguese and English versions). For descriptive statistics of the studied variables, see Table 1.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics of the studied variables

Note: Differences in sample sizes are due to incomplete questionnaires. SOI, Sociosexual Orientation Inventory.

2.2.1. Sociodemographic questionnaire

The sociodemographic questionnaire included questions about age, sex and gender, sexual orientation, ethnicity, social status (family income and education) and relationship status. Relationship status was a dichotomous variable, with participants indicating if they were currently in a stable relationship. The majority of the participants were in a stable relationship (63.7%), while 36.3% of the women were single.

2.2.2. The Center of Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale

The Center of Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CESD-8) is a reduced version of the original Center of Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (Bracke et al., Reference Bracke, Levecque and Van de Velde2008) used to identify populations under risk of depression. This instrument is composed of eight items that measure, on four-point Likert scales, how often in the last week participants felt depressed, lonely, sad, happy or tired, enjoyed life, slept restlessly or felt that they could not continue. We used the version translated (translated/back-translated) into Brazilian Portuguese (Cronbach α= 0.845).

2.2.3. The Subjective Happiness Scale

The Subjective Happiness Scale (SHS) is a four-item measure that evaluates happiness on a seven-point responses scale (Lyubomirsky & Lepper, Reference Lyubomirsky and Lepper1999). We used the version translated and validated for Brazilian Portuguese by Damásio et al. (Reference Damásio, Zanon and Koller2014). An example of an item is: ‘In general, I consider myself’ with the options ranging from ‘(1) not a very happy person’ to ‘(7) a very happy person’ (Cronbach α = 0.826).

2.2.4. Satisfaction with Life Scale

The Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS) is a five-item instrument that measures the cognitive domain of life satisfaction on a seven-point response scale (Diener et al., Reference Diener, Emmons, Larsen and Griffin1985). We used the version translated and validated for Brazilian Portuguese by Giacomoni and Hutz (Reference Giacomoni and Hutz1997). An example of a item is: ‘In most ways, my life is close to my ideal’ (Cronbach α = 0.887).

2.2.5. Self-perceived Mate Value questionnaire

The Self-perceived Mate Value questionnaire includes four items that are answered on a seven-point scale (Edlund & Sagarin, Reference Edlund and Sagarin2014). An example of a question is: ‘Overall, how would you rate your level of desirability as a partner?’ We used the translated (translation/back-translation) version into Brazilian Portuguese (Cronbach α = 0.820).

2.2.6. Intrasexual Competition Scale

The Intrasexual Competition Scale includes 12 items that are answered on a seven-point scale; participants report how much the intrasexual competition tendency applies to them (1 = not applicable at all, 7 = very much applicable) (Buunk & Fisher, Reference Buunk and Fisher2009). An example item is: ‘I wouldn't hire a very attractive woman as a colleague’. We used the translated (translation/back-translation) version into Brazilian Portuguese (Cronbach α = 0.882).

2.2.7. Revised Sociosexual Orientation Inventory

The Revised Sociosexual Orientation Inventory (SOI-R) (Penke & Asendorpf, Reference Penke and Asendorpf2008) is a nine-item instrument that measures individuals’ tendency towards engaging in casual sexual variety without emotional investment. Higher scores indicate unrestricted sociosexual orientation. This scale was validated for Brazilian Portuguese by Nascimento et al. (Reference Nascimento, Hanel, Monteiro, Gouveia and Little2018); however, we used the translated version (translated/back-translated) into Brazilian Portuguese since this project started before the publication of the validation. The scale is computed as a global sociosexual orientation (Cronbach α = 0.828), and further divided into three subscales measuring sociosexual behaviour (Cronbach α = 0.785), attitude (Cronbach α = 0.783) and desire (Cronbach α = 0.728).

2.3. Procedure

The anonymous volunteers were recruited through social media (e.g., Facebook, Instagram) and institutional e-mails. The inclusion criteria were to be a Brazilian woman over 18 years old and to have access to a computer/tablet/cell phone with internet connection. First, the participants agreed with the consent term, then they responded to anonymous online questionnaires via Qualtrics (Qualtrics, Provo, UT, USA). Participants took around 45 min to complete the entire survey. The study was approved by the IRB of the Institute of Psychology, University of Sao Paulo (no. 90370517.1.0000.5561). The data were collected during 2018–2019, before the outbreak of the COVID-19 pandemic that disrupted much of normal routine and decreased well-being (e.g., Zacher & Rudolph, Reference Zacher and Rudolph2020).

2.4. Data analyses

From the 1,547 initial participants, we included 1,173 cisgender predominantly heterosexual women of 18 years or older (M age = 31.89; SDage = 11.10); thus transgender, predominately non-heterosexual (e.g., bisexual or homosexual) women and those younger than 18 years old did not enter into the final analyses. First, using IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, version 21 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA), we checked data normality. Most data were not normally distributed, and we thus conducted non-parametric correlations and categorical regressions. We ran non-parametric Kendall correlations because this analysis is more resistant to outliers (Croux & Dehon, Reference Croux and Dehon2010). To explore the data and verify correlations among the independent variables that implied the possibility of multicollinearity among independent variables, we correlated well-being measures (i.e., life satisfaction and happiness), ill-being (i.e., depression), age, intrasexual competition, mate value and sociosexuality (i.e., global sociosexuality, and sociosexual desire, behaviour and attitude). The independent variables are only weakly and moderately associated which discounted multicollinearity. This was further supported by the variance inflation factors that were not near the value of 10 and the average variance inflation factor was not greater than 1.

To investigate more directly whether reproduction evolution-relevant variables predicted women's well- or ill-being, we conducted categorical regressions because: (a) this method uses an optimal scaling feature that solves the problem of lack of linearity of the scales; and (b) it calculates an optimal regression equation and the effect of each independent variable (i.e., mate value, intrasexual competition, sociosexuality, relationship status and age) on the dependent variables (i.e., depression, life satisfaction and happiness). For these analyses, we used the three dimensions of sociosexuality instead of the global sociosexuality. Scale variables were treated as numerical. For all the analyses, we used a 95% confidence interval.

3. Results

3.1. Non-parametric (Kendall's) correlations among measures of well-being, sociosexuality, intrasexual competition, mate value and age

The results showed a moderately negative correlation among depression and well-being measures, and a strongly positive correlation between the two well-being measures (i.e., life satisfaction and happiness). Depression was negatively and weakly associated with mate value and age, and positively and weakly correlated with intrasexual competition, global sociosexuality and sociosexual desire. Happiness and life satisfaction were positively and moderately/weakly associated with mate value and age, and negatively and weakly correlated with intrasexual competition, global sociosexuality and sociosexual desire.

Age was positively and weakly correlated with sociosexual behaviour and negatively and weakly correlated with intrasexual competition. There was no correlation between intrasexual competition and mate value. Intrasexual competition was positively and weakly associated with global sociosexuality, sociosexual desire and sociosexual behaviour. Mate value was positively and weakly associated only with sociosexual behaviour.

For further details, see the whole correlation matrix in Table S1 (Supplementary Material).

3.2. Evolutionary predictors of women's well-being (categorical regressions)

To test if well-being would be positively predicted by mate value and negatively predicted by intrasexual competition and sociosexuality, and if depression would be positively predicted by intrasexual competition and sociosexuality and inversely predicted by mate value, we conducted categorical regressions.

The results indicated that depression was negatively predicted by mate value and age, and positively by intrasexual competition and sociosexual desire. In contrast, happiness was positively predicted by mate value, marginally positively predicted by relationship status and negatively by intrasexual competition and sociosexual desire. Life satisfaction was positively predicted by mate value (see Table 2). Thus, women with higher self-perceived mate value and lower intrasexual competition scored higher on well-being measures and lower on depression.

Table 2. Summary of the categorical regressions ran for ill- and well-being

Note: Statistically significant values are in bold (P ≤ 0.05).

3.3. Additional analyses

Because of the divergence among previous results on the relationship between age and well-being, we ran additional analysis to estimate better curve fit to our data. Age entered the curve estimation as an independent variable and depression, happiness and life satisfaction as dependent variables.

The cubic model explains depression the best (R 2 = 0.047; F (3, 835) = 13.757; P ≤ 0.001; constant = 37,771; b 1 = −1,547; b 2 = 0.039; b 3 = 0.000). It indicates that in this study depression decreased from 18 years of age until approximately age 25. Then it remained stable until around 50 years, when it fell again (see Figure S1 in Supplementary Material).

The inverse model fits best the relation between age and happiness (R 2 = 0.012; F (1, 1018) = 12.00; P = 0.001; constant = 20.519; b 1 = −55.137), suggesting that between 18 and 30 years of age happiness levels grow more sharply than after 30 years (Figure S2). Life satisfaction also increases with time, but rather linearly (R 2 = 0.007; F (1, 1074) = 7.548; P = 0.006; constant = 15.887; b 1 = 0.047; Figure S3).

4. Discussion

In this study, we investigated associations among well-being (i.e., happiness and life satisfaction), ill-being (i.e., depression) and evolutionary reproduction-related factors (i.e, mate value, intrasexual competition, sociosexuality and age) in a large sample of a less-WEIRD sample of Brazilian women. Overall, women who scored higher in mate value and lower in intrasexual competition and sociosexual desire reported higher well-being and lower depression; age affected depression negatively.

Women with higher sociosexual desire (but not attitudes or behaviour) had lower well-being. Sociosexual desire usually shows the biggest sex difference with men outscoring women (del Río et al., 2019). It is related to dissatisfaction with sexual life and is negatively related to relationship commitment (Barrada et al., Reference Barrada, Castro, Correa and Ruiz-Gómez2018). Furthermore, women who did not conform to traditional gender roles (i.e. masculine women) reported higher sociosexuality (Bartova et al., Reference Bártová, Štěrbová, Varella and Valentova2020).

Short-term mating has multiple costs, such as sexually transmitted infections and lack of parental investments. Hence, it is a viable route toward fitness, albeit a risky one. Although women can benefit from short-term mating in terms of resources or offspring genetic variability, they mostly cannot increase their reproductive success with higher numbers of sex partners as much as men, and one might thus expect their greater selectivity of long-term partners (Buss & Schmitt, Reference Buss and Schmitt2019). In general, women and men invest more into parenting than mating, indicatig that long-term relationships offer a safer route towards reproductie advantages (Valentova et al., Reference Valentova, Junior, Štěrbová, Varella and Fisher2020). Thus, sexual desire focused more on a long-term partner might, on average, improve personal satisfaction, whereas desire for uncommitted casual encounters might show the opposite trend. Indeed, although women reported not being concerned about their sexual partner's intentions and did not differ in number of sexual partners from men, they were still concerned about their partner's feelings for them and felt more vulnerable after having uncommitted sex (Hehman & Salmon, Reference Hehman and Salmon2020). These results suggest that, despite women's willingness to engage in uncommitted sexual relationships, they present psychological mechanisms that made them more sensitive when engaging in a sexual relationship with a partner who did not want to invest more in them (Townsend et al., Reference Townsend, Wasserman and Rosenthal2015).

Proximate social factors can also be at play. In particular, a double standard rewarding casual sex in men and discouraging such behaviour in women (Rudman et al., Reference Rudman, Glick, Marquardt and Fetterolf2017) might lead to lower well-being among women who have, for instance higher sex drive aimed at a variety of sexual partners or unsecure attachment style.

Intrasexual competition had a negative impact on well-being and increased with depression. Consonant with the current study, Gilbert et al. (Reference Gilbert, McEwan, Bellew, Mills and Gale2009) observed that in depressed individuals the need to avoid inferiority seems to be stronger, particularly when individuals evaluate themselves as belonging to a low social rank. Intrasexual competition in professional and social environments may trigger ill-being, including depression. In general, depression can be a reaction to one's fear of rejection when feeling subordinate (Gilbert et al., 2009; Langner et al., 2012).

Depression seems to be also linked to intrasexual competition for mates. Higher tendency/necessity to compete over partners might be, on the proximate level, accompanied by feelings of insufficiency, which decreases self-esteem and overall well-being. Moreover, whenever intrasexual competition is intense (e.g., in an environment with a higher proportion of women than men), lower-mate-value women are more prone to adopting an unrestricted mating strategy (Millar et al., 2019). For example, Kirsner et al. (Reference Kirsner, Figueredo and Jacobs2003) reported that participants with higher depression scores evaluated themselves as having lower mate value, and lower-mate-value women tend to accept a higher number of potential romantic partners, including ones with similar mate value (Lee et al., Reference Lee, Loewenstein, Ariely, Hong and Young2008). In contrast, higher-mate-value women tend to be more demanding when choosing their romantic partners (Buss & Shackelford, Reference Buss and Shackelford2008). Thus, we might assume that lower perceived mate value might increase the necessity to compete over partners, which decreases subjective well-being.

Indeed, women with lower self-reported mate value scored lower on well-being and higher on depression. Mate value is positively associated with self-esteem (Mafra & Lopes, Reference Mafra and Lopes2014), which, in turn, is also positively correlated with well-being (e.g., Dogan et al., 2013). Indeed, some researchers consider self-esteem to be a measure of psychological well-being (e.g., Vrangalova & Ong, Reference Vrangalova and Ong2014). Moreover, low-mate-value individuals tend to use more cost-inflicting mate retention behaviours while high-mate-value individuals tend to use more benefit-provisioning mate retention behaviours (Miner et al., Reference Miner, Starratt and Shackelford2009; Salkicevic et al., 2014). Cost-inflicting behaviours and low mate value are negatively correlated with relationship satisfaction whereas benefit-provisioning mate retention behaviours and high mate value are positively correlated with relationship satisfaction (Salkicevic et al., Reference Salkicevic, Stanic and Grabovac2014). Dush and Amato (Reference Dush and Amato2005) found positive correlations among life satisfaction, life happiness and relationship happiness, suggesting that the cost-inflicting behaviours used by low-mate-value partners decrease well-being. In fact, Hromatko et al. (Reference Hromatko, Bajoghli, Rebernjak, Joshaghani and Tadinac2015) found that the higher the mate value of an individual's partner, the higher the relationship satisfaction. Thus, women with higher mate value would attract high-mate-value partners more easily and would use more benefit-provisioning tactics to retain their partners, increasing well-being. These tactics diminish the necessity for intrasexual competition and orientation towards uncommitted sex, reducing ill-being and increasing well-being.

We found that relationship status marginally positively predicted happiness, which is in line with a previous study showing that the higher the relationship commitment is, the higher the subjective well-being level (i.e., married individuals reported the highest well-being scores; Dush & Amato, Reference Dush and Amato2005). Altogether, these findings suggest that a long-term committed sexual strategy may be related to higher well-being and lower ill-being. Indeed, long-term romantic bonds are universal and a prevailing human mating system (Fletcher et al., Reference Fletcher, Simpson, Campbell and Overall2015), and there is some evidence that relationship commitment increases reproductive success in naturally fertile populations (Sorokowski et al., Reference Sorokowski, Sorokowska, Butovskaya, Karwowski, Groyecka, Wojciszke and Pawłowski2017). Further, long-term mating orientation is positively correlated to mate value and slow life history (Strouts et al., Reference Strouts, Brase and Dillon2017) and couples with similar mate value tend to have a more lasting relationship and report higher relationship satisfaction (Salkicevic et al., Reference Salkicevic, Stanic and Grabovac2014).

In the same vein, women in our sample who were more concerned about mating (i.e., higher sociosexuality and intrasexual competition) presented lower scores of happiness and higher scores of depression. Accordingly, among individuals whose well-being was related to their meaning of life, kin-care motivation had higher importance than mate-seeking motivation (Ko et al., Reference Ko, Pick, Kwon, Barlev, Krems, Varnum and Kenrick2019). In comparison, in participants whose well-being was related to maximising pleasures in life, mate-seeking had the same importance as kin-care. That is, even among individuals who linked their well-being more to pleasures (i.e., high sociosexuality), mate-seeking (i.e., lower sociosexuality) was still not more important than kin-care (Ko et al., Reference Ko, Pick, Kwon, Barlev, Krems, Varnum and Kenrick2019). Additionally, individuals who reported high levels of kin-care motivation also reported higher life satisfaction and lower depression whereas the opposite (i.e., low life satisfaction and high depression indexes) was found in participants who presented high mate-seeking motivation (Ko et al., Reference Ko, Pick, Kwon, Barlev, Krems, Varnum and Kenrick2019).

Finally, age negatively predicted depression. We found that a non-linear (cubic) curve described the relationship between age and depression. Depression decreased between 18 and approximately 25 years, then remained stable until 50 years, when depression seemed to diminish again (Figure S1, Supplementary Material). These findings corroborated previous studies (Mirowsky & Ross, Reference Mirowsky and Ross1992). Indeed, according to the National Institute of Mental Health (2017), depression seems to be most common among adolescent US women (20%). In Brazil, between 2015 and 2018 the incidence of depression increased by 115% in the age group of 15–29 years, according to a survey by the Ministry of Health (G1, 2019).

5. Limitations and future directions

Despite having a large sample, we used a cross-sectional study design, which limits causal explanation. Future research could use experimental design, longitudinal study design or more specific questions about the circumstances that drive well- and ill-being. We did not have a sample large enough to analyse women older than 50 years to better investigate the relationship between age and well-being. Also, our sample relies on cisgender predominantly heterosexual women mainly from the state of Sao Paulo, thus restricting our results to a non-representative portion of the Brazilian population. Minorities are still targets of discrimination and show lower well-being (Perales, Reference Perales2016), poor self-esteem and high ill-being (Witcomb et al., 2019). Studies including non-heterosexual women and transgender women would improve our understanding of feminine reproduction-related aspects (Valentova & Varella, Reference Valentova and Varella2016) and provide more information to improve well-being of this part of the population. Also, our sample was composed of mostly self-declared white participants. Future studies would benefit from recruiting a more ethnically diverse sample or focusing on non-white populations that might be of higher risk of decreased well-being.

6. Conclusion

We showed that women's mate value and age are positively associated with well-being and negatively associated with ill-being, whereas intrasexual competition and sociosexual desire exhibited the opposite trend. Our results suggest that strategies which can increase women's reproductive success under some circumstances do not necessarily lead to feelings of well-being (for example, when women are competing with high-mate-value rivals). Thus, in women, subjective well-being might trace fitness enhancement mainly through the long-term relationship strategy rather than through mating efforts.

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/ehs.2021.18

Acknowledgement

We thank all of the participants who dedicated their time in collaborating with this research. We further thank to the anonymous researchers and the editors for valuable suggestions and feedback.

Author contribution

Anthonieta Looman Mafra, Renata Defelipe, and Jaroslava V. Valentova conceived and designed the study. Anthonieta Looman Mafra, Jaroslava V. Valentova and Marco Antonio C. Varella collected the data. Anthonieta Looman Mafra performed statistical analyses. Anthonieta Looman Mafra wrote the first draft of the article. Renata Defelipe and Jaroslava V. Valentova edited the manuscript. Marco Antonio Varella and John Townsend reviewed the manuscript. Jaroslava V. Valentova was the supervisor of the project.

Financial support

This work was supported by the São Paulo Research Foundation (Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa do Estado de São Paulo, FAPESP; grant number 2018/16370-5); the Coordination for the Improvement of Higher Education Personnel (Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior, CAPES); and the National Council for Scientific and Technological Development (Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico, CNPq).

Conflict of interest

Anthonieta Looman Mafra, Renata Pereira de Felipe, Marco Antonio Varella, John M. Townsend, and Jaroslava Varella Valentova declare no conflicts of interest.

Research transparency and reproducibility

The data is available at: DOI 10.17605/OSF.IO/PDSXZ

References

Abed, R., Mehta, S., Figueredo, A. J., Aldridge, S., Balson, H., Meyer, C., … Palmer, R. (2012). Eating disorders and intrasexual competition: Testing an evolutionary hypothesis among young women. The Scientific World Journal, Article ID 290813, 8 pages. doi:10.1100/2012/290813Google ScholarPubMed
Aghababaei, N., & Błachnio, A. (2015). Well-being and the Dark Triad. Personality and Individual Differences, 86, 365368. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2015.06.043CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Apicella, C., Norenzayan, A., & Henrich, J. (2020). Beyond WEIRD: A review of the last decade and a look ahead to the global laboratory of the future. Evolution and Human Behavior, 41(5), 319329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrada, J. R., Castro, Á., Correa, A. B., & Ruiz-Gómez, P. (2018). The tridimensional structure of sociosexuality: Spanish validation of the Revised Sociosexual Orientation Inventory. Journal of Sex & Marital Therapy, 44(2), 149158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bártová, K., Štěrbová, Z., Varella, M. A. C., & Valentova, J. V. (2020). Femininity in men and masculinity in women is positively related to sociosexuality. Personality and Individual Differences, 152, 109575. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2019.109575.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bracke, P., Levecque, K., Van de Velde, S. (2008). The psychometric properties of the CES-D 8 Depression Inventory and the estimation of cross-national differences in the true prevalence of depression. 3mc2008 – Proceedings. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/237379690_The_psychometric_properties_of_the_CES-D_8_depression_inventory_and_the_estimation_of_cross-national_differences_in_the_true_prevalence_of_depressionGoogle Scholar
Buss, D. M., & Schmitt, D. P. (2019). Mate preferences and their behavioral manifestations. Annual Review of Psychology, 70, 77110. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-010418-103408CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buss, D. M., & Shackelford, T. K. (2008). Attractive women want it all: Good genes, economic investment, parenting proclivities, and emotional commitment. Evolutionary Psychology, 6(1), 147470490800600116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buunk, A. P., & Fisher, M. (2009). Individual differences in intrasexual competition. Journal of Evolutionary Psychology, 7(1), 3748. doi:10.1556/JEP.7.2009.1.5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Croux, C. and Dehon, C. (2010). Influence functions of the Spearman and Kendall correlation measures. Statistical Methods and Applications, 19, 497515.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Damásio, F., Zanon, C., & Koller, S. (2014). Validation and psychometric properties of the Brazilian version of the Subjective Happiness Scale. Universitas Psychologica, 13(1). doi:10.11144/Javeriana.UPSY13-1.vppb.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
del Río, E. F., Ramos-Villagrasa, P. J., Castro, A., & Barrada, J. R. (2019). Sociosexuality and bright and dark personality: The prediction of behavior, attitude, and desire to engage in casual sex. International Journal Environmental Research and Public Health, 16, 2731. doi:10.3390/ijerph16152731CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diener, E. D., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49(1), 7175. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dogan, T., Totan, T., & Sapmaz, F. (2013). The role of self-esteem, psychological well-being, emotional self-efficacy, and affect balance on happiness: A path model. European Scientific Journal, 9, 3142. doi:10.19044/esj.2013.v9n20p%pGoogle Scholar
Dush, C. M. K., & Amato, P. R. (2005). Consequences of relationship status and quality for subjective well-being. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 22(5), 607627. doi: 10.1177/0265407505056438CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edlund, J. E., & Sagarin, B. J. (2014). The mate value scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 64, 7277. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2014.02.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Faravelli, C., Scarpato, M. A., Castellini, G., & Sauro, C. L. (2013). Gender differences in depression and anxiety: The role of age. Psychiatry Research, 210(3), 13011303. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2013.09.027CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fielder, R. L., & Carey, M. P. (2010). Predictors and consequences of sexual ‘hookups’ among college students: A short-term prospective study. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39(5), 11051119. doi:10.1007/s10508-008-9448-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fisher, M. L., & Fernández, A. M. (2017). The influence of women's mate value on intrasexual competition. In Fisher, M. L. (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of women and competition (pp. 281300). Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199376377.013.52Google Scholar
Fletcher, G. J., Simpson, J. A., Campbell, L., & Overall, N. C. (2015). Pair-bonding, romantic love, and evolution: The curious case of Homo sapiens. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(1), 2036. doi:10.1177/1745691614561683CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foos, P. W., & Clark, M. C. (2011). Adult age and gender differences in perceptions of facial attractiveness: Beauty is in the eye of the older beholder. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 172(2), 162175.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
French, J. E., Altgelt, E. E., & Meltzer, A. L. (2019). The implications of sociosexuality for marital satisfaction and dissolution. Psychological Science, 30(10), 14601472. doi:10.1177/0956797619868997CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Furman, W., & Collibee, C. (2014). A matter of timing: Developmental theories of romantic involvement and psychosocial adjustment. Development and Psychopathology, 26(401), 11491160. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579414000182CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
G1 (2019). Atendimentos do SUS a jovens com depressão crescem 115% em três anos [SUS care for young people with depression grows 115% in three years]. https://g1.globo.com/bemestar/depressao/noticia/2019/09/17/atendimentos-do-sus-a-jovens-com-depressao-crescem-115percent-em-tres-anos.ghtmlGoogle Scholar
Gangestad, S. W., & Simpson, J. A. (2000). The evolution of human mating: Trade-offs and strategic pluralism. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 573644. doi:10.1017/S0140525X0000337XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giacomoni, C. H., & Hutz, C. S. (1997). A mensuração do bem-estar subjetivo: Escala de afeto positivo e negativo e escala de satisfação de vida [Resumos]. In Sociedade Interamericana de Psicologia (Org.), Anais XXVI Congresso Interamericano de Psicologia (p. 313).Google Scholar
Gilbert, P., McEwan, K., Bellew, R., Mills, A., & Gale, C. (2009). The dark side of competition: How competitive behaviour and striving to avoid inferiority are linked to depression, anxiety, stress and self-harm. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 82, 123136. doi:10.1348/147608308X379806CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grello, C. M., Welsh, D. P., & Harper, M. S. (2006). No strings attached: The nature of casual sex in college students. Journal of Sex Research, 43(3), 255267. doi:10.1080/00224490609552324CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gupta, N. D., Etcoff, N. L., & Jaeger, M. M. (2016). Beauty in mind: The effects of physical attractiveness on psychological well-being and distress. Journal of Happiness Studies, 17, 13131325. doi:10.1007/s10902-015-9644-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hehman, J. A., & Salmon, C. A. (2020). Beyond sex differences: Predictors of negative emotions following casual sex. Evolutionary Psychological Science, 6, 97108. doi:10.1007/s40806-019-00217-wCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 6183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hromatko, I., Bajoghli, H., Rebernjak, B., Joshaghani, N., & Tadinac, M. (2015). Relationship satisfaction as a function of mate value. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences, 9(4), 242.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kenrick, D. T., & Krems, J. A. (2018). Well-being, self-actualization, and fundamental motives: An evolutionary perspective. In Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Tay, L. (Eds.), Handbook of well-being. DEF.Google Scholar
Kirsner, B. R., Figueredo, A. J., & Jacobs, W. J. (2003). Self, friends, and lovers: Structural relations among Beck Depression Inventory scores and perceived mate values. Journal of Affective Disorders, 75, 131148. doi:10.1016/s0165-0327(02)00048-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ko, A., Pick, C. M., Kwon, J. Y., Barlev, M., Krems, J. A., Varnum, M. E. W., … Kenrick, D. T. (2019). Family matters: Rethinking the psychology of human social motivation. Perspectives on Psychological Science. doi:10.1177/1745691619872986Google ScholarPubMed
Ko, A., & Suh, E. M. (2019). Does physical attractiveness buy happiness? Women's mating motivation and happiness. Motivation and Emotion, 43, 111. doi:10.1007/s11031-018-9717-zCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langner, C. A., Epel, E. S., Matthews, K. A., Moskowitz, J. T., & Adler, N. E. (2012). Social hierarchy and depression: the role of emotion suppression. The Journal of Psychology, 146(4), 417436. doi:10.1080/00223980.2011.652234CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, L., Loewenstein, G, Ariely, D, Hong, J., & Young, J. (2008). If i’m not hot, are you hot or not?— Physical attractiveness evaluations and dating preferences as a function of one’s own attractiveness. Psychological Science, 19, 669677. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02141.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lippa, R. A. (2009). Sex differences in sex drive, sociosexuality, and height across 53 nations: Testing evolutionary and social structural theories. Archives of Sexual behavior, 38(5), 631651.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lyubomirsky, S., & Lepper, H. S. (1999). A measure of subjective happiness: Preliminary reliability and construct validation. Social Indicators Research, 46(2), 137155. doi:10.1023/A:1006824100041CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maestripieri, D., Klimczuk, A., Traficonte, D., & Wilson, C. (2014). A greater decline in female facial attractiveness during middle age reflects women's loss of reproductive value. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mafra, A. L. (2017). Limited reproductive potential. In Shackelford, T. K., & Weekes-Shackelford, V. A. (eds.), Encyclopedia of evolutionary psychological science. Springer. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_243-1Google Scholar
Mafra, A. L. (2019). Female mate choice (intersexual selection). In Shackelford, T. K., & Weekes-Shackelford, V. A. (eds.), Encyclopedia of evolutionary psychological science. Springer doi:10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_3575-1Google Scholar
Mafra, A. L., & Lopes, F. L. (2014). ‘Am I good enough for you?’ Features related to self-perception and self-esteem of Brazilians from different socioeconomic status. Psychology, 5(7), 653663. doi:10.4236/psych.2014.57077CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marlowe, F. (1998). The nubility hypothesis. Human Nature, 9(3), 263271.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McPhillips, D. (2016). U.S. News. U.S. among most depressed countries in the world. https://www.usnews.com/news/best-countries/articles/2016-09-14/the-10-most-depressed-countriesGoogle Scholar
Millar, M. G., Westfall, R. S., & Walsh, M. (2019). The moderating effects of mate-value on the relationship between perceived sex ratio and mating strategies. Personality and Individual Differences, 145, 3943. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2019.03.019CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miner, E. J., Starratt, V. G., & Shackelford, T. K. (2009). It's not all about her: Men's mate value and mate retention. Personality and Individual Differences, 47, 214218. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.03.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mirowsky, J., & Ross, C. (1992). Age and depression. Journal of Health and Social behavior, 33(3), 187205. http://www.jstor.org/stable/2137349CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muggleton, N. K., & Fincher, C. L. (2017). Unrestricted sexuality promotes distinctive short- and long-term mate preferences in women. Personality and Individual Differences, 111, 169173. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2017.01.054CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nascimento, B. S., Hanel, P. P. H., Monteiro, R. P., V. Gouveia, V., & Little, A. C. (2018). Sociosexuality in Brazil: Validation of the SOI-R and its correlates with personality, self-perceived mate value, and ideal partner preferences. Personality and Individual Differences, 124, 98104. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2017.12.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
National Institute of Mental Health. (2017). Statistics: Major depression. https://www.nimh.nih.gov/health/statistics/major-depression.shtmlGoogle Scholar
Oldehinkel, A. J., & Bouma, E. M. C. (2011). Sensitivity to the depressogenic effect of stress and HPAaxis reactivity in adolescence: A review of gender differences. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(8), 17571770. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.10.013.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Penke, L., & Asendorpf, J. B. (2008). Beyond global sociosexual orientations: A more differentiated look at sociosexuality and its effects on courtship and romantic relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(5), 1113. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.95.5.1113CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perales, F. (2016). The costs of being ‘different’: Sexual identity and subjective wellbeing over the life course. Social Indicators Research, 127, 827849. doi:10.1007/s11205-015-0974-xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pereira, K. J., da Silva, C. S. A., Havlíček, J., Kleisner, K., Varella, M. A. C., Pavlovič, O., … Valentova, J. V. (2019). Femininity–masculinity and attractiveness – Associations between self-ratings, third-party ratings and objective measures. Personality and Individual Differences, 147, 166171. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2019.04.033CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rad, M. S., Martingano, A. J., & Ginges, J. (2018). Toward a psychology of Homo sapiens: Making psychological science more representative of the human population. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(45), 1140111405. doi:10.1073/pnas.1721165115CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rudman, L. A., Glick, P., Marquardt, T., & Fetterolf, J. C. (2017). When women are urged to have casual sex more than men are: Perceived risk moderates the sexual advice double standard. Sex Roles, 77(5–6), 409418.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salkicevic, S., Stanic, A. L., & Grabovac, M. T. (2014). Good mates retain us right: Investigating the relationship between mate retention strategies, mate value, and relationship satisfaction. Evolutionary Psychology, 12(5), 10381052.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmitt, D. P. (2005). Sociosexuality from Argentina to Zimbabwe: A 48-nation study of sex, culture, and strategies of human mating. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(2), 247. doi:10.1017/S0140525X05000051CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmitt, D. P., & Jonason, P. K. (2019). Self-esteem as an adaptive sociometer of mating success: Evaluating evidence of sex-specific psychological design across 10 world regions. Personality and Individual Differences, 143, 1320. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2019.02.011CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Solomon, M. B., & Herman, J. P. (2009). Sex differences in psychopathology: Of gonads, adrenals and mental illness. Physiology and Behavior, 97(2), 250258. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.02.033CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sorokowski, P., Sorokowska, A., Butovskaya, M., Karwowski, M., Groyecka, A., Wojciszke, B., … Pawłowski, B. (2017). Love influences reproductive success in humans. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1922. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01922CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strouts, P. H., Brase, G. L., & Dillon, H. M. (2017). Personality and evolutionary strategies: The relationships between HEXACO traits, mate value, life history strategy, and sociosexuality. Personality and Individual Differences, 115, 128132. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2016.03.047CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Townsend, J. M. (1987). Sex differences in sexuality among medical students: Effects of increasing socioeconomic status. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 16, 427446. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF01541424CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Townsend, J. M. (1998). What women want – what men want. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Townsend, J. M., Jonason, P. K., & Wasserman, T. H. (2020). Associations between motives for casual sex, depression, self-esteem, and sexual victimization. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 49(4), 11891197. doi:10.1007/s10508-019-01482-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Townsend, J. M., Kline, J., & Wasserman, T. (1995). Low-investment copulation: Sex differences in motivations and emotional reactions. Ethology and Sociobiology, 16, 2551. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0162-3095(94)00027-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Townsend, J. M., & Wasserman, T. H. (2011). Sexual hookups among college students: Sex differences in emotional reactions. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 40(6), 11731181. doi:10.1007/s10508-011-9841-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Townsend, J. M., Wasserman, T. H., & Rosenthal, A. (2015). Gender difference in emotional reactions and sexual coercion in casual sexual relations: An evolutionary perspective. Personality and Individual Differences, 85, 4149. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2015.04.031CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Troisi, A. (2008). Psychopathology and mental illness. In Crawford, C. & Krebs, D. (Eds.), Foundations of Evolutionary Psychology (pp. 453474).Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
Valentova, J. V., Junior, F. P. M., Štěrbová, Z., Varella, M. A. C., & Fisher, M. L. (2020). The association between Dark Triad traits and sociosexuality with mating and parenting efforts: A cross-cultural study. Personality and Individual Differences, 154, 109613.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valentova, J. V., & Varella, M. A. C. (2016). Further steps towards a truly integrative theory of sexuality. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 45, 517520. doi:10.1007/s10508-015-0630-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Varella, M. A. C., Valentova, J. V., Pereira, K. J., & Bussab, V. S. R. (2014). Promiscuity is related to masculine and feminine body traits in both men and women: Evidence from Brazilian and Czech samples. Behavioural Processes, 109, 3439. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2014.07.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vrangalova, Z. (2015). Does casual sex harm college students’ well-being? A longitudinal investigation of the role of motivation. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 44(4), 945959.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vrangalova, Z., & Ong, A. D. (2014). Who benefits from casual sex? The moderating role of sociosexuality. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 5(8), 883891. doi:10.1177/1948550614537308CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, P. J., & Andrews, P. W. (2002). Toward a revised evolutionary adaptationist analysis of depression: The social navigation hypothesis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 72(1), 114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Witcomb, G. L., Claes, L., Bouman, W. P., Nixon, E., Motmans, J., & Arcelus, J. (2019). Experiences and psychological wellbeing outcomes associated with bullying in treatment-seeking transgender and gender-diverse youth. LGBT Health, 6(5), 216226. doi:10.1089/lgbt.2018.0179CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Workman, L.. & Reader, W. (2014). Evolutionary psychopathology and Darwinian medicine. Evolutionary psychology: An introduction (3rd ed., pp. 358–397). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zacher, H., & Rudolph, C. W. (2020). Individual differences and changes in subjective wellbeing during the early stages of the COVID-19 pandemic. American Psychologist. Advance online publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/amp0000702Google ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptive statistics of the studied variables

Figure 1

Table 2. Summary of the categorical regressions ran for ill- and well-being

Supplementary material: File

Mafra et al. supplementary material

Mafra et al. supplementary material 1
Download Mafra et al. supplementary material(File)
File 55.1 KB
Supplementary material: File

Mafra et al. supplementary material

Mafra et al. supplementary material 2

Download Mafra et al. supplementary material(File)
File 265.8 KB