Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-lnqnp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T07:12:21.230Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Dual Evolutionary Foundations of Political Ideology Predict Divergent Responses to COVID-19

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 April 2023

Kyle Fischer*
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
Ananish Chaudhuri
Affiliation:
Department of Economics, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand Behavioral Economics area, CESifo, Munich, Germany
Quentin D. Atkinson
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand School of Anthropology and Museum Ethnography, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
*
*Corresponding author. E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Political conservatives' opposition to COVID-19 restrictions is puzzling given the well-documented links between conservatism and conformity, threat sensitivity, and pathogen aversion. We propose a resolution based on the Dual Foundations Theory of ideology, which holds that ideology comprises two dimensions, one reflecting trade-offs between threat-driven conformity and individualism, and another reflecting trade-offs between empathy-driven cooperation and competition. We test predictions derived from this theory in a UK sample using individuals' responses to COVID-19 and widely-used measures of the two dimensions – ‘right-wing authoritarianism’ (RWA) and ‘social dominance orientation’ (SDO), respectively. Consistent with our predictions, we show that RWA, but not SDO, increased following the pandemic and that high-RWA conservatives do display more concerned, conformist, pro-lockdown attitudes, while high-SDO conservatives display less empathic, cooperative attitudes and are anti-lockdown. This helps explain paradoxical prior results and highlights how a focus on unidimensional ideology can mask divergent motives across the ideological landscape.

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

In the wake of the COVID-19 pandemic, a popular view that gained ground was that political progressives were in favour of government-imposed restrictions to prevent the spread of the virus while the conservative focus was on resuming normal activities to prevent an economic downturn. This is supported by many studies that show an association between conservatism and reduced concern about COVID-19 and reduced compliance with pandemic prevention measures (Allcott et al. Reference Allcott2020; Burkova et al. Reference Burkova2021; Calvillo et al. Reference Calvillo2020; Christensen et al. Reference Christensen2020; Clinton et al. Reference Clinton2021; Collins, Mandel, and Schywiola Reference Collins, Mandel and Schywiola2021; Conway et al. Reference Conway2021; Gadarian, Goodman, and Pepinsky Reference Gadarian, Goodman and Pepinsky2021; Gollwitzer et al. Reference Gollwitzer2020; Painter and Qiu Reference Painter and Qiu2021; Pennycook et al. Reference Pennycook2022; Stroebe et al. Reference Stroebe2021). This variation in responses to COVID-19 has been attributed to partisanship, the polarizing rhetoric of political elites, and variation in trust of and exposure to different information sources (Calvillo et al. Reference Calvillo2020; Gollwitzer et al. Reference Gollwitzer2020; Graham et al. Reference Graham2020; Motta, Stecula, and Farhart Reference Motta, Stecula and Farhart2020; Painter and Qiu Reference Painter and Qiu2021; Pennycook et al. Reference Pennycook2022; Ruisch et al. Reference Ruisch2021; Samore et al. Reference Samore2021; Sloan et al. Reference Sloan2021; Stroebe et al. Reference Stroebe2021). For example, conservatives in the USA were inclined to follow and believe Donald Trump's anti-lockdown rhetoric and conservative news sources. Conservatives in the UK may also have taken cues from their prime minister, Boris Johnson, who, initially, was relatively slow in his response to the pandemic but later urged people to physically distance themselves and stay home. However, the above accounts do not elucidate what, if any, psychological mechanisms underpin the observed differences in COVID-19 responses across the political spectrum. Moreover, they appear inconsistent with work in political psychology linking conservatism to increased threat sensitivity, including pathogen aversion (Hibbing, Smith, and Alford Reference Hibbing, Smith and Alford2014; Jost et al. Reference Jost2017) and progressivism, to decreased compliance and decreased conformity (Jost et al. Reference Jost2018). They also do not explain why conservatives in some countries, and some conservative populations, were concerned about COVID-19 and pro-lockdowns (Brouard, Vasilopoulos, and Becher Reference Brouard, Vasilopoulos and Becher2020; Chen, Frey, and Presidente Reference Chen, Frey and Presidente2021; Choma et al. Reference Choma2021; Dryhurst et al. Reference Dryhurst2020; Gelfand et al. Reference Gelfand2021; Mula et al. Reference Mula2022; Pavlovic, Todosijevic, and Stanojevic Reference Pavlovic, Todosijevic and Stanojevic2020).

Here, we argue that these apparent contradictions arise from the assumption that political ideologyFootnote 1 is one-dimensional (that is, conservative v. progressive). As highlighted in a recent review article, a unidimensional view of political ideology is, if not incorrect, then certainly incomplete (Claessens et al. Reference Claessens2020). Scales measuring unidimensional ideology often have low external validity and produce more than one latent variable in factor analyses (Duckitt and Sibley Reference Duckitt and Sibley2009; Treier and Hillygus Reference Treier and Hillygus2009). In contrast, over the last few decades, many scholars using a diverse range of methodologies and theoretical frameworks have identified two basic dimensions of political ideology (Claessens et al. Reference Claessens2020; Duckitt and Sibley Reference Duckitt and Sibley2009; Federico et al. Reference Federico2013; Feldman and Johnston Reference Feldman and Johnston2014; Johnston and Ollerenshaw Reference Johnston and Ollerenshaw2020). The first dimension, linked to measures like ‘social dominance orientation’ (SDO) and hierarchy v. egalitarianism (Claessens et al. Reference Claessens2020; Duckitt and Sibley Reference Duckitt and Sibley2009; Pratto et al. Reference Pratto1994), predicts views on issues such as inequality, taxation, and social welfare – people high in SDO, for example, tend to be on the political right and hold economically conservative views (Duriez and Van Hiel Reference Duriez and Van Hiel2002; Lönnqvist and Kivikangas Reference Lönnqvist and Kivikangas2019; Perry and Sibley Reference Perry and Sibley2013; Pratto et al. Reference Pratto1994). The second dimension is linked to measures like ‘right-wing authoritarianism’ (RWA), cultural tightness v. looseness, and collectivism v. individualism (Claessens et al. Reference Claessens2020; Duckitt and Sibley Reference Duckitt and Sibley2009; Gelfand et al. Reference Gelfand2011; Triandis and Gelfand Reference Triandis and Gelfand1998), which predicts views on issues like same-sex marriage, abortion, and other traditional or religious values. People high in RWA are also part of the political right and hold socially conservative views (Cizmar et al. Reference Cizmar2014; Duckitt et al. Reference Duckitt2010; Duriez and Van Hiel Reference Duriez and Van Hiel2002; Passini Reference Passini2020).

Synthesizing the above work on two-dimensional ideology with relevant work in primatology and evolutionary anthropology on the evolution of human social life, the recently proposed Dual Foundations Theory of political ideology provides an ultimate, evolutionary explanation for the existence and nature of the two dimensions (Claessens et al. Reference Claessens2020). The theory also elucidates and makes precise predictions regarding the psychological and socio-environmental antecedents of the two dimensions that are directly relevant to the COVID-19 pandemic. Rising to the call for more theory-driven inference in the social sciences (Muthukrishna and Henrich Reference Muthukrishna and Henrich2019), here we derive and test predictions from the Dual Foundations Theory regarding how two-dimensional ideology will (a) relate to responses to COVID-19 and (b) be affected by the threat of COVID-19. We focus our predictions on SDO and RWA because they are among the most widely used measures of the two dimensions of ideology, they have good psychometric properties, and they have been shown to predict a range of political behaviours and attitudes (Duckitt and Sibley Reference Duckitt and Sibley2009; Duckitt and Sibley Reference Duckitt, Sibley, Sibley and Barlow2017; Johnston and Ollerenshaw Reference Johnston and Ollerenshaw2020; Stangor and Leary Reference Stangor and Leary2006)Footnote 2. Our findings provide a deeper understanding of the psychological and evolutionary origins of divergent pandemic responses, helping to explain the abovementioned paradoxical results that political scientists and others have found among politically progressive v. conservative individuals and populations.

The Dual Evolutionary Foundations of Political Ideology

Claessens et al. (Reference Claessens2020) argue that the two repeatedly and independently identified dimensions of ideology can be understood as the product of two basic tensions inherent to the evolution of human group living (Tomasello et al. Reference Tomasello2012). SDO and related constructs concern the trade-off between cooperation for the common good v. individual self-interest and competition. RWA and related constructs concern the trade-off between threat-driven group conformity and social control v. individual autonomy and open-mindedness. Strategic responses to these dual challenges of group living are argued to give rise to contemporary variations in political attitudes, values, and policy preferences. This variation within populations is maintained by a combination of (1) fluctuating selection on heritable biological predispositions, (2) social learning, and (3) evolved species-typical responses to socio-environmental triggers – known as ‘behavioural plasticity’.

Under this Dual Foundations Theory, SDO can be explained as underpinned by a competitive, self-interested motivation for dominance, driven by a view of the world as a ‘competitive jungle’ versus a more cooperative, other-regarding orientation motivated by empathic and egalitarian preferences. Consistent with this, people low in SDO and similar measures show greater empathic concern and cooperative behaviour in economic games, while those high in SDO score higher on traits such as Machiavellianism and display more competitive behaviour (Chiao et al. Reference Chiao2009; Fischer, Atkinson, and Chaudhuri Reference Fischer, Atkinson, Chaudhuri and Chaudhuri2021; Jones and Figueredo Reference Jones and Figueredo2013; Sidanius et al. Reference Sidanius2013).

In contrast, the Dual Foundations Theory conceptualizes RWA as underpinned by drives for ingroup conformity and norm enforcement that promote group cohesion in the face of threats to group viability. In addition, perceived threats and uncertainty in the environment are expected to act as a trigger, increasing norm-enforcing conformity and, in turn, RWA (Claessens et al. Reference Claessens2020). Consistent with this, people high in RWA (versus low) tend to be more conformist, punitive, prejudiced against norm-violators, and sensitive to threatening/disgusting stimuli (Feldman Reference Feldman2003; Fischer et al. Reference Fischer2023; Hunsberger Reference Hunsberger1996; McKee and Feather Reference McKee and Feather2008; Shaffer and Duckitt Reference Shaffer and Duckitt2013). Moreover, increased experimentally induced or real threats (for example, group conflict or disease) are associated with increased conformity (Murray and Schaller Reference Murray and Schaller2012), norm enforcement (Sääksvuori, Mappes, and Puurtinen Reference Sääksvuori, Mappes and Puurtinen2011), and RWA (Asbrock and Fritsche Reference Asbrock and Fritsche2013; Duckitt and Fisher Reference Duckitt and Fisher2003). Others have also identified population-level correlations between levels of threat (for example, warfare or disease) and RWA or related variables (Gelfand et al. Reference Gelfand2011; Lindén, Björklund, and Bäckström Reference Lindén, Björklund and Bäckström2018; Terrizzi, Shook, and McDaniel Reference Terrizzi, Shook and McDaniel2013; Tybur et al. Reference Tybur2016).

Based on the Dual Foundations Theory, we make three key predictions regarding (1) how SDO and RWA will differentially relate to cooperative/other-regarding and conformist/norm-enforcing responses to COVID-19, (2) how SDO and RWA will differentially relate to concern about COVID-19, and (3) how the threats associated with COVID-19 will affect (or not affect) SDO and RWA scores.

Prediction 1: Low-SDO progressives will support cooperative/other-regarding responses to COVID-19; high-RWA conservatives will support conformist/norm-enforcing responses to COVID-19.

As noted above, studies on unidimensional ideology and the COVID-19 pandemic have mostly linked political conservatism to reduced cooperation and compliance (Allcott et al. Reference Allcott2020; Burkova et al. Reference Burkova2021; Calvillo et al. Reference Calvillo2020; Christensen et al. Reference Christensen2020; Clinton et al. Reference Clinton2021; Collins, Mandel, and Schywiola Reference Collins, Mandel and Schywiola2021; Conway et al. Reference Conway2021; Gadarian, Goodman, and Pepinsky Reference Gadarian, Goodman and Pepinsky2021; Gollwitzer et al. Reference Gollwitzer2020; Painter and Qiu Reference Painter and Qiu2021; Pennycook et al. Reference Pennycook2022; Stroebe et al. Reference Stroebe2021). According to the Dual Foundations Theory, this may be driven by high-SDO conservatives' reduced cooperation, and it may mask a link between high-RWA conservatism and conformity. However, this remains an open question because studies of how ideology relates to COVID-19 do not tend to distinguish between cooperative/empathic and conformist/norm-enforcing responses to COVID-19 or control for shared variance between the two dimensions of ideology. The latter is important because the two dimensions often covary and influence each other's effects on other variables (Costello and Lilienfeld Reference Costello and Lilienfeld2021). For example, a study that does not control for RWA may find an association between SDO and support for uncooperative responses but, given that high-SDO individuals are very often also higher in RWA, it is difficult to tell whether the result is driven by SDO and not RWA. Suggestive evidence supporting Prediction 1 comes from a study on COVID-19 in the UK, which found that only low-SDO progressives display prosocial attitudes (Politi et al. Reference Politi2021) and another finding that only authoritarians display punitive attitudes towards rule-breakers (Mellon, Bailey, and Prosser Reference Mellon, Bailey and Prosser2021).

Prediction 2: High-RWA conservatives will be concerned about the pandemic in general and the many threats associated with it (that is, not only familiar and unfamiliar others' health but also their health and finances and the economy); low-SDO progressives will be concerned about the pandemic in general and, specifically, about threats that trigger empathic concern (that is, familiar and unfamiliar others' health).

Most existing studies link concern about COVID-19 to political progressivism or related variables (Burkova et al. Reference Burkova2021; Calvillo et al. Reference Calvillo2020; Clinton et al. Reference Clinton2021; Collins, Mandel, and Schywiola Reference Collins, Mandel and Schywiola2021; Gadarian, Goodman, and Pepinsky Reference Gadarian, Goodman and Pepinsky2021; Pennycook et al. Reference Pennycook2022; Stroebe et al. Reference Stroebe2021) but some studies link concern about COVID-19 to political conservatism (Dryhurst et al. Reference Dryhurst2020; Mula et al. Reference Mula2022; Pavlovic, Todosijevic, and Stanojevic Reference Pavlovic, Todosijevic and Stanojevic2020). The Dual Foundations Theory predicts that the former relationship is driven by low-SDO progressives' empathic concern while the latter relationship is driven by high-RWA conservatives' threat sensitivity. Consistent with this, a study on COVID-19 in the US, that controls for shared variance between the two dimensions of ideology, links both economic progressivism and social conservatism to precautionary behaviours but does not measure different kinds of threats (Samore et al. Reference Samore2021).

Prediction 3: RWA but not SDO will significantly increase in response to threats posed by COVID-19.

Longitudinal studies find weak or inconsistent support for a link between pandemic threats and increased unidimensional conservatism (Beall, Hofer, and Schaller Reference Beall, Hofer and Schaller2016; Golec de Zavala et al. Reference Golec de Zavala2020; Inbar et al. Reference Inbar2016; Rosenfeld and Tomiyama Reference Rosenfeld and Tomiyama2021; Tiokhin and Hruschka Reference Tiokhin and Hruschka2017). However, several longitudinal studies (Golec de Zavala et al. Reference Golec de Zavala2020; Karwowski et al. Reference Karwowski2020; Pazhoohi and Kingstone Reference Pazhoohi and Kingstone2021; Rosenfeld and Tomiyama Reference Rosenfeld and Tomiyama2021) and an experimental study (Karwowski et al. Reference Karwowski2020) finds at least some support for a link between COVID-19 and increased RWA or social conservatism. The only longitudinal study that we are aware of that examines both dimensions of ideology found that scores on the RWA, but not on the SDO scale, increased in a Polish sample (Golec de Zavala et al. Reference Golec de Zavala2020) There was no attempt to link this increase to actual or perceived exposure to COVID-19 threats.

Here, we test the above predictions using longitudinal data on SDO and RWA from a large UK sample, as well as data on empathic concern, a broad array of attitudes about cooperative/other-regarding and conformist/norm-enforcing responses to COVID-19, and perceived concern about and actual exposure to COVID-19 threats. We control for (1) demographic variables, (2) shared variance between SDO and RWA to tease apart their separate effects, and (3) unidimensional left-right political affiliation (a composite of political orientation and party affiliation) to show that the effects of SDO and RWA are not somehow merely an artefact of political identity.

Methods

Participants and Procedure

We sampled participants in the UK in three waves using Prolific's online recruitment service (https://www.prolific.co/)Footnote 3. First, between 8 October and 7 November 2019 for another study (unpublished) where we measured SDO and RWA. This was approximately three months before the pandemic began (the first confirmed coronavirus case in the UK was on 31 January 2020). Second, between 18 April and 28 April 2020, where we measured SDO and RWA again, as well as a broad array of experiences of and responses to the pandemic. This was during the first lockdown in the UK and the first spike of COVID-19 cases (Supplementary Figure S1), and only a few weeks after the prime minister of the UK at the time, Boris Johnson, tested positive for coronavirus and urged people with symptoms like his to stay home. Third, between 15 June and 28 June 2020 – towards the end of the first spike of COVID-19 cases (Figure S1) – where we measured empathic concern.

For the first wave of data collection in late 2019, we recruited 553 participants (the survey was open to all Prolific participants who were over eighteen years old and lived in the UK). For the second wave, we approached these same participants. After excluding participants who did not meet our pre-registered exclusion criteria (https://aspredicted.org/blind.php?x=xs6jm8) or who, in the first wave, took under eight or over ninety minutes to complete the session, gave the same response to more than two-thirds of Likert scale items, or incorrectly answered at least one of two attention checks, we were left with 433 participants (age: 18–72, mean 37; sex: 285 female; race: 44 non-white; political affiliation: left = 226, centre = 57, right = 97, other or unknown = 53). For the third wave of data collection, we were able to recruit 368 of the same participants from the second wave, of whom some were excluded due to exclusion criteria from the first two waves and having completion times outside of three standard deviations of the mean for the third wave, leaving 357 participants for analyses involving empathic concern (age: 18–72, mean 38; sex: 235 female; race: 41 non-white; political affiliation: left = 188, centre = 46, right = 78, other = 45). However, we lost observations for certain analyses because some participants did not respond to every question and, for analyses where we controlled for left-right political affiliation, we removed data from six participants who gave conflicting information regarding this variable (that is, they indicated that they voted for the Tory Party but identified as left-wing or indicated that they voted for the Labour or Green Party but identified as right-wing). Nonetheless, the minimum number of observations for any analyses not involving empathic concern was 374, and for analyses involving empathic concern it was 308. Analyses using G*Power suggest that we need 175 participants to detect a correlation effect size of r = 0.21 for eighty per cent power and 289 participants for ninety-five per cent power. Our sample size is comfortably above this threshold. Considering that most of the UK population is white and on the political left (Burton and Tunnicliffe Reference Burton and Tunnicliffe2022) our sample also appears sufficiently diverse. There were no significant differences between participants from the first and second waves and participants who dropped out from these waves in terms of SDO, RWA, political affiliation, socioeconomic status, and sex, but there were differences in age and a slight difference in race for participants from the second wave (see Section 1 in the supplementary material). These differences cannot explain the longitudinal trends we observe, since our first- and second-wave comparison was within subjects.

In all waves of data collection, participants were directed from Prolific to a Qualtrics survey where they provided informed consent. In the first wave, participants completed SDO and RWA scales as well as other items and tasks not related to the current study. In the second wave, participants completed the same SDO and RWA scales as well as items on COVID-19 (COVID-19 items were separated into different blocks based on the method of response, with block order randomized). In both waves, the SDO and RWA items were randomized in the same block and this block appeared in random order with other blocks. In the third and final wave of data collection, participants answered several scales for another study (blocks and items within each block were randomized), of which we used only empathic concern here.

Materials

Unless stated otherwise all the below items were answered on Likert scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree).

Explanatory variables

We measured SDO with the sixteen-item SDO7 Scale (Ho et al. Reference Ho2015) and RWA with the thirty-six-item Authoritarianism-Conservatism-Traditionalism Scale (Duckitt et al. Reference Duckitt2010). We also measured empathic concern with the items (answered on a Likert scale, 1 = does not describe me well, 5 = describes me very well) related to this from the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (Davis Reference Davis1983). Cronbach's alpha reliability was 0.92 for SDO, 0.96 for RWA, and 0.86 for empathic concern.

Outcome variablesFootnote 4

(A) Cooperative/other-regarding responses to COVID-19. We used the mean of three items for cooperative, other-regarding responses to COVID-19: (1) ‘The government should waive all insurance costs and hospital fees for testing and treating COVID-19’, (2) ‘Paid leave should be granted to anyone diagnosed with COVID-19’, and (3) ‘I am very concerned about those most vulnerable to COVID-19’ (Pfattheicher et al. Reference Pfattheicher2020).Footnote 5

(B) Conformist/norm-enforcing responses to COVID-19. We used the mean of the items in Table 1 in our main analyses. Example items include the following, which we adapted from another study unrelated to COVID-19 (Stamkou et al. Reference Stamkou2019): ‘Imagine a person, ‘K’, who does not feel sick and so ignores the rules and goes out without a facemask and does not try at all to keep a safe two-metre distance from other people. To what extent does K's behaviour make you feel outraged?’ and ‘Finally, how do you think K should be treated?’ (answered on a Likert scale: 1 = ‘very leniently’, 7 = ‘very harshly’).

Table 1. Summary of principal components analysis results for the conformist/norm-enforcement items (n = 425) including pattern matrix, eigenvalues, proportion of variance, and Cronbach's alpha for each principal component

Notes: Factor loadings over 0.40 appear in bold. Items 3–6 and 10 were adapted from a recent study on COVID-19 (Conway, Woodward, and Zubrod Reference Conway, Woodward and Zubrod2020) and item 12 (12.1–12.5) was adapted from a study not related to COVID-19 (Stamkou et al. Reference Stamkou2019).

(C) Concern about COVID-19. We measured concern by asking participants how worried they are about the COVID-19 pandemic, their health, the health of familiar others (family and friends), the health of unfamiliar others, the economy, and their finances (see Section 2 of the supplementary material for the exact wording of these items). These were answered on a sliding scale from 1 (‘not at all worried’) to 7 (‘extremely worried’).

(D) Experiences of and exposure to COVID-19 threats. We asked participants various questions (see Section 2 of the supplementary material for a full list) that we put into a composite measure of exposure to COVID-19 threats (set to 1 if participants responded ‘Yes’ to one or more items, and 0 otherwise). For example, we asked participants the following: ‘Has anyone you personally know tested positive for COVID-19?’ [Yes, No, Not sure] (Everett et al. Reference Everett2020), and ‘Has COVID-19 impacted you negatively from a financial point of view?’ [Yes, No, Not sure], which was adapted from a recent study (Conway, Woodward, and Zubrod Reference Conway, Woodward and Zubrod2020).

Pre-Registration and Data Analysis

We pre-registered our predictions and analyses after the survey had been uploaded to and advertised via Prolific, but before we had downloaded and accessed any data (https://aspredicted.org/blind.php?x=xs6jm8). We pre-registered Predictions 1 and 3 mentioned in the introduction. Prediction 2 was based on support for Predictions 1 and 3 as we began to analyse the data, which is, therefore, presented as an exploratory analysis.

Regarding statistical analyses, we pre-registered that we would (1) use paired-samples t-tests to determine whether RWA or SDO increased following the onset of the pandemic, (2) use structural equation modelling (SEM) if fit indices support this or, if not, use standard regression analyses applied to scale means, with SDO and RWA (both measured before the pandemic) together in the models predicting the mean of either of our cooperative/other-regarding or conformist/norm-enforcing responses to COVID-19. We decided to include both SDO and RWA in the models to control for shared variance between them and thereby tease apart their separate effects, as recommended by a recent study (Costello and Lilienfeld Reference Costello and Lilienfeld2021). We also pre-registered that we would control for COVID-19 experiences/exposure, age, sex, race, socioeconomic status, and political affiliation. We adhered to this plan, but we also performed some additional exploratory analyses (see below and the results section for details). We did not use SEM because model fit assumptions for this were not met (for example, the RMSEA fit index for RWA was 0.12). We explored whether we could improve SEM performance by removing scale items, but this required removing many items, which compromised their coverage (the items are counterbalanced and selected to cover the different subcomponents of SDO and RWA). Consequently, we opted to retain the scales in their entirety and use average SDO and RWA scores in multiple regression analyses.

We used standardized beta estimates to measure effect sizes. We incorporated pre-registered covariates in the above regressions using our abovementioned composite measure of exposure to COVID-19 threats, as well as data provided by Prolific for age, sex, race (white v. non-white), socioeconomic status (indicated on a ladder from 1 = lowest to 10 = highest), and political affiliation. Prolific provides two sources of political affiliation data, neither of which is complete for all participants – one indicating which party they voted for and one indicating whether they identify as left-wing, centrist, or right-wing. To maximize data coverage, we combined information from these variables such that participants were classified as (1) left-wing if they voted for the Labour or Green Party or identified as left-wing (we excluded data for three participants who voted for one of these parties but identified as right-wing), (2) right-wing if they voted for the Tory Party or UK Independence Party or identified as right-wing (we excluded data for three participants who voted for the Tory Party but identified as left-wing), and (3) centrist if they identified as centrist. To test whether our interpretation of our results is contingent on the inclusion of the political affiliation covariate, we also reran all our analyses with this variable removed and reported these results, which generally did not meaningfully change, in the supplementary material (Section 5). Assumption checks revealed no significant problems with multicollinearity, non-linearity, heteroscedasticity, outliers, or non-independence of errors. However, most error distributions showed some departure from normality. To account for this, we report bootstrapped confidence intervals (standardized). We performed all analyses in R Statistical Software (R Core Team 2021).

Results

In Section 3 of the supplementary material, we provide means, standard deviations, and histograms for, and correlations between, all relevant variables.

Our first pre-registered prediction was that low-SDO progressives will support cooperative/other-regarding responses to COVID-19 whereas high-RWA conservatives will support conformist/norm-enforcing responses. In line with this, SDO was a significant negative predictor of the mean of all cooperative/other-regarding attitudes (RWA was unrelated to this), whereas RWA was a significant positive predictor of the mean of all conformist/norm-enforcing attitudes (Fig. 1; Supplementary Table S3). While we did not pre-register a prediction for SDO and conformist/norm-enforcing attitudes, we found that SDO was a weak (significant at the p < 0.1 level) negative predictor of conformist/norm-enforcing attitudes (Fig. 1; Supplementary Table S3).

Figure 1. Results for linear regressions with social dominance orientation (SDO) and right-wing authoritarianism (RWA) controlling for each other, predicting cooperative/other-regarding and conformist/norm-enforcing attitudes about COVID-19.

Notes: We control for age, sex, race, socioeconomic status, left-right political affiliation, and pandemic threat exposure in all models. See supplementary Table S3 for more detailed results, including covariate effects.

To better understand these relationships, we ran a principal component analysis with oblique rotation (Oblimin) on all the conformist/norm-enforcing items. The Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin measure verified the sampling adequacy for the analysis: KMO = 0.89, considered ‘great’ (Hutcheson and Sofroniou Reference Hutcheson and Sofroniou1999), and all KMO values for individual items were >0.72 (well above the acceptable limit of 0.5). Bartlett's test of sphericity, χ2(120) = 4,613.34, p < 0.001, indicated that correlations between items were sufficiently large for principal component analysis. Four components had eigenvalues over Kaiser's criterion of 1 (Kaiser Reference Kaiser1960) and in combination explained seventy-five per cent of the variance, with high internal reliabilities (Table 1). Interpretation of the factor loadings in Table 1 suggests that the first component (PC1) captures support for lockdown rules, the second (PC2) captures moral emotions towards rule breakers, the third (PC3) captures support for strict border control, and the fourth (PC4) captures support for severe government and military enforcement of lockdown rules.

We then ran regression analyses to examine whether SDO and RWA predicted these four principal components. We found that RWA positively predicted all of them while SDO significantly negatively predicted only PC1 (Fig. 2; Supplementary Table S4).

Figure 2. Results for linear regressions with social dominance orientation (SDO) and right-wing authoritarianism (RWA), controlling for each other, predicting different types of conformist/norm-enforcing attitudes revealed by principal components analysis (PC1–4 = Principal Component 1–4).

Notes: We control for age, sex, race, socioeconomic status, left-right political affiliation, and pandemic threat exposure in all models. See supplementary Table S4 for more detailed results, including covariate effects.

To investigate whether the putatively conformist/norm-enforcing responses of those low in SDO could plausibly reflect support for these measures among those with stronger empathic/other-regarding preferences, we also examined which of the conformist/norm-enforcing responses were predicted by our measure of empathic concern (controlling for RWA and covariates). We found that empathic concern showed the same pattern as low SDO; that is, predicting only PC1, albeit weakly (significant at the p < 0.1 level; Supplementary Table S5). In addition, analyses using the ‘Mediation’ package in R (Tingley et al. Reference Tingley2014) showed that this relationship was mediated by SDO (Supplementary Figure S3). Thus, the negative relationship between SDO and the mean of conformist/norm-enforcing responses appears to be explained by low-SDO progressives' empathy-driven support for lockdown rules (PC1).

We also explored whether the effects of SDO on support for cooperative/other-regarding or conformist/norm-enforcing attitudes would become stronger as RWA increases, and vice versa, by including interaction terms for SDO and RWA in the relevant models. We found no significant interactions between SDO and RWA (Supplementary Tables S6 & S7).

Our second prediction was that high-RWA conservatives will show concern about the pandemic in general, including threats to the self, others, personal finances, and the economy, while low-SDO progressives will be concerned about the pandemic to the extent that threats trigger empathic concern; that is, the health of others. As expected, RWA significantly positively predicted all our measures of concern about COVID-19 (Fig. 3; Supplementary Table S8). Also, as expected, we find that SDO weakly (statistically significant at the p < 0.1 level) and negatively predicted worrying about the pandemic in general (Model 1 in Fig. 3), but specifically about the health of unfamiliar others (Model 4 in Fig. 3) and, to a lesser degree, the health of familiar others (Model 3 in Fig. 3; statistically significant at the p < 0.1 level; see Supplementary Table S8).

Figure 3. Results for linear regressions with social dominance orientation (SDO) and right-wing authoritarianism (RWA) controlling for each other, predicting concerned/threat-sensitive attitudes about COVID-19.

Notes: We control for age, sex, race, socioeconomic status, left-right political affiliation, and pandemic threat exposure in all models. See supplementary Table S8 for more detailed results, including covariate effects.

We also included interaction terms for SDO and RWA in the relevant above models to explore whether the effects of SDO/RWA on concern about COVID-19 threats would become stronger as RWA/SDO increased. We found no significant interactions between SDO and RWA (Supplementary Table S9).

Our third and final prediction was that RWA, but not SDO, will significantly increase in response to threats posed by COVID-19. In line with this pre-registered prediction, there was a significant increase in mean RWA measured during the pandemic compared to before the pandemic (before: M = 3.517, SE = 0.054; during: M = 3.576, SE = 0.057; t(432) = 2.837, p = 0.002*) with a small effect size (r 2 = 0.018) while SDO remained unchanged (before: M = 2.471, SE = 0.049; during: M = 2.480, SE = 0.053; t(432) = 0.311, p = 0.756, r 2 = 0).

To investigate this further, we used regression analyses to examine whether the change in RWA was predicted by the perceived COVID-19 threat (Item C.1 in Section 2 of the supplementary material measuring general concern about the pandemic) or exposure to COVID-19 threats (a composite of Items D.1–6 in Section 2 of the supplementary material), controlling for demographic variables and left-right political affiliation. We found that perceived threat – concern about the pandemic – had a significant positive effect on the change in RWA (β = 0.13, SE = 0.05, p = 0.012*, bootstrapped 95 per cent CI: [0.06, 0.28]). However, self-reported exposure to COVID-19 threats did not significantly predict the change in RWA (β = −0.06, SE = 0.12, p = 0.629, bootstrapped 95 per cent CI [−0.33, 0.24]). See Supplementary Table S10 for full model descriptions.

Discussion

We found support for each of our three basic predictions, derived from the dual evolutionary foundations of political ideology, regarding the divergent effects of SDO and RWA on responses to COVID-19. First, we found that SDO negatively predicted support for cooperative/other-regarding responses to the pandemic while RWA positively predicted support for all the conformist/norm-enforcing responses that we measured. Second, RWA positively predicted concern across different domains of pandemic threats while SDO showed a negative association with concern specifically about others (that is, empathic concern). Third, RWA (and not SDO) increased following the outbreak of the pandemic and the magnitude of this effect was predicted by the perceived pandemic threat (concern about the pandemic). These effects persist after accounting for a range of covariates. Below, we discuss each finding in turn.

(1) Cooperative/other-regarding and conformist/norm-enforcing attitudes about COVID-19.

As expected, people low in SDO were more likely to display cooperative/other-regarding COVID-19 attitudes and policy preferences, while RWA was unrelated to these measures. By contrast, RWA positively predicted support for conformist/norm-enforcing responses to the pandemic. This relationship held across all types of conformist/norm-enforcing responses, including responses that relate most obviously to the social control and group viability concerns, theorized to underlie RWA (and social conservatism generally) – that is, moralistic and punitive attitudes towards rule violators and support for severe government and military enforcement of lockdown rules and strict border control. These relationships held when controlling for left-wing v. right-wing political affiliation, indicating that they are likely not simply an artefact of left-right political polarisation or partisan politics. Hence, as predicted by the Dual Foundations Theory, people low in SDO support COVID-19 responses that appeal to their cooperative/other-regarding preferences to protect the vulnerable while people high in RWA support measures to stop the spread of the virus that is consistent with their general support for social control.

We also found that people low in SDO were likely to support conformist/norm-enforcing responses. However, our follow-up analyses revealed that (1) this related only to support for lockdown rules, which was the only type of conformist/norm-enforcing attitude that was also linked to empathic concern, and (2) SDO mediated the relationship between empathic concern and support for lockdown rules.

(2) Concern about COVID-19.

Our findings regarding concern about the pandemic are also in accordance with the Dual Foundations Theory, confirming an association between RWA and threat sensitivity: RWA predicted increased concern about all the COVID-19 threats that we measured. Thus, while some prior research emphasized that RWA and social conservatism are related to specific domains of threat such as violence or dangerous out-groups (Brandt et al. Reference Brandt2021; Duckitt and Fisher Reference Duckitt and Fisher2003; Sinn Reference Sinn2019) or disease threats (Murray and Schaller Reference Murray and Schaller2012; Terrizzi, Shook, and McDaniel Reference Terrizzi, Shook and McDaniel2013), our results suggest that RWA may be related to more domain-general threat sensitivity. However, this should be interpreted with caution since all the threats we measured are at least indirectly pandemic-related.

The Dual Foundations Theory predicts that SDO should be unrelated to most threats but, in contrast to high-RWA individuals, the greater empathic concern of low-SDO individuals should make them concerned about threats to others. We found some support for this in that people low in SDO tended to be concerned about the pandemic in general and, specifically, the health of others (especially unfamiliar others) rather than threats to their own health and finances or the economy. This suggests that prior work linking political progressivism to concern about the pandemic (Burkova et al. Reference Burkova2021; Calvillo et al. Reference Calvillo2020; Clinton et al. Reference Clinton2021; Collins, Mandel, and Schywiola Reference Collins, Mandel and Schywiola2021; Gadarian, Goodman, and Pepinsky Reference Gadarian, Goodman and Pepinsky2021; Pennycook et al. Reference Pennycook2022; Stroebe et al. Reference Stroebe2021) likely reflects empathic concern (more than general threat sensitivity) among low-SDO progressives.

(3) Change in ideology as a result of COVID-19 threats.

While a small effect, the increase in RWA that we observed following the onset of COVID-19 is consistent with our pre-registered prediction based on the Dual Foundations Theory. It is also consistent with previous longitudinal studies that have also found small shifts, specifically towards authoritarian and socially conservative views in the wake of terrorist attacks in Spanish and Swedish samples (Echebarria-Echabe and Fernández-Guede Reference Echebarria-Echabe and Fernández-Guede2006; Lindén, Björklund, and Bäckström Reference Lindén, Björklund and Bäckström2018), and the COVID-19 pandemic in Polish and US samples (Golec de Zavala et al. Reference Golec de Zavala2020; Pazhoohi and Kingstone Reference Pazhoohi and Kingstone2021). Our findings replicate and extend this work, demonstrating that the effect of a threat on RWA (and the lack of an effect on SDO) generalizes to the UK political context. In addition, our finding that concern about the pandemic (rather than actual exposure to COVID-19 threats) predicted the magnitude of participants' change in RWA suggests that a psychological mechanism driving the shift towards higher RWA is a perceived COVID-19 threat.

Conclusion

Taken together, our results support the Dual Foundations Theory of political ideology (Claessens et al. Reference Claessens2020) and help to explain prior findings and resolve apparent contradictions in the literature. Our findings reveal a divergent pattern of support for COVID-19 responses between high-SDO and high-RWA conservatives, with increased concern about the pandemic and support for compliance with pandemic prevention measures associated with low SDO but high RWA. This alignment between low SDO and high RWA chimes with prior work showing that economic progressives (who tend to score lower on SDO) and social conservatives (who tend to score higher on RWA) are both concerned with protection as opposed to freedom (Federico and Malka Reference Federico and Malka2018; Malka et al. Reference Malka2014; Malka, Lelkes, and Soto Reference Malka, Lelkes and Soto2019). Nevertheless, we show that support for protection, at least in the context of COVID-19, appears to reflect different psychological motives in the two groups. Our theoretical model and results indicate that while low SDO responses to the pandemic are driven by empathy, high RWA responses are driven by a desire for conformity and norm enforcement (that is, social control) and protection of the self and in-group (threat sensitivity).

Our findings indicate that prior work linking progressive ideology on a unidimensional spectrum to increased support for a strong COVID-19 response (Allcott et al. Reference Allcott2020; Burkova et al. Reference Burkova2021; Calvillo et al. Reference Calvillo2020; Christensen et al. Reference Christensen2020; Clinton et al. Reference Clinton2021; Collins, Mandel, and Schywiola Reference Collins, Mandel and Schywiola2021; Conway et al. Reference Conway2021; Gadarian, Goodman, and Pepinsky Reference Gadarian, Goodman and Pepinsky2021; Gollwitzer et al. Reference Gollwitzer2020; Painter and Qiu Reference Painter and Qiu2021; Pennycook et al. Reference Pennycook2022; Stroebe et al. Reference Stroebe2021) reflects an association between low SDO (but not low RWA) and empathic concern for others. Our findings also suggest that, by placing high-RWA conservatives together with high-SDO conservatives (who may be more reluctant to make personal sacrifices and behavioural changes that they see as only benefiting others and negatively impacting the economy), many studies on unidimensional ideology may have masked threat-sensitive, pro-lockdown responses among high-RWA conservatives. The correlation between general support for COVID-19 responses and unidimensional ideology, then, is not a general feature of human psychology, but a product of the interplay between the different motives driving pandemic responses on the left and the right, and the local political context. This is supported by the fact that most studies on unidimensional ideology find less concern among conservatives than progressives in studies conducted in the US, with its unique political climate (for example, particularly strong polarization and Donald Trump who downplayed the threat). When we consider a more diverse sample of populations, the relationship is more mixed (Brouard, Vasilopoulos, and Becher Reference Brouard, Vasilopoulos and Becher2020; Chen, Frey, and Presidente Reference Chen, Frey and Presidente2021; Choma et al. Reference Choma2021; Dryhurst et al. Reference Dryhurst2020; Gelfand et al. Reference Gelfand2021; Manson Reference Manson2020; Mula et al. Reference Mula2022; Passini Reference Passini2022; Pavlovic, Todosijevic, and Stanojevic Reference Pavlovic, Todosijevic and Stanojevic2020; Pennycook et al. Reference Pennycook2022; Samore et al. Reference Samore2021).

Our findings are also consistent with prior work linking conservatism to general threat sensitivity (Feldman Reference Feldman2013; Hibbing, Smith, and Alford Reference Hibbing, Smith and Alford2014; Shaffer and Duckitt Reference Shaffer and Duckitt2013) and the observed strong COVID-19 response among right-leaning countries such as Saudi Arabia (Chen, Frey, and Presidente Reference Chen, Frey and Presidente2021; Gelfand et al. Reference Gelfand2021). We find that RWA increased in the UK following the outbreak of COVID-19 and that support for government COVID-19 responses among conservatives is linked to a focus on conformity and norm enforcement among those high in RWA in particular. Similarly, national-level studies have tended to focus on measures of the conformist (social control) dimension of ideology, such as collectivism and cultural ‘tightness’ (Chen, Frey, and Presidente Reference Chen, Frey and Presidente2021; Gelfand et al. Reference Gelfand2021). The inherent group focus of conformist ideology may also make it a particularly important predictor of response strategy at the national level.

While the size of the ideological effects we observe is at least as large as other demographic predictors of responses to the pandemic, it is worth noting that a considerable portion of the variance in responses remains unexplained in our models. Other factors, such as partisan politics and the fact that conservatives and progressives follow different leaders and sources of information undoubtedly also play a role (Calvillo et al. Reference Calvillo2020; Gollwitzer et al. Reference Gollwitzer2020; Graham et al. Reference Graham2020; Motta, Stecula, and Farhart Reference Motta, Stecula and Farhart2020; Painter and Qiu Reference Painter and Qiu2021; Pennycook et al. Reference Pennycook2022; Ruisch et al. Reference Ruisch2021; Samore et al. Reference Samore2021; Sloan et al. Reference Sloan2021; Stroebe et al. Reference Stroebe2021). Boris Johnson's initial minimisation of the outbreak, followed later by greater support for lockdowns and physical distancing after contracting the disease himself, was likely an important factor shaping views among Conservative voters, for example. However, this cannot account for the divergent effects we observe for SDO and RWA when controlling for political affiliation. Similarly, a recent US-based study found that factors such as lower trust in liberal/moderate/scientific sources of information and higher economic conservatism drove the lack of precautionary behaviours found among socially conservative Republicans and Independents (Samore et al. Reference Samore2021). Crucially, however, when controlling for these factors, socially conservative views do predict greater precautionary behaviours among Republicans and Independents.

Beyond their implications for understanding political ideology, our findings are also likely to be of interest to politicians, policymakers, and communicators seeking to manage responses to pandemics like COVID-19. Effective collective response requires broad political support, which can be undermined if responses become politically polarized. Our findings indicate that support for a strong response to COVID-19 can come from both the political left and right but it rests on two very different psychological drives that we all possess to some degree – a desire for cooperation motivated by empathic concern for others that is stronger among those low in SDO (who tend to be on the political left) and a desire for strict social control to mitigate threats to the self and the nation that is stronger among those high in RWA (who tend to be on the political right). Communicators seeking to generate bipartisan support need to acknowledge and appeal to both sets of concerns, as well as the values held by people high in SDO or low in RWA.

The basic human drives inherent to the way we respond to crises like COVID-19 have deep evolutionary roots – a trade-off between cooperation for the common good and self-interested competition, and a trade-off between group conformity and individual freedom. The dual evolutionary foundations of political ideology provide a principled framework with which to connect these basic social drives to the modern political landscape and individual responses to crises such as the COVID-19 pandemic, connections which we hope can help us unite in tackling the challenges they present.

Supplementary Material

Online appendices are available at https://doi.org/10.1017/S000712342200076X.

Data Availability Statement

Replication data for this article can be found in Harvard Dataverse at: https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/LUFHIS.

Acknowledgements

We wish to thank Scott Claessens and Annemarie Fischer for checking some of the code and for helpful suggestions regarding some of the statistical analyses.

Financial Support

Funding for this study was provided by a Royal Society of New Zealand Marsden Fund grant (to QDA, grant number 17-UOA-074); and a Rutherford Discovery Fellowship (to QDA, grant number 11-UOA-019).

Conflicts of Interest

None.

Ethical Standards

All participants provided informed consent and were compensated for their participation. Ethical approval for this study was granted by the University of Auckland Human Participants Ethics Committee (ref: 023639).

Footnotes

1 The term ‘ideology’ is used in many, sometimes contradictory ways (Converse Reference Converse2006; Jost Reference Jost2006; Jost, Federico, and Napier Reference Jost, Federico and Napier2009), even within political science (Gerring Reference Gerring1997). Here, we build on prior work that considers political ideology in the psychological sense as ‘an interrelated set of moral and political attitudes that possesses cognitive, affective, and motivational components’ (Jost Reference Jost2006, 653).

2 We conceptualize SDO and RWA as proxies for the two dimensions of ideology. Researchers have identified numerous pairs of scales measuring what appear to be the same two underlying ideological dimensions (Claessens et al. Reference Claessens2020; Duckitt and Sibley Reference Duckitt and Sibley2009). The scales can vary in the extent to which they focus on abstract values and attitudes (e.g. traditionalism and egalitarianism) versus more concrete policy preferences (e.g. social and economic conservatism) and the SDO and RWA scales that fall somewhere in between. In introducing the Dual Process Model that was a precursor to the Dual Foundations Theory, Duckitt and Sibley (Reference Duckitt and Sibley2009) reject a characterisation of SDO and RWA as personality measures, describing them as an ideological response that is the product of an interaction between personality and environmental context. Consistent with this, SDO and RWA scales are widely used as measures of ‘ideology’ in psychology, and they correlate highly with, and factor together with, attitudinal measures of economic and social conservatism, respectively (Duckitt and Sibley Reference Duckitt and Sibley2009; Duckitt and Sibley Reference Duckitt, Sibley, Sibley and Barlow2017; Johnston and Ollerenshaw Reference Johnston and Ollerenshaw2020; Stangor and Leary Reference Stangor and Leary2006).

3 We chose Prolific over MTurk because comparisons indicate it has superior data quality and sample diversity (Palan and Schitter Reference Palan and Schitter2018; Peer et al. Reference Peer2017; Peer et al. Reference Peer2022).

4 Participants also answered some questions on COVID-19 not reported here.

5 The wording of the first item was a combination of two items from a recent study (Gadarian, Goodman, and Pepinsky Reference Gadarian, Goodman and Pepinsky2021): ‘The government should waive insurance costs and hospital fees for treating COVID-19’ and ‘Make all testing for COVID-19 free for all Americans’.

References

Allcott, H et al. (2020) Polarization and public health: Partisan differences in social distancing during the coronavirus pandemic. Journal of Public Economics 191, 104254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Asbrock, F and Fritsche, I (2013) Authoritarian reactions to terrorist threat: Who Is being threatened, the me or the we? International Journal of Psychology 48, 3549.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burton, M and Tunnicliffe, R (2022) Membership of Political Parties in Great Britain. London: House of Commons Library.Google Scholar
Beall, AT, Hofer, MK and Schaller, M (2016) Infections and elections: Did an Ebola outbreak influence the 2014 U.S. Federal elections (and If so, How)? Psychological Science 27, 595605.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brandt, MJ et al. (2021) The association between threat and politics depends on the type of threat, the political domain, and the country. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 47, 324–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brouard, S, Vasilopoulos, P and Becher, M (2020) Sociodemographic and psychological correlates of compliance with the COVID-19 public health measures in France. Canadian Journal of Political Science 53, 253258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burkova, VN et al. (2021) Predictors of anxiety in the COVID-19 pandemic from a global perspective: Data from 23 countries. Sustainability (Switzerland) 13, 4017.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Calvillo, DP et al. (2020) Political ideology predicts perceptions of the threat of COVID-19 (and susceptibility to fake news about it). Social Psychological and Personality Science 11, 1119–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, C, Frey, CB and Presidente, G (2021) Culture and contagion: Individualism and compliance with COVID-19 policy. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization 190, 191200.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chiao, JY et al. (2009) Neural basis of preference for human social hierarchy versus egalitarianism. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1167, 174–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Choma, BL et al. (2021) Ideological and psychological predictors of COVID-19-related collective action, opinions, and health compliance across three nations. Journal of Social and Political Psychology 9, 123–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christensen, SR et al. (2020) Political and personal reactions to COVID-19 during initial weeks of social distancing in the United States. PLoS ONE 15, 116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cizmar, AM et al. (2014) Authoritarianism and American political behavior from 1952 to 2008. Political Research Quarterly 67, 7183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Claessens, S et al. (2020) The dual evolutionary foundations of political ideology. Nature Human Behaviour 4, 336–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clinton, J et al. (2021) Partisan pandemic: how partisanship and public health concerns affect individuals’ social mobility during COVID-19. Science Advances 7, 18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collins, RN, Mandel, DR and Schywiola, SS (2021) Political identity over personal impact: early U.S. Reactions to the COVID-19 pandemic. Frontiers in Psychology 12, 118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Converse, PE (2006) The nature of belief systems in mass publics (1964). Critical Review 18, 174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conway, LG, Woodward, SR and Zubrod, A (2020) Social Psychological Measurements of COVID-19: Coronavirus Perceived Threat, Government Response, Impacts, and Experiences Questionnaires. PsyArXiv.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conway, LG et al. (2021) Why are conservatives less concerned about the coronavirus (COVID-19) than liberals? Comparing political, experiential, and partisan messaging explanations. Personality and Individual Differences 183, 111124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costello, TH and Lilienfeld, SO (2021) Social and economic political ideology consistently operate as mutual suppressors: Implications for personality, social, and political psychology. Social Psychological and Personality Science 12, 1425–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, MH (1983) Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 44, 113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dryhurst, S et al. (2020) Risk perceptions of COVID-19 around the world. Journal of Risk Research 23 (7–8), 113.Google Scholar
Duckitt, J and Fisher, K (2003) The impact of social threat on worldview and ideological attitudes. Political Psychology 24, 199222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duckitt, J and Sibley, CG (2009) A dual-process motivational model of ideology, politics, and prejudice. Psychological Inquiry 20, 98109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duckitt, J and Sibley, CG (2017) The dual process motivational model of ideology and prejudice. In Sibley, CG and Barlow, FK (eds), The Cambridge Handbook of the Psychology of Prejudice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 188221.Google Scholar
Duckitt, J et al. (2010) A tripartite approach to right-wing authoritarianism: the authoritarianism-conservatism-traditionalism model. Political Psychology 31, 685715.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duriez, B and Van Hiel, A (2002) The march of modern fascism: A comparison of social dominance orientation and authoritarianism. Personality and Individual Differences 32, 11991213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Echebarria-Echabe, A and Fernández-Guede, E (2006) Effects of terrorism on attitudes and ideological orientation. European Journal of Social Psychology 36, 259–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Everett, JAC et al. (2020) The effectiveness of moral messages on public health behavioral intentions during the COVID-19 pandemic. PsyArXiv Preprints.Google Scholar
Federico, CM and Malka, A (2018) The contingent, contextual nature of the relationship between needs for security and certainty and political preferences: Evidence and implications. Political Psychology 39, 348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Federico, CM et al. (2013) Mapping the connections between politics and morality: The multiple sociopolitical orientations involved in moral intuition. Political Psychology 34, 589610.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feldman, S (2003) Enforcing social conformity: a theory of authoritarianism. Political Psychology 24, 4174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feldman, S (2013) Comments on: authoritarianism in social context: The role of threat. International Journal of Psychology 48, 55–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feldman, S and Johnston, C (2014) Understanding the determinants of political ideology: Implications of structural complexity. Political Psychology 35, 337–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischer, K, Atkinson, QD and Chaudhuri, A (2021) Experiments in political psychology. In Chaudhuri, A (ed.), A Research Agenda for Experimental Economics. Cheltenham, UK and Northampton, MA, USA: Edward Elgar Publishing, pp. 163190.Google Scholar
Fischer, K, Chaudhuri, A and Atkinson, QD (2023) “Replication Data for: ‘Dual Evolutionary Foundations of Political Ideology Predict Divergent Responses to COVID-19’”. https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/LUFHIS, Harvard Dataverse, V1. UNF:6:vf6+ISb1YMhLWe40fW4h1Q== [fileUNF].Google Scholar
Fischer, K, et al. (2023) Left- and Right-Wing Authoritarianism Share a Foundation in Cognitive Rigidity, Conformity, and Threat/Disgust Sensitivity. PsyArXiv.Google Scholar
Gadarian, SK, Goodman, SW and Pepinsky, TB (2021) Partisanship, health behavior, and policy attitudes in the early stages of the COVID-19 pandemic. PLoS ONE 16, 113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gelfand, MJ et al. (2011) Differences between tight and loose cultures: A 33-nation study. Science 332, 1100–04.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gelfand, MJ et al. (2021) The relationship between cultural tightness-looseness and COVID-19 cases and deaths: A global analysis. The Lancet. Planetary health 5196, 110.Google Scholar
Gerring, J (1997) Ideology: a definitional analysis. Political Research Quarterly 50, 957–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Golec de Zavala, A et al. (2020) The COVID-19 pandemic, authoritarianism, and rejection of sexual dissenters in Poland. Psychology of Sexual Orientation and Gender Diversity 8, 250260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gollwitzer, A et al. (2020) Partisan differences in physical distancing predict infections and mortality during the coronavirus pandemic. Nature Human Behaviour 4, 1186–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graham, A et al. (2020) Faith in trump, moral foundations, and social distancing defiance during the coronavirus pandemic. Socius 6, 123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hibbing, JR, Smith, KB and Alford, JR (2014) Differences in negativity bias underlie variations in political ideology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 37, 297307.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ho, AK et al. (2015) The nature of social dominance orientation: Theorizing and measuring preferences for intergroup inequality using the New SDO7 scale. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 109, 1003–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hunsberger, B (1996) Religious fundamentalism, right-wing authoritarianism, and hostility toward homosexuals in non-Christian religious groups. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion 6, 3949.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hutcheson, G and Sofroniou, N (1999) The Multivariate Social Scientist: Introductory Statistics Using Generalized Linear Models. London: Sage Publications.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Inbar, Y et al. (2016) Can a naturally occurring pathogen threat change social attitudes? Evaluations of gay men and lesbians during the 2014 Ebola epidemic. Social Psychological and Personality Science 7, 420–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnston, CD and Ollerenshaw, T (2020) How different are cultural and economic ideology? Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 34, 94101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, DN and Figueredo, AJ (2013) The core of darkness: Uncovering the heart of the dark triad. European Journal of Personality 27, 521–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jost, JT (2006) The end of the end of ideology. American Psychologist 61, 651–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jost, JT, Federico, CM and Napier, JL (2009) Political ideology: Its structure, functions, and elective affinities. Annual Review of Psychology 60, 307–37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jost, JT et al. (2017) The politics of fear: Is there an ideological asymmetry in existential motivation? Social Cognition 35, 324–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jost, JT et al. (2018) Ideological asymmetries in conformity, desire for shared reality, and the spread of misinformation. Current Opinion in Psychology 23, 7783.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaiser, HF (1960) The application of electronic computers to factor analysis. Educational and Psychological Measurement 20, 141–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karwowski, M et al. (2020) When in danger, turn right: Does COVID-19 threat promote social conservatism and right-wing presidential candidates? Human Ethology 35, 3748.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lindén, M, Björklund, F and Bäckström, M (2018) How a terror attack affects right-wing authoritarianism, social dominance orientation, and their relationship to torture attitudes. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 59, 547–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lönnqvist, JE and Kivikangas, M (2019) Economic attitudes, social attitudes and their psychological underpinnings – A study of the Finnish political elite. Frontiers in Psychology 10, 111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malka, A, Lelkes, Y and Soto, CJ (2019) Are cultural and economic conservatism positively correlated? A large-scale cross-national test. British Journal of Political Science 49, 1045–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malka, A et al. (2014) Do needs for security and certainty predict cultural and economic conservatism? A cross-national analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 106, 1031–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manson, JH (2020) Right-wing authoritarianism, left-wing authoritarianism, and pandemic-mitigation authoritarianism. Personality and Individual Differences 167, 110251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McKee, IR and Feather, NT (2008) Revenge, retribution, and values: Social attitudes and punitive sentencing. Social Justice Research 21, 138–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mellon, J, Bailey, J and Prosser, C (2021) How do coronavirus attitudes fit into Britain's ideological landscape? Parliamentary Affairs 74, 597616.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Motta, M, Stecula, D and Farhart, C (2020) How right-leaning media coverage of COVID-19 facilitated the spread of misinformation in the early stages of the pandemic in the U. S. Canadian Journal of Political Science 53, 335342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mula, S et al. (2022) Concern with COVID-19 pandemic threat and attitudes towards immigrants: The mediating effect of the desire for tightness. Current Research in Ecological and Social Psychology 3, 100028.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, DR and Schaller, M (2012) Threat(s) and conformity deconstructed: Perceived threat of infectious disease and its implications for conformist attitudes and behavior. European Journal of Social Psychology 42, 180188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muthukrishna, M and Henrich, J (2019) A problem in theory. Nature Human Behaviour 3, 221–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Painter, M and Qiu, T (2021) Political beliefs affect compliance with government mandates. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization 185, 688701.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palan, S and Schitter, C (2018) Prolific.Ac – A subject pool for online experiments. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Finance 17, 22–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Passini, S (2020) The relationship between value and ideological orientations using the refined theory of basic values. Journal of Social and Political Psychology 8, 708–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Passini, S (2022) Songs and flags: Concern for COVID-19 and submission to authority. Personality and Individual Differences 185, 111251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pavlovic, Z, Todosijevic, B and Stanojevic, D (2020) Support for the measures in fighting the COVID-19 pandemic: the role of political ideology. Psihologija 54, 207222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pazhoohi, F and Kingstone, A (2021) Associations of political orientation, xenophobia, right-wing authoritarianism, and concern of COVID-19: Cognitive responses to an actual pathogen threat. Personality and Individual Differences 182, 111081.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peer, E et al. (2017) Beyond the Turk: Alternative platforms for crowdsourcing behavioral research. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 70, 153–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peer, E et al. (2022) Data quality of platforms and panels for online behavioral research. Behavior Research Methods 54, 16431662.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pennycook, G et al. (2022) Beliefs about COVID-19 in Canada, the United Kingdom, and the United States: A novel test of political polarization and motivated reasoning. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 48, 750–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perry, R and Sibley, CG (2013) A dual-process motivational model of social and economic policy attitudes. Analyses of Social Issues and Public Policy 13, 262–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pfattheicher, S et al. (2020) The emotional path to action: Empathy promotes physical distancing and wearing of face masks during the COVID-19 pandemic. Psychological Science 31, 1363–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Politi, E et al. (2021) No man is an island: psychological underpinnings of Prosociality in the midst of the COVID-19 outbreak. Personality and Individual Differences 171, 110534.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pratto, F et al. (1994) Social dominance orientation: A personality variable predicting social and political attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 67, 741–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
R Core Team (2021) R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.Google Scholar
Rosenfeld, DL and Tomiyama, AJ (2021) Can a pandemic make people more socially conservative? Political ideology, gender roles, and the case of COVID-19. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 51, 425–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruisch, BC et al. (2021) Examining the left-right divide through the lens of a global crisis: Ideological differences and their implications for responses to the COVID-19 pandemic. Political Psychology 42, 795816.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sääksvuori, L, Mappes, T and Puurtinen, M (2011) Costly punishment prevails in intergroup conflict. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 278, 3428–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Samore, T et al. (2021) Of pathogens and party lines: social conservatism positively associates with COVID-19 precautions among U.S. Democrats but not republicans. PLoS ONE 16, e0253326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shaffer, B and Duckitt, J (2013) The dimensional structure of people's fears, threats, and concerns and their relationship with right-wing authoritarianism and social dominance orientation. International Journal of Psychology 48, 617.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sidanius, J et al. (2013) You're inferior and not worth our concern: The interface between empathy and social dominance orientation. Journal of Personality 81, 313–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sinn, JS (2019) Mapping ideology: Combining the Schwartz value circumplex with evolutionary theory to explain ideological differences. Evolutionary Psychological Science 5, 4457.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sloan, MM et al. (2021) Pandemic emotions: The extent, correlates, and mental health consequences of fear of COVID-19. Sociological Spectrum 41, 369–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stamkou, E et al. (2019) Cultural collectivism and tightness moderate responses to norm violators: Effects on power perception, moral emotions, and leader support. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 45, 947–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stangor, C and Leary, SP (2006) Intergroup beliefs: Investigations from the social side. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology 38, 243–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stroebe, W et al. (2021) Politicization of COVID-19 health-protective behaviors in the United States: Longitudinal and cross-national evidence. PLoS ONE 16, e0256740.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Terrizzi, JA, Shook, NJ and McDaniel, MA (2013) The behavioral immune system and social conservatism: A meta-analysis. Evolution and Human Behavior 34, 99108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tingley, D et al. (2014) Mediation: R package for causal mediation analysis. Journal of Statistical Software 59, 138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tiokhin, L and Hruschka, D (2017) No evidence that an Ebola outbreak influenced voting preferences in the 2014 elections after controlling for time-series autocorrelation: A commentary on Beall, Hofer, and Schaller (2016). Psychological Science 28, 1358–60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M et al. (2012) Two key steps in the evolution of human cooperation: The interdependence hypothesis. Current Anthropology 53, 673–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Treier, S and Hillygus, DS (2009) The nature of political ideology in the contemporary electorate. Public Opinion Quarterly 73, 679703.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Triandis, HC and Gelfand, MJ (1998) Converging measurement of horizontal and vertical individualism and collectivism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 74, 118–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tybur, JM et al. (2016) Parasite stress and pathogen avoidance relate to distinct dimensions of political ideology across 30 nations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 113, 12408–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Summary of principal components analysis results for the conformist/norm-enforcement items (n = 425) including pattern matrix, eigenvalues, proportion of variance, and Cronbach's alpha for each principal component

Figure 1

Figure 1. Results for linear regressions with social dominance orientation (SDO) and right-wing authoritarianism (RWA) controlling for each other, predicting cooperative/other-regarding and conformist/norm-enforcing attitudes about COVID-19.Notes: We control for age, sex, race, socioeconomic status, left-right political affiliation, and pandemic threat exposure in all models. See supplementary Table S3 for more detailed results, including covariate effects.

Figure 2

Figure 2. Results for linear regressions with social dominance orientation (SDO) and right-wing authoritarianism (RWA), controlling for each other, predicting different types of conformist/norm-enforcing attitudes revealed by principal components analysis (PC1–4 = Principal Component 1–4).Notes: We control for age, sex, race, socioeconomic status, left-right political affiliation, and pandemic threat exposure in all models. See supplementary Table S4 for more detailed results, including covariate effects.

Figure 3

Figure 3. Results for linear regressions with social dominance orientation (SDO) and right-wing authoritarianism (RWA) controlling for each other, predicting concerned/threat-sensitive attitudes about COVID-19.Notes: We control for age, sex, race, socioeconomic status, left-right political affiliation, and pandemic threat exposure in all models. See supplementary Table S8 for more detailed results, including covariate effects.

Supplementary material: Link

Fischer et al. Dataset

Link
Supplementary material: File

Fischer et al. supplementary material

Fischer et al. supplementary material

Download Fischer et al. supplementary material(File)
File 2.2 MB