Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-l7hp2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T16:10:58.795Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Working memory multicomponent model outcomes in individuals with traumatic brain injury: Critical review and meta-analysis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 November 2024

Bar Lambez*
Affiliation:
Loewenstein Rehabilitation Center, Raanana, Israel Department of Psychology and Gonda Multidisciplinary Brain Research Center, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat-Gan, Israel
Eli Vakil
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Gonda Multidisciplinary Brain Research Center, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat-Gan, Israel
Philippe Azouvi
Affiliation:
AP-HP, GH Paris Saclay, Hôpital Raymond Poincaré, service de Médecine Physique et de Réadaptation, boulevard Raymond Poincaré, Garches, France Equipe INSERM DevPsy, CESP, UMR, Université Paris-Saclay, UVSQ, France
Claire Vallat-Azouvi
Affiliation:
Laboratoire DysCo, University of Paris-8-Saint-Denis, 2, rue de la Liberté, Saint-Denis, France Antenne UEROS- UGECAMIDF, Raymond-Poincaré Hospital, 104, boulevard Raymond-Poincaré, Garches, France
*
Corresponding author: Bar Lambez; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective:

Traumatic Brain Injury (TBI) often leads to cognitive impairments, particularly regarding working memory (WM). This meta-analysis aims to examine the impact of TBI on WM, taking into account moderating factors which has received little attention in previous research, such as severity of injury, the different domains of Baddeley’s multi-component model, and the interaction between these two factors, as well as the interaction with other domains of executive functions.

Method:

Following Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analyses guidelines, a systematic review and meta-analysis searched Google Scholar, PubMed, and PsycNET for studies with objective WM measures. Multiple meta-analyses were performed to compare the effects of TBI severity on different WM components. Twenty-four English, peer-reviewed articles, mostly cross-sectional were included.

Results:

TBI significantly impairs general WM and all Baddeley’s model components, most notably the Central Executive (d’ = 0.74). Severity categories, mild-moderate and moderate-severe, were identified. Impairment was found across severities, with “moderate-severe” demonstrating the largest effect size (d’ = 0.81). Individuals with moderate-severe TBI showed greater impairments in the Central Executive and Episodic Buffer compared to those with mild-moderate injury, whereas no such differences were found for the Phonological Loop and Visuospatial Sketchpad.

Conclusions:

These findings enhance our understanding of WM deficits in varying severities of TBI, highlighting the importance of assessing and treating WM in clinical practice and intervention planning.

Type
Critical Review
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of International Neuropsychological Society

Introduction

Traumatic brain injury (TBI) causes a wide range of emotional, behavioral and cognitive impairments (Andelic et al., Reference Andelic, Sigurdardottir, Schanke, Sandvik, Sveen and Roe2010; Azouvi et al., Reference Azouvi, Arnould, Dromer and Vallat-Azouvi2017), particularly in the domain of memory (Vakil, Reference Vakil2005), and executive functions (EF) (Jourdan et al., Reference Jourdan, Bayen, Pradat-Diehl, Ghout, Darnoux, Azerad and Azouvi2016; Ruet et al., Reference Ruet, Jourdan, Bayen, Darnoux, Sahridj, Ghout and Azouvi2018). Working memory (WM), defined as a control cognitive process, including a limited-capacity system responsible for temporary maintenance and storage of information, is one memory domain greatly affected by TBI (Dunning et al., Reference Dunning, Westgate and Adlam2016).

There are several WM models, however the most cited is the multicomponent model of Baddeley and Hitch (Baddeley, Reference Baddeley2003; Baddeley et al., Reference Baddeley, Allen and Hitch2018; Baddeley, Reference Baddeley2000; Baddeley & Hitch, Reference Baddeley, Hitch and G. H.1974), including four components: Central Executive, Phonological Loop, Visuo-spatial Sketchpad and the Episodic Buffer (Table 1). The Central Executive is responsible for simultaneous visual and verbal information storage, coordination and processing, as well as for attentional control (Baddeley & Della, Reference Baddeley and Della1996). The Phonological Loop and Visuo-spatial Sketchpad are two slave systems with limited capacity storage. The Phonological Loop stores short-term verbal or auditory material, including both storage and articulatory rehearsal components. The Visuo-spatial Sketchpad stores spatial or visual information.

Table 1. Components of Baddeley’s Working Memory model. The table presents the main functions of each one of the four components of the model, of the main tasks used to assess each component, and of the corresponding brain areas

The Episodic Buffer is the last WM component presented in the extended model (Baddeley, Reference Baddeley2020). It is responsible for integrating multi-modal information from all the latter three components with long-term memory (both semantic and episodic) and reconstructing multi-modal representation. In contrast to the other components, it has been much less studied. A meta-analysis on WM in children after TBI found no study regarding the Episodic Buffer (Phillips et al., Reference Phillips, Parry, Mandalis and Lah2017).

WM in general has been argued to be supported by a network of brain regions, but relying mainly on the prefrontal cortex (D’Esposito, Reference D’Esposito2007). Research has demonstrated that in parallel, each WM component separately uses additional brain areas (Baddeley, Reference Baddeley2020) (see Table 1 for more details). As WM and its components rely heavily on the frontal lobes, it is to be expected that after TBI, WM functioning will be greatly compromised, due to the vulnerability of prefrontal areas in this condition. Indeed, patients who sustained TBI have been found to perform worse compared to controls on a large variety of WM tasks, such as n-back (Sanchez-Carrion et al., Reference Sanchez-Carrion, Fernandez-Espejo, Junque, Falcon, Bargallo, Roig, Bernabeu, Tormos and Vendrell2008), dual-task (Allain et al., Reference Allain, Etcharry-Bouyx and Le Gall2001; Asloun et al., Reference Asloun, Soury, Couillet, Giroire, Joseph, Mazaux and Azouvi2008; Couillet et al., Reference Couillet, Soury, Lebornec, Asloun, Joseph, Mazaux and Azouvi2010), serial span (McAllister et al., Reference McAllister, Flashman, Sparling and Saykin2004; Perlstein et al., Reference Perlstein, Cole, Demery, Seignourel, Dixit, Larson and Briggs2004) and especially in tasks with high loads and pressure (Mangels et al., Reference Mangels, Craik, Levine, Schwartz and Stuss2002; Withaar et al., Reference Withaar, Stowe, Withaar and Hoeks2000). Earlier studies have suggested that WM deficits are mainly due to impairment of the Central Executive component. For example, compared to a control group, patients with severe TBI were found to have only marginal impairments in slave system functioning, but a significant deficit in Central Executive (Vallat-Azouvi et al., Reference Vallat-Azouvi, Weber, Legrand and Azouvi2007). This finding can be related to executive functioning deficits which are frequently observed after severe TBI (Kavé & Heinik, Reference Kavé and Heinik2017; Larson et al., Reference Larson, Clayson and Farrer2012). Indeed, Central Executive tasks are strongly related to executive functioning processes, particularly flexibility and shifting (Lehto, Reference Lehto2018), with mutual associations with the prefrontal cortex (Goldman-Rakic, Reference Goldman-rakic2012).

Recent systematic reviews and meta-analyses have examined the impairments of WM in individuals after TBI (Dunning et al., Reference Dunning, Westgate and Adlam2016; Phillips et al., 2015). However, findings were limited specifically in relation to the impact of different TBI severity on each of the model components (Bryer et al., Reference Bryer, Medaglia, Rostami and Hillary2013; Dunning et al., Reference Dunning, Westgate and Adlam2016; Forbes et al., Reference Forbes, Carrick, McIntosh and Lawrie2009; Nee et al., Reference Nee, Brown, Askren, Berman, Demiralp, Krawitz and Jonides2013). The present study aims to deepen our understanding of WM impairments after TBI, while addressing these limitations.

The main research questions were as follows: (1) Which individual WM component (i.e., Central Executive, Phonological Loop, Visuospatial Sketchpad, or Episodic Buffer) is most affected following TBI? (2) Does the overall impairment in WM (ignoring individual components) vary as a function of TBI severity? (3) How do these two variables interact? In other words, are different WM subcomponents affected differently by injury severity (for example is the effect of injury severity larger on the Central Executive than on slave systems?). (4) Are Central Executive deficits related to more general deficits in EF? To address these questions, multiple meta-analyses were performed, using data retrieved from the relevant studies. First, we hypothesized that patients with TBI would demonstrate poorer WM functioning as compared to controls, regardless of injury severity and WM component. Second, we hypothesized that greater injury severity would have larger effects on WM functioning. Furthermore, based on previous studies (Vallat-Azouvi et al., Reference Vallat-Azouvi, Weber, Legrand and Azouvi2007) we expected some WM subcomponents would be more sensitive to TBI than others: individuals with greater TBI severity were expected to have a greater deficit of the Central Executive with a less severe deficit in Slave Systems, compared to the less severely affected patients. By contrast, patients with mild to moderate TBI were not expected to present such a dissociation between the Central Executive and the Slave systems. We hypothesized this dissociation could be partially explained by greater executive functioning deficits after severe TBI. Lastly, as little research has been conducted on the Episodic Buffer, we did not have any a priori hypothesis regarding the effects of severity on this component.

Materials and methods

Literature search and selection criteria

The study was designed to summarize and synthesize research evidence appropriately, in order to inform proper practice and policy (Cook et al., Reference Cook, Mulrow and Haynes1997). We have developed the study protocol according to the guidelines of Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analyses Protocol (PRISMA-P) (Moher et al., Reference Moher, Liberati, Tetzlaff and Altman2009; Shamseer et al., Reference Shamseer, Moher, Clarke, Ghersi, Liberati, Petticrew, Shekelle and Stewart2015). In addition, the meta-analysis was prospectively registered with the PROSPERO International prospective register of systematic reviews (registration number: CRD42023397052).

Our study contained four stages: literature identification, screening, selection, and analysis. Literature selection was based on strict eligibility criteria, yielding a set of studies included in the present meta-analysis. Literature identification consisted of a vast literature search for papers published from January 1990 until January 2023. We used the Google Scholar, PubMed and PsycNET search engines to search within abstracts for the terms Working memory AND TBI. To better control publication bias, we searched Google Scholar for additional articles that may have been missed on the initial search. We also included the additional Working memory multi-component, phonological loop, central executive, visuospatial sketchpad, episodic buffer and injury severity search terms. All searches were conducted in English. Literature was also sourced from previously published literature reviews, including the aforementioned published literature reviews (Bryer et al., Reference Bryer, Medaglia, Rostami and Hillary2013; Dunning et al., Reference Dunning, Westgate and Adlam2016; Eierud et al., Reference Eierud, Craddock, Fletcher, Aulakh, King-Casas, Kuehl and Laconte2014; Iverson, Reference Iverson2010; Königs et al., Reference Königs, Engenhorst and Oosterlaan2016; Phillips et al., 2015; Rohling et al., Reference Rohling, Binder, Demakis, Larrabee, Ploetz and Langhinrichsen-Rohling2011; Zhu et al., Reference Zhu, Ling and Ding2019). Following initial extraction, TBI severity was divided into two main categories: Mild-Moderate and Moderate-Severe. Ideally, we would have liked to have three distinct groups: Mild, Moderate, and Severe. Unfortunately, in all studies reviewed in this Meta-analysis, patient groups were categorized either as Mild-Moderate or Moderate-Severe. Though Moderate severity is included in both severity groups, it is reasonable to assume that the Moderate-Severe are more severely impaired than the Mild-Moderate group. Working Memory components were divided into four categories: Central executive, Phonological loop, Visuospatial sketchpad, and Episodic buffer. An additional domain of Executive function was also included. The rationale was to compare the effect of TBI on the Central Executive to its effect on other domains of EF, namely information generation (assessed with verbal fluency) and mental flexibility (assessed with tasks such as the Trail Making Test-B), which are assumed to be partly related, although independent of WM functioning. No Ethical Approval was needed for this study.

Inclusion criteria

The studies included had working memory outcomes tested on participants medically diagnosed with TBI (a change in brain function, or other evidence of brain damage/pathology by an external trauma or blow to the head, via brain imaging techniques; see Menon, Schwab, Wright, & Maas, 2010), with information about severity such as Glasgow Coma Scale (GCS), duration of post-traumatic amnesia (PTA), duration of loss of consciousness (LOC) and medical records based on clinical observation (Levin et al., Reference Levin, Goldstein, High and Williams1988; Williamson et al., Reference Williamson, Scott, Adams, Adams and Parsons2004). We included studies regardless of TBI cause, mechanism of injury and medication. All studies were in English and peer-reviewed, including adult participants in an age range of 20–65 (in order to control for the confounding effect of aging on cognition). The outcomes had to be examined as the effect of TBI on at least one of the four distinct aforementioned WM components of Baddeley’s WM model (Baddeley, Reference Baddeley2000). Additionally, the outcomes had to be measured by objective and validated neuropsychological tests or paradigms. In an attempt to ensure that we are including in each test category compatible tests, valid tests and paradigms were either standardized tests or clearly described paradigms, reflecting specifically the studied WM component. Papers included utilized normative data or statistical information which could be used to generate statistical analysis of group difference through effect size (d-value). All studies must also have included a control group/comparison group with matched cases and controls on age, education and gender. To this end, we included both longitudinal and cross-sectional designs, as the number of papers was limited.

Exclusion criteria

We did not include studies evaluating the effect of TBI on emotional, physiological or neurological functions. Additional excluded studies involved adults with TBI severity who did not report outcomes separately by severity, or studies with general WM score without scores of a distinct WM component or detailed assessment tool.

Meta-analysis procedure

From the primary and broad search terms we reviewed, 852 studies in English were retrieved. Studies in foreign languages and Duplications were removed (827) before narrowing down to studies with cognitive assessment of WM functions in distinct TBI severity (743 removed). The magnitude of search terms allowed us to ensure all relevant studies were identified, even if their abstract did not necessarily include WM or cognitive assessment as a keyword. Eighty-four publications which reviewed WM effects after TBI were identified and carefully reviewed. The majority of the eliminated studies did not have a separate WM component assessment or did not have a matched control group (by age, gender and education). Additional exclusion reasons included inability to access relevant data such as descriptive statistics or calculated effect sizes. Twenty-four studies were deemed to fulfill all inclusion and exclusion criteria (from unique publications). Because of the clinical sample, all studies used cross-sectional designs. Two studies also used longitudinal designs (Kumar et al., Reference Kumar, Rao, Chandramouli and Pillai2013; Sanchez-Carrion et al., Reference Sanchez-Carrion, Fernandez-Espejo, Junque, Falcon, Bargallo, Roig, Bernabeu, Tormos and Vendrell2008). Twenty-three studies recruited control groups of matched age, gender, and education. One study (Johansson et al., Reference Johansson, Berglund and Rnnbck2009) had two distinct TBI severity groups and an additional control group, thus the data from the control group were included twice (with just half the stated sample size). An additional study recruited matched comparison of distinct and varying TBI severity (Zimmermann et al., Reference Zimmermann, Mograbi, Hermes-Pereira, Fonseca and Prigatano2017). From each study, we calculated and included all significant and non-significant results. Three studies were included from analysis of previous meta-analyses and the rest were from additional searches. Asterisks in the reference section are used to identify studies included in the meta-analyses, and Figure 1 provides an overview of the search process.

Figure 1. Flow chart of the literature search process according to Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analyses guidelines.

Study characteristics

There were a total of 2114 participants across studies: 828 participants with TBI and 1286 controls. The average age at testing was 33.4 years, range 20.3–64.11. Gender breakdown was reported in all studies accept two (53% males).

The determination of TBI severity across studies was done using a single or a combination of indicators: GCS, duration of LOC, duration of PTA, medical examination. There were 14 studies with mild-moderate patients with TBI (462 participants) and 12 studies with moderate-severe patients with TBI (366 participants).

Eighteen studies (75%) reported the mean time since injury, which ranged from 0.17 to 31.56 years. Sixteen of these studies were cross-sectional. All studies recruited participants at variable lengths of time post injury. The two longitudinal studies examined participants at fixed time periods across the first 10 months post injury.

Quality assessment

The quality of the data in the included studies was assessed by two reviewers (BL and CVA), using the National Institutes of Health (NIH) quality assessment tools (NIH, 2014). NIH quality assessment tool was chosen as it is more comprehensive, thus enabling us an extensive quality assessment of all included studies.

Data coding

Statistical data was collected from each study including participant number, means, and SD for both groups. The results of each working memory test were categorized according to the WM component it tested.

Group means and SD of neuropsychological WM scores were recorded. If these studies included results of tests assessing executive function domain, they were also recorded.

In case a study had multiple scores for one WM component or executive functioning domain, we first extracted all the data and then chose the score of the most commonly cited tests for each measure. Thus, for each published study it was possible to have five effect sizes. Finally, for multiple effect sizes in a specific component or domain derived from equally relevant tests, the highest effect size was used reflecting the most robust effect (Lopez-Lopez et al., 2018). The following variables were included in a coding sheet: study identification number, study name and authors, year of publication, number of total participants, number of clinical and non-clinical participants with gender characteristics, TBI severity of clinical sample, number of participants in each group, age at testing, time from injury and age at injury, years of education, the mean age and age range of participants, language of testing, and which cognitive tests were used for each working memory component and executive functioning domain. We created a coding sheet with all tests and results, as well as for multiple test results for a single component. However, following the method detailed above we created a final coding sheet including one component score (M and SD) for each WM component or executive function tested within a single study. All data from the studies were extracted independently by the authors and coded. Inter-rater reliability was 100%. All data were extracted from papers.

Statistical analyses

Analyses of effect sizes were calculated according to Cohen (Cohen, Reference Cohen2013), due to usage of comparison groups with different sample sizes, thus requiring us to weight the estimation of the sample sizes. Effect-size calculation was based on the recommended formula: mean clinical group minus the mean control group change divided by the pooled SD with a bias adjustment (Cohen, Reference Cohen2013). Effect-size calculations were first carried out using the “psychometrica” website (Lenhard, Reference Lenhard2017) and then entered into an Excel sheet for total effect sizes and homogeneous effect calculations.

Studies with insufficient and missing data were excluded from the meta-analysis. Given the heterogeneity of TBI sample characteristics, and WM assessments within domains in the included studies, we chose a priori to use random-effects models (Field & Gillett, Reference Field and Gillett2010).

Effect sizes were also calculated according to Rosenthal (Reference Rosenthal1991). In order to assess homogeneity/heterogeneity, we examined the data using the Q test (Sanchez-Meca & Fulgencio, Reference Sanchez-Meca and Fulgencio1997; Shadish & Haddock, Reference Shadish, Haddock, Cooper and Hedges1994) and I 2 (Higgins & Thompson, Reference Higgins and Thompson2002). Accordingly, if the Q value was not significant, then the effect sizes were considered homogeneous, and the mean effect size was considered the best estimation for the data. However, if the Q is significant, moderators should be suggested, as the effect sizes were considered to be heterogeneous. Additionally, as Q test’s ability to detect heterogeneity is limited, we combined the use of I 2 to determine the degree of heterogeneity. Alternatively, I 2 statistic quantifies the proportion of total variation among study results attributable to heterogeneity rather than sampling error. Higher I 2 values signify greater heterogeneity. Although there is no fixed threshold for interpretation, PRISMA guidelines suggest 0–25% indicates low heterogeneity; 25–50% implies moderate heterogeneity; 50–75% suggests high heterogeneity; and 75–100% indicates very high heterogeneity.

Results

Overall WM functioning

Our final meta-analysis included 24 studies and 25 distinct group comparisons, which yielded 66 effect sizes: 53 for the four WM components and 13 for executive functioning domain. Out of the WM components, the Central Executive was the most frequently assessed (n = 20). The Phonological Loop was assessed in sixteen studies (n = 16), the Visuo-spatial Sketchpad was assessed in 12 and the Episodic Buffer in five studies.

Initially the overall WM impairment in TBI was entered into a meta-analysis comprising all the published studies. The results indicated TBI participants had significantly poorer WM functioning than controls, with a medium-large effect size of 0.74. Results were deemed homogeneous, generating a Q value of 14.16, (p < 0.05) and I 2 = 0. A funnel plot of included studies did not show any asymmetry, an indication that significant publication bias was not likely (Figure 2).

Figure 2. Funnel plot showing effect sizes for interventions delineated by sample size.

Assessment of the different subcomponents of WM

Therefore, the second stage was to determine which WM component was most impaired following TBI, and whether each component could be categorized as having a homogenic, significant effect. Studies were divided into four meta-analyses reviewing the different WM components; all model components categories demonstrated moderating, homogeneous, and significant results. As can be seen in Table 2, the Central Executive component was affected most significantly, demonstrating the highest effect size of 0.74 (df = 19, 95% CIL = 0.51, 1.04, Q = 12.13, p < 0.05, I 2 = 0). The Episodic Buffer component demonstrated the lowest effect size of 0.49 (df = 4, 95% CIL = 0.07, 1.04); and was deemed homogeneous (Q = 1.37, p < 0.05, I 2 = 0). For both Slave system components, we have found moderate effect sizes: 0.64 for Phonological Loop component (df = 15, 95% CIL = 0.58, 0.86) and 0.54 for Visuo-spatial Sketchpad (df = 11, 95% CIL = 0.49, 0.89). Both were deemed to be homogeneous (Phonological Loop - Q = 14.03, p<0.05, I 2 = 0; Q = 15.49, p < 0.05, I 2 = 29.0).

Table 2. Effect size and methodology of studies sorted by working memory domains

Influence of injury severity

At the third stage, following comparison of WM deficits across the various components, we investigated the second factor: injury severity. Which severity level could be classified as having the largest WM deficits, regardless of WM component, and whether each severity could be classified as having a homogeneous effect? As in most studies, meta-analyses run on injury severity included Mild-Severe severity. When studies were separated into two meta-analyses reviewing the two levels of injury severity (Mild-Moderate TBI, n = 14; Moderate-Severe, n = 12), homogeneous results were generated. Moderate-severe injury severity demonstrated the greatest effect size, 0.80, (df = 11, 95% CIL = 0.51, 1.06), which was homogeneous across WM components (Q = 8.72, p < 0.05, I 2 = 0). Mild-moderate TBI demonstrated a lower but moderate effect size, 0.65 (df = 13, 95% CIL = 0.53, 0.97, although still homogeneous (Q = 9.81, p < 0.05, I 2 = 0). The smaller variability of each severity level suggests that injury severity is also a moderator affecting WM functioning (see Table 3).

Table 3. Effect size and methodology of studies sorted by injury severity categories

Interactions between injury severity and subcomponents of WM

The fourth stage of the current study was to test the hypothesis that there is an interaction between the two factors: is the deficit in particular WM components affected by the injury severity? All interactions demonstrated homogeneous and significant results (see Figure 3). Central Executive demonstrated the highest effect size for Moderate-Severe TBI of 0.87, (df = 9, 95% CIL = 0.41, 1.3) but a moderate effect size for Mild-Moderate TBI of 0.58 (df = 8, 95% CIL = 0.55, 0.9). Overall, studies were deemed to be homogeneous (Mild-Moderate- Q = 6.84, p < 0.05, I 2 = 0; Moderate-Severe- Q = 3.27, p < 0.05, I 2 = 0). The Slave system components (i.e., Phonological Loop and Visuo-spatial Sketchpad) both had moderate effect sizes for the different severities; the Phonological Loop component in Mild-Moderate TBI demonstrated a slightly lower effect size of 0.59 (df = 9, 95% CIL = 0.59, 0.85) than in Moderate-Severe TBI - 0.66 (df = 6, 95% CIL = 0.48, 0.83). Both results were assumed to be homogeneous (Mild-Moderate - Q = 8.77, p<0.05, I 2 = 0; Moderate-Severe- Q = 6.23, p < 0.05, I 2 = 3.78). The Visuo-spatial Sketchpad component showed similar effect sizes on both severity levels; Mild-Moderate TBI demonstrated an effect size of 0.54 (df = 5, 95% CIL = 0.54, 0.95) and Moderate-Severe TBI demonstrated 0.51 (df = 6, 95% CIL = 0.14, 0.81). Both results were deemed homogeneous (Mild-Moderate -Q = 8.83, p < 0.05, I 2 = 43.37; Moderate-Severe- Q = 4.18, p < 0.05, I 2 = 0). The Episodic Buffer component demonstrated interesting results with a small effect size for Mild-moderate TBI of 0.28, (df = 1, 95% CIL = -0.37, 0.98) but moderate effect size for Moderate-Severe TBI of 0.64 (df = 2, 95% CIL = 0.01, 1.17). Overall, studies were assumed to be homogeneous (Mild-Moderate- Q = 0.06, p < 0.05, I 2 = 0; Moderate-Severe- Q = 0.45, p < 0.05, I 2 = 0, see 43).

Figure 3. Forest plot showing effect sizes and confidence intervals for the various working memory domains for different injury severity categories.

Relationships between WM and EF

First (Figure 4), we found executive functioning was largely affected, demonstrating a high effect size of 0.86 (df = 12, 95% CIL = 0.51, 0.83). However, results were not deemed homogeneous, generating a Q value of 15.33, (p > 0.05) and I 2 = 21.72 which reflects the presence of one or more moderators. Two potential moderators were deemed to be TBI severity and executive functioning domain (fluency vs. flexibility). The interaction between executive functioning and severity revealed similar results: executive functioning demonstrated an effect size for Mild-Moderate TBI of 0.85 (df = 7, 95% CIL = 0.41, 0.85) and for Moderate-Severe of 0.88 (df = 5, 95% CIL = 0.55, 0.9; Q = 2.91, p < 0.05, I 2 = 0). The executive function in Mild-moderate TBI was not deemed to be homogeneous (Q = 11.55, p > 0.05, I 2 = 39.4), suggesting the presence of the second executive functioning ability moderator. When studies were separated into two meta-analyses reviewing fluency and flexibility domains of executive functioning, a homogeneous effect size was generated for both. As can be seen in Table 5, Mild-Moderate participants were less affected in executive functioning flexibility, with an effect size of 0.6 generated (Q = 0.43, p < 0.05, I 2 = 0, 95% CIL = −0.34, 0.67), less than in executive functioning fluency, with an effect size of 0.94 (Q = 0.34, p < 0.05, I 2 = 0, 95% CIL = 0.4, 1.49). Similarly, Moderate-Severe participants were less affected in executive functioning flexibility, with an effect size of 0.80 generated (Q = 0.92, p < 0.05, I 2 = 0, 95% CIL = 0.40, 1.16) than in executive functioning fluency, with an effect size of 0.96 (Q = 0.02, p < 0.05, I 2 = 0, 95% CIL = 0.37, 1.58). To summarize, performance in the flexibility domain of EF was moderated by injury severity in a similar way than the functioning of the Central Executive of WM, while the fluency domain, which was more severely impaired, did not appear to be moderated by injury severity, contrary to what was found for WM.

Figure 4. Forest plot showing effect sizes and confidence intervals for central executive working memory, executive functions flexibility and fluency for different injury severity categories.

Table 4. Effect size and methodology of studies sorted by working memory domains and injury severity categories

Table 5. Effect size and methodology of studies sorted by executive function fluency and executive function flexibility memory domains

Discussion

This meta-analysis and review is the first to examine all of the WM components of Baddeley’s’ multi-component model (Baddeley, Reference Baddeley2020) in adult patients with TBI at varying severities. Surprisingly, very few meta-analyses have been conducted on such “multi” topics. Meta-analyses that have been conducted in the field have either focused on schizophrenia (Forbes et al., Reference Forbes, Carrick, McIntosh and Lawrie2009), stroke (Lugtmeijer et al., Reference Lugtmeijer, Lammers, de Haan, de Leeuw and Kessels2020) or pediatric TBI (Phillips et al., 2015), with only three of the components, or examined only one model component on controls (Nee et al., Reference Nee, Brown, Askren, Berman, Demiralp, Krawitz and Jonides2013). Furthermore, even empirical or clinical studies including all WM model components at varying TBI severity, were difficult to find. Therefore, we aimed to investigate the effect of varying TBI severities on all components in Baddeley’s’ “multi-component” model through objective WM outcomes. We were interested in whether WM components were differently affected by the level of TBI severity in adults. We used 24 studies, 21 of which used cross-sectional and three used longitudinal study designs.

The first and most significant finding was that the WM domain as well as all of Baddeley’s model components were impaired following TBI, producing a general homogeneous moderate-large to large effect size. Secondly, WM deficit at moderate-severe TBI showed a large effect size compared to a moderate effect size in mild-moderate TBI. These findings provide statistical validation for conclusions of previous qualitative reviews (Azouvi et al., Reference Azouvi, Arnould, Dromer and Vallat-Azouvi2017; McAllister et al., Reference McAllister, Flashman, Sparling and Saykin2009; Vakil & Greenstein, Reference Vakil and Greenstein2021; Vakil, Reference Vakil2005).

Of the different model components studied, regardless of severity, Central Executive was found to have the largest impairment generating a large effect size. This was followed by Slave System components of both Phonological Loop and Visuo-spatial Sketchpad with moderate effect sizes, and finally with a slightly smaller effect size, of Episodic Buffer. The results regarding Central Executive and Slave System components are consistent with findings that regardless of injury severity, dysfunction in WM is mainly due to impairment in Central Executive (Phillips et al., 2015). Nevertheless, our review shows that the Central Executive has been more extensively studied than Slave System components. Furthermore, to date there has been little literature examining Episodic Buffer in patients with TBI, adding our findings of moderate Episodic Buffer impairment to the recent consolidating formulation of the WM model with the addition of Episodic Buffer (Baddeley, Reference Baddeley2000, Reference Baddeley2020).

When studying the interaction between severity and components of WM, we found greater deficit in Central Executive as severity increases (Mild-moderate TBI: large effect size; Moderate-severe TBI: moderate effect size) but similar, moderate effect size, for deficit in Slave System components of Phonological Loop and Visuo-spatial Sketchpad in all severities. Regarding executive functions, the deficit in the flexibility domain was found to be of the same magnitude and to show the same interaction with injury severity as impairment of the Central Executive of WM. Additionally, Episodic Buffer was found to have a moderate to small effect size for mild-moderate TBI, and a moderate effect size for moderate-severe TBI.

The finding of a moderator effect of injury severity on the Central Executive but not (or to a lesser degree) on the Slave Systems was an interesting result. Indeed, the Central Executive dysfunction might presumably be related to post-traumatic prefrontal dysfunction, which increases with more severe injury. This finding is consistent with previous studies suggesting that WM impairments after TBI are related to an altered activation of a distributed network involving prefrontal structures (Chai et al., Reference Chai, Abd Hamid and Abdullah2018; Christodoulou et al., Reference Christodoulou, DeLuca, Ricker, Madigan, Bly, Lange and Ni2001; Sanchez-Carrion et al., Reference Sanchez-Carrion, Fernandez-Espejo, Junque, Falcon, Bargallo, Roig, Bernabeu, Tormos and Vendrell2008). Indeed, the Central Executive has been found to rely heavily on prefrontal structures, particularly the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) (Jimura et al., Reference Jimura, Chushak, Westbrook and Braver2018; Kim et al., Reference Kim, Kroger, Calhoun and Clark2015; Murty et al., Reference Murty, Sambataro, Radulescu, Altamura, Iudicello, Zoltick, Weinberger, Goldberg and Mattay2011; Osaka et al., Reference Osaka, Osaka, Kondo, Morishita, Fukuyama, Aso and Shibasaki2003).

In contrast, Slave System functioning has been found to rely more on posterior (parietal) cortex than on prefrontal areas (Andersen & Cui, Reference Andersen and Cui2009; Owen et al., Reference Owen, McMillan, Laird and Bullmore2005), which may explain why Slave System are less affected by TBI severity. The similar moderate effect size for both severity levels is perhaps due to the diffuse nature of the injury, regardless of severity (Graham et al., Reference Graham, Jolly, Zimmerman, Bourke, Scott, Cole and Sharp2020; Smith & Meaney, Reference Smith and Meaney2016).

We hypothesized that dissociation between Central Executive and Slave System impairments at different injury severity levels could be partially explained by greater executive functioning deficits after severe TBI. However, we found a large effect size of global executive functioning deficits in both severities. After a careful examination of the heterogeneity found in executive functioning measures, we identified a moderator effect of executive functioning tasks, reflecting that different tasks measure different domains of executive functioning. As this is consistent with the fractioning models of EF, to inhibition, interference control, cognitive flexibility (Diamond, Reference Diamond2013) or updating, inhibition and shifting (Miyake et al., Reference Miyake, Friedman, Emerson, Witzki, Howerter and Wager2000), we ran separate meta-analyses for verbal fluency tasks and flexibility/shifting tasks. We have done this on the basis of studies showing verbal fluency as a separate executive function common factor modulating more global efficiency and affected differently than other components of EF (Gustavson et al., Reference Gustavson, Elman, Panizzon, Franz, Zuber, Sanderson-Cimino, Reynolds, Jacobson, Xian, Jak, Toomey, Lyons and Kremen2020). This might be due to language processing involvement as a crucial component of this task (Whiteside et al., Reference Whiteside, Kealey, Semla, Luu, Rice, Basso and Roper2016).

Tasks measuring flexibility / shifting showed a similar interaction with injury severity as Central Executive tasks, suggesting that the Central Executive and flexibility are two cognitive domains that exhibit a biological gradient with TBI; they are more impaired on average as TBI severity increases. This pattern was not found for the executive functioning fluency tasks, yielding a large effect size for both severities. The possible overlapping prefrontal activation of Central Executive and shifting EF, both requiring the integration and manipulation of information, might offer a possible explanation for EF and Central Executive effect size similarity (Chai et al., Reference Chai, Abd Hamid and Abdullah2018; Uddin, Reference Uddin2021).

An additional finding is a small effect size for Episodic Buffer in mild-moderate TBI, as opposed to a moderate effect size for moderate-severe TBI. This result suggests that the Episodic Buffer relies both on Slave System and Central Executive components, with greater involvement of Central Executive and smaller (but still present) involvement of Slave System. Due to possible measurement confounding (Dunning et al., Reference Dunning, Westgate and Adlam2016; de Pontes Nobre et al., Reference de Pontes Nobre, de Carvalho Rodrigues, Sbicigo, da Rosa Piccolo, Zortea, Duarte Junior and de Salles2013), there is less literature about Episodic Buffer in patients with TBI. However, dual-modality tasks for measuring Episodic Buffer appear to support the Episodic Buffer model component as linking between the Central Executive and slave systems (Baddeley et al., Reference Baddeley, Allen and Hitch2018; Baddeley, Reference Baddeley2000). Possibly, the dissociation of Episodic Buffer dysfunction at different severity levels might be explained by the mutual reliance of Episodic Buffer, Central Executive and flexibility in EF in the engagement of strategy selection (Collette & Van Der Linden, Reference Collette and Van Der Linden2002).

Studying a WM component through different TBI severities, may shed new theoretical light on the WM structure. First, regarding the findings of dissociation for the similar effect size on Slave Systems at both severity levels, but larger effect size in executive functioning of flexibility for severe TBI : this suggests that brain injury at all severity levels affects a wide range of cognitive functions, reducing general cognitive capacity (reflected by the impairment of both Slave Systems and Central Executive) (Azouvi et al., Reference Azouvi, Arnould, Dromer and Vallat-Azouvi2017; Vallat-Azouvi et al., Reference Vallat-Azouvi, Weber, Legrand and Azouvi2007). However, in the more severe TBI cases, the unique frontal lobe profile is largely expressed as affecting Central Executive more clearly (Kavé & Heinik, Reference Kavé and Heinik2017; Larson et al., Reference Larson, Clayson and Farrer2012). However, the current results do not reveal what is behind that primary capacity reduction regardless of severity. Future research might answer that question.

Secondly, we find that the dissociation between severities in Episodic Buffer as well support the Episodic Buffer as a multi-link component, linking Slave System, Central Executive, long-term memory, and EF. Expanding Baddeley’s’ link between Central Executive and long-term memory (Baddeley, Reference Baddeley2017), our current finding of the larger, moderate, effect size of Episodic Buffer in moderate-severe TBI, might explain its role in common dysfunctions after moderate-severe TBI of long-term memory and Central Executive (Azouvi et al., Reference Azouvi, Arnould, Dromer and Vallat-Azouvi2017; Cappa et al., Reference Cappa, Conger and Conger2011; Vallat-Azouvi et al., Reference Vallat-Azouvi, Weber, Legrand and Azouvi2007). Nevertheless, we cannot exclude the possibility of a link between Episodic Buffer and Slave Systems, but those links need further investigation.

Our results might also suggest some clinical implications, indicating a variety of WM deficit ’profiles’ in all TBI severities. At the group level, the present results suggest that, on average, individuals with TBI, particularly after severe TBI, suffer from severe deficits of the Central Executive, while slave systems are less severely impaired. However, the profile of impairment was found to be different across different injury severity levels. In addition, we cannot exclude that the profile of impairments across the WM components may vary from one individual to the other, depending on the underlying brain lesions. These findings should encourage clinicians to assess specifically each WM component in a given patient, in order to adapt interventions more precisely to each individual’s impairments, rather than addressing only one global measure of WM for assessment or treatment.

We need to acknowledge several limitations in the present study. Given the small number of studies that conformed to our strict inclusion criteria, we did not separate studies according to age, gender, or time since injury. These potential moderators should be evaluated in future studies, especially time since injury and age at injury which are known to affect WM functioning (Azouvi et al., Reference Azouvi, Arnould, Dromer and Vallat-Azouvi2017; Dunning et al., Reference Dunning, Westgate and Adlam2016) and could not be analyzed in our study due to insufficient data. In addition, it should be acknowledged that the included studies defined severity of TBI using various markers which do not always align, such as GCS, duration of LOC or duration of PTA. This may have impacted on the accuracy of the results. Additional important factors to mention, that may cause possible bias, and were not assessed in our study due to insufficient data: blind assessment and method of recruitment; it is impossible to know whether patients have more or less initial complaints of WM deficits, introducing a bias of recruitment affecting the results. Future studies examining WM structure should be theoretically based, examining four components of WM under several clinical populations. Comparisons of different deficit profiles (i.e., focal injury in vascular disease or diffuse axonal injury in multiple sclerosis) besides varying severities, could expand our understanding of the relations between the model’s components.

Despite these limitations, it is essential to conduct this meta-analysis and to examine the functioning of WM in TBI patients.

Conclusion

Despite the limitations presented, the outcomes of the present meta-analysis and review revealed important trends regarding WM structure and relations between Baddeley’s four components model, allowing for a better understanding of WM deficits in varying severities of TBI. The theoretical and clinical implications of these deficits are not yet well understood; however, findings suggest WM should first be assessed and then treated in all routine clinical practice and when developing plans for interventions (Hellgren et al., Reference Hellgren, Samuelsson, Lundqvist and Borsbo2015; Vallat et al., Reference Vallat, Azouvi, Hardisson, Meffert, Tessier and Pradat-Diehl2010; Vallat-Azouvi et al., Reference Vallat-Azouvi, Pradat-Diehl and Azouvi2009, Reference Vallat-Azouvi, Pradat-Diehl and Azouvi2014). Future research should focus on studies with strict inclusion criteria for known moderating factors, including wider heterogeneity of pathologies and WM deficit profile. This will result in further revelations about general WM structure and specific WM deficits in individuals with TBI.

Acknowledgements

None.

Funding statement

This work was supported by a grant from the Rose K. Ginzburg Cathedra for Alzheimer’s Research [Grant No. 259147].

Competing interests

The authors have no competing interests to declare that are relevant to the content of this article.

References

Aboitiz, S., & García, R. (2010). The phonological loop: a key innovation in human evolution. Current Anthropology, 51(S1), S55–S65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allain, P., Etcharry-Bouyx, F., & Le Gall, D. (2001). A case study of selective impairment of the central executive component of working memory after a focal frontal lobe damage. Brain and Cognition, 45(1), 2143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alloway, T. P., Rajendran, G., & Archibald, L. M. D. (2009). Working memory in children with developmental disorders. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 42(4), 251262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andelic, N., Sigurdardottir, S., Schanke, A. K., Sandvik, L., Sveen, U., & Roe, C. (2010). Disability, physical health and mental health 1 year after traumatic brain injury. Disability and Rehabilitation, 32(13), 11221131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andersen, R. A., & Cui, H. (2009). Intention, action planning, and decision making in parietal-frontal circuits. Neuron, 63(5), 568583.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Asloun, S., Soury, S., Couillet, J., Giroire, J. M., Joseph, P. A., Mazaux, J. M., & Azouvi, P. (2008). Interactions between divided attention and working-memory load in patients with severe traumatic brain injury. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 30(4), 481490.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Azouvi, P., Arnould, A., Dromer, E., & Vallat-Azouvi, C. (2017). Neuropsychology of traumatic brain injury: An expert overview. Revue Neurologique, 173(7-8), 461472.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(11), 417423.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4(10), 556559.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. (2017). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory?. In Exploring working memory: Selected works of Alan Baddeley (pp. 297311). https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315111261 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A. (2020). Working memory. In Memory (pp. 71111). Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780429449642-4 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A., Allen, R., & Hitch, G. (2018). Binding in visual working memory. In Exploring working memory (pp. 312331). Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315111261-25 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A., & Della, S. (1996). Working memory and executive control. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 351(1346), 13971404.Google ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In G. H., B. (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Belleville, S., Gilbert, B., Fontaine, F., Gagnon, L., Ménard, É., & Gauthier, S. (2006). Improvement of episodic memory in persons with mild cognitive impairment and healthy older adults: Evidence from a cognitive intervention program. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 22(5–6), 486499. https://doi.org/10.1159/000096316 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bryer, E. J., Medaglia, J. D., Rostami, S., & Hillary, F. G. (2013). Neural recruitment after mild traumatic brain injury is task dependent: A meta-analysis. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 19(7), 751762.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cappa, K. A., Conger, J. C., & Conger, A. J. (2011). Injury severity and outcome: A meta-analysis of prospective studies on TBI outcome. Health Psychology, 30(5), 542560.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chai, W. J., Abd Hamid, A. I., & Abdullah, J. M. (2018). Working memory from the psychological and neurosciences perspectives: A review. In Frontiers in psychology. vol. 9, p. 401, Frontiers Media S.A. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00401 Google Scholar
Chen, J., Wu, H., Liao, P., Hsu, L., Tseng, C., Liu, L., & Chiu, T. (2012). Working memory in patients with mild traumatic brain injury: Functional MR imaging analysis. Radiology, 264(3), 844851. https://doi.org/10.1148/radiol.12112154 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christodoulou, C., DeLuca, J., Ricker, J. H., Madigan, N. K., Bly, B. M., Lange, G.,…, Ni, A. C. (2001). Functional magnetic resonance imaging of working memory impairment after traumatic brain injury. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 71(2), 161168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, J. (2013). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Taylor & Francis.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collette, F., & Van Der Linden, M. (2002). Brain imaging of the central executive component of working memory. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 26(2), 105125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cook, D., Mulrow, C., & Haynes, R. B. (1997). Synthesis of best evidence for clinical decisions. Annals of Internal Medicine, 126(5), 376380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coste, C., Agar, N., Petitfour, E., Quinette, P., Guillery-Girard, B., Azouvi, P., & Piolino, P. (2011). Exploring the roles of the executive and short-term feature-binding functions in retrieval of retrograde autobiographical memories in severe traumatic brain injury. Cortex, 47(7), 771786. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2010.07.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coste, C., Navarro, B., Vallat-Azouvi, C., Brami, M., Azouvi, P., & Piolino, P. (2015). Disruption of temporally extended self-memory system following traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychologia, 71, 133145. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.03.014 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Couillet, J., Soury, S., Lebornec, G., Asloun, S., Joseph, P. A., Mazaux, J. M., & Azouvi, P. (2010). Rehabilitation of divided attention after severe traumatic brain injury: A randomised trial. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 20(3), 321339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Curtis, E., & D’Esposito, M. (2003). Persistent activity in the prefrontal cortex during working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(9), 415423.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
D’Esposito, M. (2007). From cognitive to neural models of working memory. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 362(1481), 761772.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Pontes Nobre, A., de Carvalho Rodrigues, J., Sbicigo, J., da Rosa Piccolo, L., Zortea, M., Duarte Junior, S., & de Salles, J. (2013). Tasks for assessment of the episodic buffer: A systematic review. Psychology and Neuroscience, 6(3), 331343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64(1), 135168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dores, A. R., Barbosa, F., Carvalho, I. P., Almeida, I., Guerreiro, S., da Rocha, B. M., de Sousa, L., & Castro-Caldas, A. (2017). Study of behavioural and neural bases of visuo-spatial working memory with an fMRI paradigm based on an n-back task. Journal of Neuropsychology, 11(1), 122134. https://doi.org/10.1111/jnp.12076 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Draper, K., & Ponsford, J. (2008). Cognitive Functioning Ten Years Following Traumatic Brain Injury and Rehabilitation. Neuropsychology, 22(5), 618625. https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.22.5.618 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunning, D. L., Westgate, B., & Adlam, A. L. R. (2016). A meta-analysis of working memory impairments in survivors of moderate-to-severe traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychology, 30(7), 811819.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eierud, C., Craddock, R. C., Fletcher, S., Aulakh, M., King-Casas, B., Kuehl, D., & Laconte, S. M. (2014). Neuroimaging after mild traumatic brain injury: Review and meta-analysis. NeuroImage: Clinical, 4, 283294.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Field, A. P., & Gillett, R. (2010). How to do a meta-analysis. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 63(3), 665694.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forbes, N. F., Carrick, L. A., McIntosh, A. M., & Lawrie, S. M. (2009). Working memory in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 39(6), 889905.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldman-rakic, P. S. (2012). The prefrontal landscape: implications of functional architecture for understanding human mentation and the central executive. In The prefrontal cortex executive and cognitive functions (pp. 14451453). https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198524410.003.0007 Google Scholar
Graham, N. S. N., Jolly, A., Zimmerman, K., Bourke, N. J., Scott, G., Cole, J. H., Sharp, D. J., & et al. (2020). Diffuse axonal injury predicts neurodegeneration after moderate-severe traumatic brain injury. Brain, 143(12), 36853698.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gustavson, D. E., Elman, J. A., Panizzon, M. S., Franz, C. E., Zuber, J., Sanderson-Cimino, M., Reynolds, C. A., Jacobson, K. C., Xian, H., Jak, A. J., Toomey, R., Lyons, M. J., Kremen, W. S. (2020). Association of baseline semantic fluency and progression to mild cognitive impairment in middle-aged men. Neurology, 95(8), e973e983.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hanten, G., Stallings-Roberson, G., Song, X., Bradshaw, M., & Levin, S. (2003). Subject ordered pointing task performance following severe traumatic brain injury in adults. Brain Injury, 17(10), 871882. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699050210147220 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hellgren, L., Samuelsson, K., Lundqvist, A., & Borsbo, B. (2015). Computerized training of working memory for patients with acquired brain injury. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 96(10), e48e49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Higgins, J. P. T., & Thompson, S. G. (2002). Quantifying heterogeneity in a meta-analysis. Statistics in Medicine, 21(11), 15391558.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iverson, G. L. (2010). Mild traumatic brain injury meta-analyses can obscure individual differences. Brain Injury, 24(10), 12461255.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jak, J., Jurick, S., Hoffman, S., Evangelista, N. D., Deford, N., Keller, A., Merritt, C., Sanderson-Cimino, M., Sorg, S., Delano-Wood, L., & Bangen, J. (2020). PTSD, but not history of mTBI, is associated with altered myelin in combat-exposed Iraq and Afghanistan Veterans. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 34(6), 10701087. https://doi.org/10.1080/13854046.2020.1730975 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jimura, K., Chushak, M. S., Westbrook, A., & Braver, T. S. (2018). Intertemporal decision-making involves prefrontal control mechanisms associated with working memory. Cerebral Cortex, 28(4), 11051116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johansson, B., Berglund, P., & Rnnbck, L. (2009). Mental fatigue and impaired information processing after mild and moderate traumatic brain injury. Brain Injury, 23(13-14), 10271040.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jourdan, C., Bayen, E., Pradat-Diehl, P., Ghout, I., Darnoux, E., Azerad, S., & Azouvi, P. (2016). A comprehensive picture of 4-year outcome of severe brain injuries. Results from the PariS-TBI study. Annals of Physical and Rehabilitation Medicine, 59(2), 100106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jurick, M., Hoffman, N., Sorg, S., Keller, V., Evangelista, D., DeFord, E., Sanderson-Cimino, M., Bangen, J., Delano-Wood, L., Deoni, S., & Jak, J. (2018). Pilot investigation of a novel white matter imaging technique in Veterans with and without history of mild traumatic brain injury. Brain Injury, 32(10), 12561265. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699052.2018.1493225 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kavé, G., & Heinik, J. (2017). Cognitive correlates of perseverations in individuals with memory impairment. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 32(1), 6370.Google ScholarPubMed
Kim, C., Kroger, J. K., Calhoun, V. D., & Clark, V. P. (2015). The role of the frontopolar cortex in manipulation of integrated information in working memory. Neuroscience Letters, 595, 2529.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Königs, M., Engenhorst, P. J., & Oosterlaan, J. (2016). Intelligence after traumatic brain injury: Meta-analysis of outcomes and prognosis. European Journal of Neurology, 23(1), 2129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Konrad, C., Geburek, J., Rist, F., Blumenroth, H., Fischer, B., Husstedt, I., Arolt, V., Schiffbauer, H., & Lohmann, H. (2011). Long-Term cognitive and emotional consequences of mild traumatic brain injury. Psychological Medicine, 41(6), 11971211. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291710001728 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kumar, S., Rao, S. L., Chandramouli, B. A., & Pillai, S. (2013). Reduced contribution of executive functions in impaired working memory performance in mild traumatic brain injury patients. Clinical Neurology and Neurosurgery, 115(8), 13261332.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kwok, Y., Lee, C., Leung, S., & Poon, S. (2008). Changes of cognitive functioning following mild traumatic brain injury over a 3-month period. Brain Injury, 22(10), 740751. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699050802336989 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Larson, M. J., Clayson, P. E., & Farrer, T. J. (2012). Performance monitoring and cognitive control in individuals with mild traumatic brain injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 18(2), 323333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lehto, J. (2018). Are executive function tests dependent on working memory capacity? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 49(1), 2950. https://doi.org/10.1080/713755616.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lenhard, D. A., & W. Psychometrica - Institut für psychologische Diagnostik 2017. https://www.psychometrica.de/index.html.Google Scholar
Lepach, C., Pauls, F., & Petermann, F. (2015). Executive functioning and visual working memory. Applied Neuropsychology:Adult, 22(2), 100107. https://doi.org/10.1080/23279095.2013.860603 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levin, H. S., Goldstein, F. C., High, W. M., & Williams, D. (1988). Automatic and effortful processing after severe closed head injury. Brain and Cognition, 7(3), 283297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lugtmeijer, S., Lammers, N. A., de Haan, E. H. F., de Leeuw, F. E., & Kessels, R. P. C. (2020). Post-stroke working memory dysfunction: A meta-analysis and systematic review. Neuropsychology Review, 31(1), 202219.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mangels, J. A., Craik, F. I. M., Levine, B., Schwartz, M. L., & Stuss, D. T. (2002). Effects of divided attention on episodic memory in chronic traumatic brain injury: A function of severity and strategy. Neuropsychologia, 40(13), 833836.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McAllister, T. W., Flashman, L. A., Sparling, M. B., & Saykin, A. J. (2004). Working memory deficits after traumatic brain injury: Catecholaminergic mechanisms and prospects for treatment - a review. Brain Injury, 18(4), 331350.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McAllister, T. W., Flashman, L. A., Sparling, M. B., & Saykin, A. J. (2009). Working memory deficits after traumatic brain injury: Catecholaminergic mechanisms and prospects for treatment — a review. Brain Injury, 18(4), 331350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McAllister, W., Sparling, B., Flashman, A., Guerin, J., Mamourian, C., & Saykin, J. (2001). Differential working memory load effects after mild traumatic brain injury. NeuroImage, 14(5), 10041012. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2001.0899 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex, frontal lobe, tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., & Altman, D. G. (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The PRISMA statement. Annals of Internal Medicine, 151(4), 264.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monti, M., Voss, W., Pence, A., McAuley, E., Kramer, F., & Cohen, J. (2013). History of mild traumatic brain injury is associated with deficits in relational memory, reduced hippocampal volume, and less neural activity later in life. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 5(AUG). https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2013.00041 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Müller, G., & Knight, T. (2006). The functional neuroanatomy of working memory: contributions of human brain lesion studies. Neuroscience, 139(1), 51–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murty, P., Sambataro, F., Radulescu, E., Altamura, M., Iudicello, J., Zoltick, B., Weinberger, R., Goldberg, E., & Mattay, S. (2011). Selective updating of working memory content modulates meso-cortico-striatal activity. NeuroImage, 57(3), 12641272. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2011.05.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nee, D. E., Brown, J. W., Askren, M. K., Berman, M. G., Demiralp, E., Krawitz, A., & Jonides, J. (2013). A meta-analysis of executive components of working memory. Cerebral Cortex, 23(2), 264282.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
NIH, Principles and guidelines for reporting preclinical research, 2014. Retrieved from National Institute of Health website: https://www.nih.gov/research-training/rigor-reproducibility/principles-guidelines-reporting-preclinical-research%0Ahttp://www.nih.gov/about/reporting-preclinical-research.htm Google Scholar
Osaka, M., Osaka, N., Kondo, H., Morishita, M., Fukuyama, H., Aso, T., & Shibasaki, H. (2003). The neural basis of individual differences in working memory capacity: An fMRI study. NeuroImage, 18(3), 789797.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Owen, A. M., McMillan, K. M., Laird, A. R., & Bullmore, E. (2005). N-back working memory paradigm: A meta-analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies. Human Brain Mapping, 25(1), 4659.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ozen, J., Itier, J., Preston, F., & Fernandes, A. (2013). Long-Term working memory deficits after concussion: Electrophysiological evidence. Brain Injury, 27(11), 12441255. https://doi.org/10.3109/02699052.2013.804207 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palacios, M., Fernandez-Espejo, D., Junque, C., Sanchez-Carrion, R., Roig, T., Tormos, M., Bargallo, N., & Vendrell, P. (2011). Diffusion tensor imaging differences relate to memory deficits in diffuse traumatic brain injury. BMC Neurology, 11, 111. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2377-11-24 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perlstein, W. M., Cole, M. A., Demery, J. A., Seignourel, P. J., Dixit, N. K., Larson, M. J., & Briggs, R. W. (2004). Parametric manipulation of working memory load in traumatic brain injury: Behavioral and neural correlates. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10(5), 724741.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phillips, N. L., Parry, L., Mandalis, A., & Lah, S. (2017). Working memory outcomes following traumatic brain injury in children: A systematic review with meta-analysis. Child Neuropsychology, 23(1), 2666.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Price, J. (2009). The computerized object and abstract designs test (COAD): A pilot study of a new test of visual working memory. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 48(2), 109123. https://doi.org/10.1348/014466508X366713 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quak, M., London, E., & Talsma, D. (2015). A multisensory perspective of working memory. Frontiers in human neuroscience, 9, 197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richmond, L., Brackins, T., & Rajaram, S. (2022). Episodic memory performance modifies the strength of the age–brain structure relationship. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 19(7), 4364.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rohling, M. L., Binder, L. M., Demakis, G. J., Larrabee, G. J., Ploetz, D. M., Langhinrichsen-Rohling, J. (2011). A meta-analysis of neuropsychological outcome after mild traumatic brain injury: Re-analyses and reconsiderations of Binder, et al, 1997, Frencham et al., (2005), and Pertab et al., (2009). The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 25(4), 608623.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenthal, R. (1991). Meta-analytic procedure for social research. Sage Publications.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rudner, M., Fransson, P., Ingvar, M., Nyberg, L., & Rönnberg, J. (2007). Neural representation of binding lexical signs and words in the episodic buffer of working memory. Neuropsychologia, 45(10), 2258–2276.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rudner, M., & Rönnberg, J. (2008). The role of the episodic buffer in working memory for language processing. Cognitive Processing, 9, 19–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruet, A., Jourdan, C., Bayen, E., Darnoux, E., Sahridj, D., Ghout, I., & Azouvi, P. (2018). Employment outcome four years after a severe traumatic brain injury: Results of the paris severe traumatic brain injury study. Disability and Rehabilitation, 40(18), 22002207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanchez-Carrion, R., Fernandez-Espejo, D., Junque, C., Falcon, C., Bargallo, N., Roig, T., Bernabeu, M., Tormos, Jé M., Vendrell, P. (2008). A longitudinal fMRI study of working memory in severe TBI patients with diffuse axonal injury. NeuroImage, 43(3), 421429.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanchez-Meca, J., & Fulgencio, M. M. (1997). Homogenity test in meta-analysis: A monte carlo comparison of statistical power and Type 1 error. Quality and Quantity, 31(4), 385399.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shadish, W. R., & Haddock, C. (1994). Combining estimates of effect size. In Cooper, H., & Hedges, L. V. (Eds.), The handbook of research synthesis (pp. 261281). Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
Shamseer, L., Moher, D., Clarke, M., Ghersi, D., Liberati, A., Petticrew, M., Shekelle, P., Stewart, L. A., the PRISMA-P Group (2015). Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analysis protocols (prisma-p) 2015: Elaboration and explanation. BMJ (Online), 349, 255274.Google Scholar
Smith, D. H., & Meaney, D. F. (2016). Axonal damage in traumatic brain injury. The Neuroscientist, 6(6), 483495.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spikman, M., Timmerman, E., Milders, V., Veenstra, S., & van der Naalt, J. (2012). Social cognition impairments in relation to general cognitive deficits, injury severity, and prefrontal lesions in traumatic brain injury patients. Journal of Neurotrauma, 29(1), 101111. https://doi.org/10.1089/neu.2011.2084 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Twick, M., & Levy, D. A. (2021). Fractionating the episodic buffer. Brain and Cognition, 154, 105800.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uddin, L. Q. (2021). Cognitive and behavioural flexibility: Neural mechanisms and clinical considerations. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 22(3), 167179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vakil, E. (2005). The effect of moderate to severe traumatic brain injury (TBI) on different aspects of memory: A selective review. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 27(8), 9771021.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vakil, E., & Greenstein, Y. (2021). Dissociations of memory processes: The contribution of research on memory impairment following traumatic brain injury (TBI)—A focused review. Psychology, 12(02), 161180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vallat, C., Azouvi, P., Hardisson, H., Meffert, R., Tessier, C., & Pradat-Diehl, P. (2010). Rehabilitation of verbal working memory after left hemisphere stroke. Brain Injury, 19(13), 11571164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vallat-Azouvi, C., Pradat-Diehl, P., & Azouvi, P. (2009). Rehabilitation of the central executive of working memory after severe traumatic brain injury: Two single-case studies. Brain Inury, 23(6), 585594.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vallat-Azouvi, C., Pradat-Diehl, P., & Azouvi, P. (2014). Modularity in rehabilitation of working memory: A single-case study. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 24(2), 220237.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vallat-Azouvi, C., Weber, T., Legrand, L., & Azouvi, P. (2007). Working memory after severe traumatic brain injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 13(5), 770780.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wager, D., & Smith, E. (2003). Neuroimaging studies of working memory. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 3(4), 255–274.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whiteside, D. M., Kealey, T., Semla, M., Luu, H., Rice, L., Basso, M. R., & Roper, B. (2016). Verbal fluency: Language or executive function measure? Applied Neuropsychology: Adult, 23(1), 2934.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williamson, D., Scott, J., & Adams, R. (2004). Neuropsychology for clinical practice: Etiology, assessment, and treatment of common neurological disorders. In Adams, R. L., & Parsons, O. (Eds.), Neuropsychology for clinical practice: Etiology, assessment, and treatment of common neurological disorders. American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/10198-000 Google Scholar
Withaar, R., Stowe, L. A., Withaar, R. G., & Hoeks, J. C. J. Toward a two-stage storage model of sentence processing tracking referents: Markedness, world knowledge and pronoun resolution view project convincing conversations view project toward a two-stage storage model of sentence processing 2000. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/268434979.Google Scholar
Zhu, J., Ling, J., & Ding, N. (2019). Association between diffusion tensor imaging findings and cognitive outcomes following mild traumatic brain injury: A PRISMA-compliant meta-analysis. ACS Chemical Neuroscience, 10(12), 48644869.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zimmermann, N., Mograbi, D. C., Hermes-Pereira, A., Fonseca, R. P., & Prigatano, G. P. (2017). Memory and executive functions correlates of self-awareness in traumatic brain injury. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 22(4), 346360.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Components of Baddeley’s Working Memory model. The table presents the main functions of each one of the four components of the model, of the main tasks used to assess each component, and of the corresponding brain areas

Figure 1

Figure 1. Flow chart of the literature search process according to Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analyses guidelines.

Figure 2

Figure 2. Funnel plot showing effect sizes for interventions delineated by sample size.

Figure 3

Table 2. Effect size and methodology of studies sorted by working memory domains

Figure 4

Table 3. Effect size and methodology of studies sorted by injury severity categories

Figure 5

Figure 3. Forest plot showing effect sizes and confidence intervals for the various working memory domains for different injury severity categories.

Figure 6

Figure 4. Forest plot showing effect sizes and confidence intervals for central executive working memory, executive functions flexibility and fluency for different injury severity categories.

Figure 7

Table 4. Effect size and methodology of studies sorted by working memory domains and injury severity categories

Figure 8

Table 5. Effect size and methodology of studies sorted by executive function fluency and executive function flexibility memory domains