Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7czq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-27T13:34:18.193Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Time after time: Factors influencing children’s comprehension of Before and After

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 November 2023

Laura WAGNER*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University, USA
Rachael Frush HOLT
Affiliation:
Department of Speech & Hearing Science, The Ohio State University, USA
*
Corresponding author: Laura Wagner; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

We investigated older children’s (7–12 years) ability to comprehend before and after sentences. Results found that three factors that influence pre-school aged children’s learning of these words continues to influence older children’s comprehension. Specifically, children’s accuracy is improved when the events can be naturally (vs. arbitrarily) ordered; when the clauses in the sentence iconically match (vs. mismatch) the order of the events in the world; and when sentences use before (vs. after). The first two factors are argued to directly facilitate the building of mental models while the last one does so indirectly because of patterns of input usage.

Type
Brief Research Report
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

By age 5 years, children can act-out a pair of events in the correct sequence, guided by the words before and after (E. V. Clark, Reference Clark1971). Nevertheless, even 12-year-olds have persistent difficulties correctly interpreting such sentences (Pyykkönen & Järvikivi, Reference Pyykkönen and Järvikivi2012). This study examines if there is continuity in this domain by investigating if the factors that influence young children’s interpretation of temporal connectives persist in influencing the performance of older children.

Previous investigations of children’s comprehension of before and after have argued for three distinct elements influencing children’s learning of these terms (Blything & Cain, Reference Blything and Cain2016; Blything et al., Reference Blything, Davies and Cain2015; E. V. Clark, Reference Clark1971; DeRuiter et al., Reference DeRuiter, Theakston, Brandt and Lieven2018; Pyykkönen & Järvikivi, Reference Pyykkönen and Järvikivi2012; Pyykkönen et al., Reference Pyykkönen, Niemi and Järvikivi2003). The first is the real-world connection between the described events. Some events are naturally ordered in the world: brushing your teeth comes before going to bed; eating dessert comes after eating vegetables. Other events can be reasonably sequenced in either order, such as going to the park and going to the zoo. Early studies (e.g., French & Brown, Reference French and Brown1977) found that preschool-aged children could only successfully interpret before and after with naturally-ordered events. However, in principle, children do not need to understand the temporal connectives at all to correctly sequence naturally-ordered event pairs, and many researchers have simply avoided them for that reason. More recently, Blything et al. (Reference Blything, Davies and Cain2015) tested 3- to 7-year-olds using both naturally- and arbitrarily-ordered events and found that children successfully interpreted the temporal sequences of before and after with both event types. However, Pyykkönen et al. (Reference Pyykkönen, Niemi and Järvikivi2003) found that event naturalness helped 8- to 12-year-olds in what appeared to be a more difficult test of temporal connectives.

A second element influencing both acquisition and processing is the iconicity of the ordering between the sentential clauses and the events themselves. For example, in the sentence, The dog went upstairs before the cat came home, the dog’s action not only precedes the cat’s action in the world, but the clause describing it precedes it in the sentence: there is an iconic mapping between the ordering in the world and the linguistic description. Non-iconic sentences have a mismatch between the word order and the ordering in the world (e.g., Before the cat came home, the dog went upstairs). Iconicity is a persistent factor in children’s interpretation of before and after, with higher accuracy rates found for iconic ordering among children starting in preschool (E. V. Clark, Reference Clark1971; DeRuiter et al., Reference DeRuiter, Theakston, Brandt and Lieven2018) and extending to 7-year-old (Blything & Cain, Reference Blything and Cain2016; Blything et al., Reference Blything, Davies and Cain2015) and even 12-year-old children (Pyykkönen & Järvikivi, Reference Pyykkönen and Järvikivi2012; Pyykkönen et al., Reference Pyykkönen, Niemi and Järvikivi2003). The dominant explanation for why iconic orders are easier is that they facilitate building a mental model of the events (cf. Gernsbacher, Reference Gernsbacher1990; Zwaan & Radvansky, Reference Zwaan and Radvansky1998). Children begin to build models as they listen to the sentence, and the iconic match between what they hear first and what they should order first helps them create a stable mental model. Moreover, iconicity is not only favored by children: iconic orders for before/after sentences were more frequent than non-iconic orders in an adult corpus study (Diessel, Reference Diessel2008), and differential ERP patterns were observed in adults for iconic and non-iconic orders (Münte et al., Reference Münte, Schiltz and Kutas1998).

A final element that influences children’s comprehension of temporal connectives is the specific words themselves: temporal orders that are conveyed using the connective after tend to be more difficult for children to understand than those that are conveyed with before (E. V. Clark, Reference Clark1971; DeRuiter et al., Reference DeRuiter, Theakston, Brandt and Lieven2018; Pyykkönen & Järvikivi, Reference Pyykkönen and Järvikivi2012; Pyykkönen et al., Reference Pyykkönen, Niemi and Järvikivi2003). DeRuiter et al. (Reference DeRuiter, Lemen, Lieven, Brandt and Theakston2021) attributed this effect to differing patterns of use in children’s input. They examined dense corpora of two parent-child dyads and found that while before and after appeared at similar rates overall, before was used more consistently to order two full clauses (before the girl went outside, she ate a donut). The connective after was more frequently used than before in non-temporal contexts (e.g., look after) and for ordering nominal events (e.g., after dinner). Given that all of the experimental studies used sentences with two full clauses, these corpus results suggest that children are more likely to have encountered that particular type of sentence with the connective before than after. However, one notable exception to before being easier for children found no effect of connective on accuracy, at least by age 7 years (Blything & Cain, Reference Blything and Cain2016).

Overall, the existing literature paints a clear picture of what influences preschool-aged children’s ability to succeed with before/after: they benefit from naturally-ordered events, iconically presented in the sentence, using the connective before Footnote 1 . However, the picture becomes more complicated when older children are considered. Blything and colleagues (Blything & Cain, Reference Blything and Cain2016; Blything et al., Reference Blything, Davies and Cain2015) found strong success in accuracy in children by the age of 7 years regardless of the naturalness of the event ordering or the connective, while Pyykkönen and colleagues (Pyykkönen & Järvikivi, Reference Pyykkönen and Järvikivi2012; Pyykkönen et al., Reference Pyykkönen, Niemi and Järvikivi2003) found that children as old as 12 years had ongoing difficulties and their accuracy was influenced by both naturalness of event ordering and connective. Even for iconicity, which both studies found to be a helpful cue, Blything and colleagues (Blything & Cain, Reference Blything and Cain2016; Blything et al., Reference Blything, Davies and Cain2015) argue that it is tempered by an interaction between the syntactic position of the connectives and the memory demands of creating mental models. They note that before creates iconic interpretations when it appears in the middle of a sentence (X before Y) and non-iconic interpretations when it appears at the beginning (before Y, X); the reverse is true for after. Thus, significant working memory demands on building the model only arise for non-iconic before, because only in that case has the listener been signaled that they must remember the first event so that they can order it after an event yet to come. When after is in initial position, it is iconic, allowing one to simply begin building the model. Encountering after in medial position is non-iconic, but it does not impose significant working memory demands on the model-building because it appears immediately before the second event. In support of their position, Blything and Cain (Reference Blything and Cain2016) found that children’s working memory significantly improved the fit of their model predicting children’s accuracy and reaction times for iconic and non-iconic before/after sentences. However, DeRuiter et al. (Reference DeRuiter, Theakston, Brandt and Lieven2018) did not find that children’s working memory predicted slightly younger children’s accuracy above and beyond the influence of iconicity and connective.

Several differences across the studies make it difficult to determine why such different results have been obtained. One major difference is the language of testing. Most studies, including those by Blything and colleagues, tested children learning English as their native language. By contrast, Pyykkönen and colleagues tested children learning Finnish. While Finnish has similarities to English in how these temporal connectives are used, it is distinctly possible that there are other linguistic features of Finnish which may make the task more difficult in that language. Another notable difference between studies is the method of presentation and response. Pyykkönen and colleagues examined children as old as 12 years, and tested them in an ecologically valid way for children in school, using written sentences and asking them to mark their responses with pencil and paper. However, requiring children to read and write may have created a cognitive load that interfered with their ability to interpret these temporal connectives. By contrast, Blything and colleagues presented their 3- to 7-year-old participants with fully animated clips of each event to be ordered. This type of presentation may have facilitated children’s ability to display their understanding, but it might also have increased the memory demands of the task as children had to remember what happened in each short video.

The current study aims to resolve the question of whether the same factors that influence accuracy in young children – naturalness of ordering, iconicity of presentation, and connective – continue to impact children learning English into the school years. We tested a broad age range from 7 – 12 years that encompasses the oldest ages tested by Blything and colleagues through the oldest children tested by Pyykkönen and colleagues. Our method was modeled most closely on that of DeRuiter et al. (Reference DeRuiter, Theakston, Brandt and Lieven2018): children were presented with sentences auditorily over headphones, eliminating the need for children to be able to read; the events were represented by individual still images which were more vivid than the written word but did not require the memory demands of a movie. And finally, we included all three factors in a within-subjects design, allowing us to determine how they may interact with each other across this age range.

Method

Participants

Sixty-five 7- to 12-year-old native English-speaking children completed the protocol (between 10 and 12 children for each annual age bin). An additional child was tested, but their data were excluded because parents reported that the child was diagnosed with autism. The mean ages of the 7-, 8-, 9-, 10-, 11-, and 12-year-old groups was 7.4, 8.5, 9.4, 10.4, 11.5, and 12.4 years, respectively. Thirty-one were female, 34 male, and 0 nonbinary. Most participants were White (n=55), with the remaining identifying as Asian-American (n=3), Black/African-American (n=2), or biracial White and Black/African-American (n=5). Parents reported no history of speech-language or hearing challenges.

Materials and Procedure

Children were tested in a fully repeated measures 2x2x2 design with two stimulus sentences per condition, resulting in 16 trials, which were randomized across participants. The three factors were iconicity (iconic/non-iconic), naturalness (natural/arbitrary), and temporal connective (before/after). All sentences contained two fully tensed clauses. Table 1 displays the sentences in each condition. On each trial, a unique RMS-equalized sentence digitally recorded by a Midland American English-speaking woman in a sound-attenuated room was presented auditorily. At the same time, two colorful cartoon images (e.g., publicly available clipart), one showing each of the described events, were displayed on a 24-inch Dell monitor. After each sentence ended, children were prompted to select one image with the question, “which event happened first?” uttered by the same speaker who recorded the sentences. The side on which the pictures were presented was randomized. As an example, Figure 1 displays the images used for one of the sentences used in the Iconic/Natural/Before condition. Children were instructed to press the left-most button on an E-Prime Serial Response Box if they thought the left-hand image reflected the correct answer and the right-most button if the right-hand image reflected the correct answer. To ensure the children understood the button-press task, they were presented with several practice trials that were not directly related to the test trials.

Table 1. Test Sentences for each Condition

Figure 1. Images displayed for one of the sentences in the Iconic/Natural/Before condition, “The girl blew out the candles before she at the cake.” The image on the left reflects the correct response to “What happened first?” and the right-hand image is the foil.

Recruitment took place at a local science museum, and testing took place in a quiet lab within the museum. Stimulus presentation was controlled with E-Prime v. 2.0 (Psychology Software Tools, 2007) on a Dell Optiplex 790 desktop computer. Auditory stimuli were presented at a comfortable level over binaural Audiotechnica headphones (model 8TH-770COM). Children were not provided feedback but were given praise and encouragement after each test trial. All testing procedures were approved by the local institutional review board. Caregivers provided verbal permission and children provided verbal assent to participate. As is customary in museum laboratory settings, participants were not compensated. Data were analyzed in SPSS v. 28.

Results

Figure 2 displays mean accuracy for each condition as a function of age group. A repeated measures ANOVA with three within-subject factors (Connective, Naturalness, Iconicity) and one between-subjects factor (age group) revealed that accuracy was better for the before connective than after, F(1,59)=59.651, p<.001, η2p=.503. Similarly, accuracy was better for natural than arbitrary temporal orders, F(1,59)=33.306, p=.038, η2p=.361, and for iconic than non-iconic temporal structures, F(1,59)=4.530, p<.001, η2p=.071. There was no main effect of age group (p=.467), nor did age group interact with any of the temporal sentence structure variables, reflecting that accuracy did not change significantly across the elementary to early-teenage range in this sample. Naturalness interacted with Connective, F(1,59)=22.601, p<.001, η2p=.277, which resulted from accuracy for before being high regardless of Naturalness (>0.92), but accuracy for after being higher when the sentences followed a natural (0.85) rather than arbitrary temporal order (0.62). Naturalness also interacted with Iconicity, F(1,59)=13.776, p<.001, η2p=.189, reflecting that accuracy was poorer with an arbitrary temporal order, particularly when the order in which events occur in the sentence was mismatched with how they occur in the real world (non-iconic). Finally, there was a three-way interaction among Connective, Naturalness, and Iconicity F(1,59)=13.285, p<.001, η2p=.184. Sentences using before were consistently perceived accurately (>.92) regardless of their iconicity or naturalness. In contrast, sentences using after were less accurate than those with before, particularly when they had an arbitrary and non-iconic temporal order (0.52).

Figure 2. Mean proportion correct (+ standard error) for each condition as a function of age group.

Discussion

The goal of this study was to examine the persistence of three temporal elements in guiding children’s temporal interpretation. The results revealed that for 7- to 12-year-olds the ability to accurately order two events is influenced by the naturalness of the ordering, the iconicity of the temporal structure, and the specific connective used. While all three factors were significant predictors of children’s accuracy on their own, there was a notable interaction among the three: children of all ages were significantly less accurate when the sentences contained an arbitrary ordering of events expressed in a non-iconic manner using the connective after.

These results are consistent with what has been previously found for younger children acquiring English (Blything & Cain, Reference Blything and Cain2016; Blything et al., Reference Blything, Davies and Cain2015; E. V. Clark, Reference Clark1971; DeRuiter et al., Reference DeRuiter, Theakston, Brandt and Lieven2018; French & Brown, Reference French and Brown1977), as well as similarly-aged children using a somewhat different method acquiring Finnish (Pyykkönen & Järvikivi, Reference Pyykkönen and Järvikivi2012; Pyykkönen et al., Reference Pyykkönen, Niemi and Järvikivi2003). They stand first of all, therefore, as an important replication of existing results in this field (cf. Nosek et al., Reference Nosek, Hardwicke, Moshontz, Allard, Corker, Dreber, Fidler, Hilgard, Kline Struhl, Nuijten, Rohrer, Romero, Scheel, Scherer, Schönbrodt and Vazire2022). They do, however, differ somewhat from Blything and colleagues (Blything & Cain, Reference Blything and Cain2016; Blything et al., Reference Blything, Davies and Cain2015) who found that only iconicity was a reliably persistent factor in 7-year-olds.

We speculate that the reason that the Blything group’s results differed from our results (as well as those of other researchers) has to do with the fact that they provided children with particularly robust support for the events. Both Blything studies noted above presented children with animated videos depicting the events to be ordered, while the current study (similar to DeRuiter et al., Reference DeRuiter, Theakston, Brandt and Lieven2018) used still pictures; Pyykkönen and colleagues (Pyykkönen & Järvikivi, Reference Pyykkönen and Järvikivi2012; Pyykkönen et al., Reference Pyykkönen, Niemi and Järvikivi2003) provided no pictorial support at all. It seems possible that more detailed event representations facilitate children’s ability to incorporate them into a mental model. Thus, the differing results across studies actually provide support for the core argument in Blything and Cain (Reference Blything and Cain2016): the fundamental process behind interpreting before/after sentences involves sequencing events within a mental model. Anything that makes it easier to create the appropriate mental model will improve accuracy and processing.

The elements examined in this study facilitate model building in different ways. Events which are naturally ordered are conceptually easier to sequence in a model than arbitrarily-ordered events because one can draw on rich background knowledge. We speculate that providing richer representations of the events – as with animated movies – offers a similar conceptual advantage. Compared to non-iconic orders, an iconic mapping between the events in a sentence and the ordering in which the events should be sequenced in the model allows for more straightforward model building. However, the advantage for before over after does not have any obvious conceptual or processing reason to facilitate model-building. To explain the difference between the connectives, we appeal to the explanation offered in DeRuiter et al. (Reference DeRuiter, Lemen, Lieven, Brandt and Theakston2021): the input data for the two connectives are different.

DeRuiter et al. (Reference DeRuiter, Lemen, Lieven, Brandt and Theakston2021) conducted an analysis on before/after usage in the dense corpora of two child-parent dyads. We verified their main findings by conducting a similar corpus analysis that drew on the full set of American English-speaking children in the CHILDES database (MacWhinney, Reference MacWhinney2000). We extracted all the tokens of before and after from mothers (N=3997) and children (N=1065). Like DeRuiter et al. (Reference DeRuiter, Lemen, Lieven, Brandt and Theakston2021) we found that the two connectives were used at very similar rates overall; but also, like them, we found that after was more likely to be used in non-temporal constructions than before. Moreover, even within sentences that were temporally ordering two events, after was more likely than before to refer to one of those events in a nominal form (e.g., before bed, after supper). Nominal events provide evidence for how to use the connectives to sequence events, but they look quite different from the kinds of sentences involving two full clauses used in the current and all previous studies. In addition, parallel to the corpus work of Diessel (Reference Diessel2008), we found that both parents and children were more likely to place the event with the connective (whether the event was expressed in a full clause or as a nominal form) second. We note that this ordering places the main clause first more often, which may facilitate processing of the temporal ordering (cf. H. H. Clark & E. V. Clark, Reference Clark and Clark1968). Interestingly, however, the preference for beginning with the main clause was far stronger for sentences containing before, where that order preserved iconicity than for sentences containing after, where an initial main clause creates a non-iconic interpretation. One additional phenomenon worth noting (which was not discussed in detail in DeRuiter et al., Reference DeRuiter, Lemen, Lieven, Brandt and Theakston2021) is that the present moment was often used as one of the times being sequenced, particularly for the connective before: before now; did you ever do that before? These utterances suggest that in typical usage, one common anchor point for creating temporal sequences in a model is the here-and-now. These results constitute another replication (again, see Nosek et al., Reference Nosek, Hardwicke, Moshontz, Allard, Corker, Dreber, Fidler, Hilgard, Kline Struhl, Nuijten, Rohrer, Romero, Scheel, Scherer, Schönbrodt and Vazire2022). In combination with DeRuiter et al. (Reference DeRuiter, Lemen, Lieven, Brandt and Theakston2021), these usage results suggest that after is less consistently used to signal temporal sequencing and further, that the types of sentences used in these studies do not represent the most common ways that children use or hear events and time being sequenced.

In conclusion, the results of the current study show that even into the middle-school years, children’s ability to interpret before and after is influenced by the same range of factors that influence younger children. Moreover, all of these factors are plausibly related to the process of constructing a mental model of the sequence of events.

Acknowledgements

We thank Allison Bean as well as the students who worked on this project: Kara McClain, Sara Gryboski and Aliya Tull. Thank you to the Center of Science and Industry (COSI) for their ongoing support. We also thank NSF SMA-1757020 for supporting one of our students. A preliminary analysis of some of these data was presented as a poster at the BU Conference on Language Development (2019).

Competing interest

The Authors declare none.

Footnotes

1 An additional factor that may contribute to children’s comprehension is the order of the clauses: before and after create subordinate clauses which can be placed either before or after the main clause of the sentence. Moreover, H H. Clark and E. V. Clark (1968) found that both clause order and iconicity of events and clauses influenced ease of processing among a range of temporal sentences. However, for sentences containing just before and after, there is a general confound between the ordering of the main and subordinate clauses and the interaction between connective type and iconicity making it quite difficult to disentangle the potential independent effects of clause ordering. We have therefore put aside the issue of main and subordinate clauses within the experiment, but will return to it in the general discussion.

References

Blything, L. P., & Cain, K. (2016). Children’s processing and comprehension of complex sentences containing temporal connectives: The influence of memory on the time course of accurate responsesDevelopmental Psychology52(10), 15171529.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blything, L. P., Davies, R., & Cain, K. (2015). Young children’s comprehension of temporal relations in complex sentences: The influence of memory on performanceChild Development86(6), 19221934.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, E. V. (1971). On the acquisition of the meaning of before and afterJournal of verbal learning and verbal behavior10(3), 266275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, H. H., & Clark, E. V. (1968). Semantic distinctions and memory for complex sentences. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 20, 129138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeRuiter, L. E., Theakston, A. L., Brandt, S., & Lieven, E. V. (2018). Iconicity affects children’s comprehension of complex sentences: The role of semantics, clause order, input and individual differencesCognition171, 202224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeRuiter, L. E., Lemen, H. C., Lieven, E. V., Brandt, S., & Theakston, A. L. (2021). Structural and interactional aspects of adverbial sentences in English mother-child interactions: an analysis of two dense corporaJournal of Child Language48(6), 11501184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diessel, H. (2008). Iconicity of sequence: A corpus analysis of the positioning of temporal adverbial clauses in English. Cognitive Linguistics, 19(3), 465490.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
French, L. A., & Brown, A. L. (1977). Comprehension of before and after in logical and arbitrary sequencesJournal of Child Language4(2), 247256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gernsbacher, M. A. (1990). Language comprehension as structure building. Hillsdale, NJ: LEA Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES project: tools for analyzing talk. Mahwah NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Münte, T. F., Schiltz, K., & Kutas, M. (1998). When temporal terms belie conceptual orderNature395(6697), 7173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nosek, B. A., Hardwicke, T. E., Moshontz, H., Allard, A., Corker, K. S., Dreber, A., Fidler, F., Hilgard, J., Kline Struhl, M., Nuijten, M. B., Rohrer, J. M., Romero, F., Scheel, A. M., Scherer, L. D., Schönbrodt, F. D., & Vazire, S. (2022). Replicability, Robustness, and Reproducibility in Psychological Science. Annual Review of Psychology, 73, 719748. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-020821-114157CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Psychology Software Tools. (2007). E-Prime 2.0. Pittsburgh, PA: Psychology Software Tools.Google Scholar
Pyykkönen, P., & Järvikivi, J. (2012). Children and situation models of multiple events. Developmental Psychology, 48, 521529. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0025526CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pyykkönen, P., Niemi, J., & Järvikivi, J. (2003). Sentence structure, temporal order and linearity: Slow emergence of adult-like syntactic performance in FinnishSKY Journal of Linguistics16, 113138.Google Scholar
Zwaan, R. A., & Radvansky, G. A. (1998). Situation models in language comprehension and memory. Psychological Bulletin, 123, 162185.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Test Sentences for each Condition

Figure 1

Figure 1. Images displayed for one of the sentences in the Iconic/Natural/Before condition, “The girl blew out the candles before she at the cake.” The image on the left reflects the correct response to “What happened first?” and the right-hand image is the foil.

Figure 2

Figure 2. Mean proportion correct (+ standard error) for each condition as a function of age group.