Introduction
A traumatic brain injury (TBI) is induced by external force, leading to abnormalities in brain structure and function. Individuals with chronic TBI (6-months or more post-injury) often have difficulties in cognitive functions such as attention, memory and executive functions, which are critical to daily life tasks. One of the most relevant mechanisms of such deficits following TBI is diffuse axonal injury (DAI; Smith, Meaney, & Shull, Reference Smith, Meaney and Shull2003). Given the prevalence of DAI following TBI, network analyses in advanced magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) are providing new insights into the mechanisms of disruptions to the brain and associated impairments following TBI (see Sharp, Scott, & Leech Reference Sharp, Scott and Leech2014, for review). Several resting-state functional MRI (fMRI) studies have identified disruptions in a variety of networks following TBI (Bonnelle et al., Reference Bonnelle, Leech, Kinnunen, Ham, Beckmann, De Boissezon and Sharp2011, Reference Bonnelle, Ham, Leech, Kinnunen, Mehta, Greenwood and Sharp2012; Ham et al., Reference Ham, Bonnelle, Hellyer, Jilka, Robertson, Leech and Sharp2014; Han et al., Reference Han, Mac Donald, Johnson, Barnes, Wierzechowski, Zonies and Brody2014; Marquez de la Plata et al., Reference Marquez de la Plata, Garces, Shokri Kojori, Grinnan, Krishnan, Pidikiti and Diaz-Arrastia2011; Mayer, Mannell, Ling, Gasparovic, & Yeo, Reference Mayer, Mannell, Ling, Gasparovic and Yeo2011; Nakamura, Hillary, & Biswal, Reference Nakamura, Hillary and Biswal2009; Pandit et al., Reference Pandit, Expert, Lambiotte, Bonnelle, Leech, Turkheimer and Sharp2013; Sharp et al., Reference Sharp, Beckmann, Greenwood, Kinnunen, Bonnelle, De Boissezon and Leech2011; Slobounov et al., Reference Slobounov, Gay, Zhang, Johnson, Pennell, Sebastianelli and Hallett2011; Sours et al., Reference Sours, Zhuo, Janowich, Aarabi, Shanmuganathan and Gullapalli2013, Reference Sours, Rosenberg, Kane, Roys, Zhuo, Shanmuganathan and Gullapalli2015; Tang et al., Reference Tang, Ge, Sodickson, Miles, Zhou, Reaume and Grossman2011).
Although the previous resting-state fMRI (rsfMRI) studies in TBI have provided valuable information for a better understanding of TBI, most of these previous studies have focused on investigating connectivity alterations within a single-network or patterns of connectivity for a single-brain-region with rest of the brain. For example, TBI-related disruptions of functional connectivity within the default mode network (DMN), which is “de-activated” when subjects engage in tasks (Raichle et al., Reference Raichle, MacLeod, Snyder, Powers, Gusnard and Shulman2001), have been observed in a prior study that had applied independent component analysis (ICA) (Sharp et al., Reference Sharp, Beckmann, Greenwood, Kinnunen, Bonnelle, De Boissezon and Leech2011). Using a seed-based approach, Mayer et al. (Reference Mayer, Mannell, Ling, Gasparovic and Yeo2011) identified that TBIs can disrupt the connectivity of seed regions from the DMN and task-related network (Fox et al., Reference Fox, Snyder, Vincent, Corbetta, Van Essen and Raichle2005), respectively, with rest of the brain.
Ham et al. (Reference Ham, Bonnelle, Hellyer, Jilka, Robertson, Leech and Sharp2014) also revealed that impaired self-awareness in TBI is associated with reduced connectivity within the fronto-parietal control network (FPCN), which is activated during tasks that engage executive functions (Vincent, Kahn, Snyder, Raichle, & Buckner, 2008). However, probing only within-network connectivity or the connectivity of a single region with rest of the brain may not be sufficient to fully understand the neural mechanisms by which TBI disrupts brain networks leading to functional impairments, since DAI may affect multiple regions encompassing multiple networks (Han et al., Reference Han, Mac Donald, Johnson, Barnes, Wierzechowski, Zonies and Brody2014). These disruptions may lead to deficits of higher order cognitive functions due to reduced interactivity among multiple networks.
To more comprehensively identify TBI-related disruptions, several studies recently investigated resting-state functional connectivity over multiple networks. For example, Bonnelle et al. (Reference Bonnelle, Ham, Leech, Kinnunen, Mehta, Greenwood and Sharp2012) demonstrated that damaged white matter tracts connecting within the salience network (SN; Seeley et al., Reference Seeley, Menon, Schatzberg, Keller, Glover, Kenna and Greicius2007) predict DMN dysfunction. Graph theoretic analyses (see Rubinov & Sporns, Reference Rubinov and Sporns2010, for review) also revealed patterns of network disruptions in TBI (Caeyenberghs et al., Reference Caeyenberghs, Leemans, Heitger, Leunissen, Dhollander, Sunaert and Swinnen2012; Caeyenberghs, Leemans, Leunissen, Michiels, & Swinnen, Reference Caeyenberghs, Leemans, Leunissen, Michiels and Swinnen2013; Han et al., Reference Han, Mac Donald, Johnson, Barnes, Wierzechowski, Zonies and Brody2014; Hillary et al., Reference Hillary, Rajtmajer, Roman, Medaglia, Slocomb-Dluzen, Calhoun and Wylie2014; Nakamura et al., Reference Nakamura, Hillary and Biswal2009; Pandit et al., Reference Pandit, Expert, Lambiotte, Bonnelle, Leech, Turkheimer and Sharp2013). For example, disruptions in “small-worldness”—the level of clustering relative to path length—of individuals with TBI have been reported in whole-brain functional networks (Nakamura et al., Reference Nakamura, Hillary and Biswal2009). Module-based graph theoretic analyses have also been used to demonstrate prominent reductions in between-module connectivity in concussive blast-related TBI at the sub-acute stage, within 6-months post-injury (Han et al., Reference Han, Mac Donald, Johnson, Barnes, Wierzechowski, Zonies and Brody2014).
In this study, we assessed chronic TBI individuals’ intrinsic functional connectivity among multiple networks associated with goal-directed cognition (or goal-directed behavior). Goal-directed cognition is key in everyday life as it represents an ability to coordinate thoughts and actions to achieve goals while adjusting these goals in the context of changing task demands. Individuals with chronic TBI often exhibit difficulties in goal-directed cognition (Levine et al., Reference Levine, Stuss, Milberg, Alexander, Schwartz and Macdonald1998; Mateer, Sohlberg, & Crinean, Reference Mateer, Sohlberg and Crinean1987; Robertson, Manly, Andrade, Baddeley, & Yiend, Reference Robertson, Manly, Andrade, Baddeley and Yiend1997; Whyte et al., Reference Whyte, Polansky, Cavallucci, Fleming, Lhulier and Coslett1996). Impaired goal-directed cognition is associated with lowered work status (Crepeau & Scherzer, Reference Crepeau and Scherzer1993), as such deficits lead to disorganization in daily life tasks such as cooking and navigating the layout of floor-plans (Levine et al., Reference Levine, Robertson, Clare, Carter, Hong, Wilson and Stuss2000).
An fMRI activation study in healthy individuals (Spreng, Stevens, Chamberlain, Gilmore, & Schacter, Reference Spreng, Stevens, Chamberlain, Gilmore and Schacter2010) revealed that goal-directed cognition induces brain activations in the DMN (Raichle et al., Reference Raichle, MacLeod, Snyder, Powers, Gusnard and Shulman2001), dorsal attention network (DAN; Corbetta & Shulman, Reference Corbetta and Shulman2002), and FPCN (Vincent et al., 2008) during an autobiographical task and a visuospatial planning task [the Tower of London task (Shallice, Reference Shallice1982)]. Specifically, DMN regions were activated during the autobiographical task representing internally focused goal-directed cognition, whereas DAN regions were activated during the visuospatial planning task representing externally focused goal-directed cognition. Importantly, regions in the FPCN were activated during both tasks, indicating that the FPCN engages in both internally and externally focused goal-directed cognition. A follow-up study (Spreng, Sepulcre, Turner, Stevens, & Schacter, Reference Spreng, Sepulcre, Turner, Stevens and Schacter2013) further identified that the FPCN mediates a dynamic balance between the DMN and DAN. Based on the frequent deficits in goal-directed cognition in chronic TBI and the previous literature in goal-directed cognition in healthy individuals, we hypothesized that individuals with chronic TBI would show disrupted interactions among DMN-DAN-FPCN. We predicted that an assessment of multiple networks-of-interest would provide more comprehensive insights on the brain mechanisms of the goal management deficits present in chronic TBI compared to a single network approach.
Methods
Participants
The data included in this analysis are part of a larger study (Krawczyk et al., Reference Krawczyk, de la Plata, Schauer, Vas, Keebler, Tuthill and Chapman2013). We analyzed 57 individuals, comprising of 40 chronic TBI individuals with upper moderate disability to lower good recovery (age 20–45 years; >6 months post-injury; 6–7 on the Extended Glasgow Outcome Scale (GOS-E; Wilson, Pettigrew, & Teasdale, Reference Wilson, Pettigrew and Teasdale1998) and 17 healthy controls (age 19–43 years), who completed MRI scans and whose MRI scans passed the Quality Assurance (QA) procedures described below. We recruited these participants from the Dallas area and screened via phone interview before inclusion. Not all TBI participants had an available, recorded Glasgow Coma Scale (GCS; Teasdale & Jennett, Reference Teasdale and Jennett1974) score for information on acute-injury severity. Thus, we estimated initial injury severity from the Ohio State University (OSU) TBI screening form (Corrigan & Bogner, Reference Corrigan and Bogner2007).
The primary causes of injury were blast, blunt force trauma, fall, athletic impacts, vehicle accidents, or combination of these events (Table 1). No TBI participants met clinical diagnostic criteria for neurological or psychiatric comorbidities. We also confirmed that all participants lacked visible focal lesions, contusions, mass shifting, or extreme cortical thinning on structural MRI scans. This step ruled out potential effects of macroscopic structural injuries on fMRI preprocessing steps including registration and subsequent functional connectivity analyses. All participants provided written informed consent, and this study was conducted in compliance with the declaration of Helsinki. This study was approved by the Institutional Review Boards of The University of Texas at Dallas and University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center.
Note: FD, Framewise Displacement; BDI-II, Beck Depression Inventory-II; PCL-S, Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Check List Stressor-Specific.
a Mean and standard deviation values were reported.
b Based on the Ohio State University TBI screening form (Corrigan & Bogner, Reference Corrigan and Bogner2007).
c PCL-S scores for the healthy controls were not available since the healthy individuals, by definition, did not have a traumatic event that they are experiencing for the assessment of PCL-S.
d p-Values were obtained with an age covariate.
e Normalized for head size.
Psychiatric Symptoms Assessments
We assessed symptom severity of depression and post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) for the participants, using the Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II; Beck, Steer, & Brown, 1996) and PTSD Check List Stressor-specific (PCL-S; Weathers, Litz, Herman, Huska, & Keane, 1993). Note that we did not acquire PCL-S scores from the healthy control participants, as PCL-S requires reporting on a specific traumatic event that participant experiences. The healthy individuals, by definition, did not experience traumatic events to be reported on for the PCL-S assessment.
Neuropsychological Assessments
We administered neuropsychological tests at the time of MRI scanning to characterize participants’ cognition in a variety of domains. These tests included similarities, matrix reasoning, and full scale intelligent quotient-2 (FSIQ-2) from the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI) for estimated current IQ (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1999); FSIQ from the Wechsler Test of Adult Reading (WTAR) for estimated premorbid IQ (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler2001); digit span forward and backward from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Third Edition (WAIS-III) for working memory (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1997); color-word, verbal fluency, card sorting, trail making from the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS) for measuring inhibitory control, switching, verbal fluency, processing speed and problem solving (Delis, Kaplan, & Kramer, Reference Delis, Kaplan and Kramer2001); immediate recall and delayed recall from the Wechsler Memory Scale-Fourth Edition (WMS-IV) for memory and recall (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler2008); verbal problem solving assessment (S.B. Chapman, unpublished data); and visual selective learning task adapted from Hanten et al. (Hanten et al., Reference Hanten, Chapman, Gamino, Zhang, Benton, Stallings-Roberson and Levin2004). We also assessed the satisfaction with life scale (Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, Reference Diener, Emmons, Larsen and Griffin1985) to measure global cognitive judgments of life satisfaction.
MRI Data Acquisition
The participants underwent MRI scanning on a Philips Achieva 3 Tesla (T) scanner (Philips Medical Systems, Netherlands) at the Advanced Imaging Research Center. In each imaging session, one or two 416-s runs of rsfMRI were acquired using a standard 32-channel head coil with T2 *-weighted image sequence [repetition time (TR)/echo time(TE)=2000/30 ms; flip angle (FA)=80°; field of view (FOV)=22.0×22.0 cm; matrix=64×64; 37 slices, 4.0 mm thick]. Total number of rsfMRI runs was different across the participants because, at the early stage of our study, we observed that the QA procedures with only one rsfMRI run yielded high rates of participant exclusion. Thus, we additionally acquired two rsfMRI runs for the remainder of the data collection (our strategy to account for differences in total number of rsfMRI scans across the participants is detailed below). During rsfMRI acquisition, the participants were asked to remain still with their eyes closed. For rsfMRI alignment, we obtained one high resolution T1-weighted image of the whole brain (TR/TE=8.2/3.8 ms; FA=12°; FOV=25.6×25.6 cm; matrix=256×256; 160 slices, 1.0 mm thick) for each participant, using the same head coil.
MRI Preprocessing
RsfMRI data were preprocessed with standard methods using a modified version of a shell script generated by afni_proc.py (http://afni.nimh.nih.gov/pub/dist/doc/program_help/afni_proc.py.html) from AFNI (Cox, Reference Cox1996). Each subject’s whole-brain structural images were first skull-stripped and registered to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI; Evans et al., Reference Evans, Collins, Mills, Brown, Kelly and Peters1993) space. For each rsfMRI run, the initial four time points were discarded to allow T1 magnetization saturation. Standard preprocessing methods were then applied, including despiking, slice timing correction, motion correction, coregistration to the structural images in the MNI space using a single affine transform with spatial resampling (4 mm isotropic), normalization to whole brain mode of 1000, band-pass filtering (0.009< f <0.08 Hz), and linear regression.
At the motion correction stage, the six rigid body motion profiles were obtained for the linear regression. In the linear regression, the rsfMRI time-series were 3rd order detrended, and several sources of signal fluctuation unlikely to be of neuronal origin were regressed out: (1) six parameters for the rigid body head motion acquired from the motion correction (Johnstone et al., Reference Johnstone, Walsh, Greischar, Alexander, Fox, Davidson and Oakes2006), (2) the signal averaged over the lateral ventricles, (3) the signal averaged over a region centered in the deep cerebral white matter, (4) the signal averaged over the whole brain [Fox et al., Reference Fox, Snyder, Vincent, Corbetta, Van Essen and Raichle2005; see the control analyses and their results for the effects of global signal regression (GSR) on network analysis results], and (5) the first temporal derivatives of aforementioned parameters.
After the linear regression, motion “scrubbing” (Power, Barnes, Snyder, Schlaggar, & Petersen, Reference Power, Barnes, Snyder, Schlaggar and Petersen2012) was performed with a framewise displacement (FD) of 0.5 mm and a standardized DVARS (http://www2.warwick.ac.uk/fac/sci/statistics/staff/academic-research/nichols/scripts/fsl/DVARS.sh) of 1.8 to prevent potential motion artifacts (Power et al., Reference Power, Barnes, Snyder, Schlaggar and Petersen2012; Satterthwaite et al., Reference Satterthwaite, Wolf, Loughead, Ruparel, Elliott, Hakonarson and Gur2012; van Dijk, Sabuncu, & Buckner, Reference van Dijk, Sabuncu and Buckner2012). A standardized DVARS of 1.8 corresponds to the median plus 1.5 times interquartile range of the standardized DVARS data across all frames and runs. The remaining rsfMRI signals were spatially blurred with 6 mm full-width-at-half-maximum (FWHM) Gaussian kernel. If two rsfMRI runs were acquired, the two preprocessed rsfMRI runs were temporally concatenated. To account for the differences in total number of frames after motion scrubbing (and different number of rsfMRI runs) across the participants and to prevent bias in estimating correlation coefficients from different degrees of freedom across rsfMRI scans, all remaining frames were trimmed to the minimum length (121 frames; 242s) across all concatenated rsfMRI scans after scrubbing as suggested in Power et al. (2014).
Quality Assurance
We visually inspected structural MRI scans to ensure that subjects had no apparent brain atrophy. In rsfMRI preprocessing, the quality of preprocessed data was visually inspected at each step. After motion “scrubbing,” we confirmed that the total length of remaining frames after the “scrubbing” was longer than 4 min, the minimum length required to reliably estimate functional connectivity (van Dijk et al., Reference van Dijk, Hedden, Venkataraman, Evans, Lazar and Buckner2010).
Network Analyses
Identification of disrupted connections
To obtain a connectivity matrix for each of the subjects, we first defined nodes as 43 regions (6 mm spheres; Table 2) affiliated with the DMN, DAN, and FPCN (Spreng et al., Reference Spreng, Sepulcre, Turner, Stevens and Schacter2013). We then calculated the Pearson correlation coefficients for time-series from each pair of the nodes. Correlation coefficients between time-series at short-distance nodes (20 mm in Euclidean distance), presumably associated with non-biological origins such as increased correlation by preprocessing and subject motion, were adjusted to be zeros (Power et al., Reference Power, Cohen, Nelson, Wig, Barnes, Church and Petersen2011). After the Fisher’s Z-transform to ensure the normality of correlations, we performed the general linear model (GLM) analysis with covariates of within-group-centered BDI-II scores on each of the connectivity matrix elements due to statistically significant group differences in BDI-II scores (Table 1). Note that PCL-S scores for the TBI group were not incorporated into the GLM analysis since BDI-II and PCL-S scores for the TBI group were highly correlated (p<10−4), introducing co-linearity into the design matrix.
Statistically significant group differences in connectivity were identified at |Z|>2.58 (p<.01 at the connection level) with correction for multiple comparisons at p<.05 using network-based statistic (NBS; Zalesky, Fornito, & Bullmore, Reference Zalesky, Fornito and Bullmore2010; https://sites.google.com/site/bctnet/comparison/nbs). A total of 10,000 permutations were generated to estimate the null distribution of maximal component size. NBS-corrected group differences in connectivity were visualized on anatomical space using BrainNet Viewer (Xia, Wang, & He, Reference Xia, Wang and He2013). The number of connections with statistically significant group differences was then identified according to (1) Euclidean distance between regions, (2) intra- versus inter-hemispheric connections, and (3) within- versus between-network connections.
Graph theoretic analyses
Network construction
Weighted and undirected networks were constructed for graph theoretic analyses. We defined an edge as the Pearson correlation coefficient for time-series from a pair of the nodes. The correlation coefficients controlled for the BDI-II scores by within-group-centered BDI-II score covariates were then thresholded by the network cost, K, an average of suparthreshold correlation coefficients. Thresholding based on K allowed us to account for the potential effects of group differences in overall functional connectivity strengths on group comparisons of the subsequent network measures 0≤K≤1. When a correlation coefficient for thresholding increases, the network becomes more sparse, yielding low cost K. When a correlation coefficient for thresholding decreases, the network becomes denser, yielding high cost K.
As numerical values and topological properties of network measures vary with these threshold levels, we performed group analyses of the network measures as a function of K, starting from 0.01 in step size of 0.01. Note that, in this thresholding procedure, only positive correlation coefficients were considered for the network construction since the meaning of negative correlations in rsfMRI is unclear at the present time (see the Discussion section for relevant limitations). For each subject, networks with higher K were obtained until the smallest positive correlation coefficient was included. Since the maximum K that allows only positive correlation coefficients was different across subjects, the available number of subjects for group comparisons of the network measures decreased as we applied higher K (i.e., lower correlation coefficient for thresholding) across subjects. Thus, we limited the ranges of K for group analyses of the network measures up to K=0.23 at which we reliably performed group analyses (N≥5 per group).
Network measures
With the connection weight matrices, we obtained the global efficiency (Latora & Marchiori, Reference Latora and Marchiori2001), local efficiency (Latora & Marchiori, Reference Latora and Marchiori2001), and cost efficiency (Achard & Bullmore, Reference Achard and Bullmore2007) for whole-brain network measures. Subsequently, we also obtained regional global efficiency and local efficiency for regional network measures. To obtain these efficiency measures, the network distance (or the shortest path length) should first be calculated. The distance between nodes i and j, d ij , was defined:
where l uv is connection length between nodes u and $v,\,g_{{i\Leftrightarrow j}}$ is the shortest path between nodes i and j and w uv is a network weight between nodes u and v. If nodes i and j are disconnected, $d_{{ij}} \,{\equals}\,\infty$ .
Global efficiency, E glob , was defined:
where E i is regional global efficiency of node i. E glob is a measure of how tightly, on average, nodes are connected with less indirect paths between nodes over the entire network. In theory, 0 ≤ E glob ≤ 1. E glob =0 when all nodes are disconnected. E glob =1 when all nodes are directly connected with maximum weights (Latora & Marchiori, Reference Latora and Marchiori2001).
Local efficiency, E loc , was defined:
where E loc,i is the efficiency of the neighborhood subgraph of node i, N i . Note that since the regional local efficiency, E loc,i , quantifies how much the neighbors of i are fault tolerant when i is removed (Latora & Marchiori, Reference Latora and Marchiori2001). Cost efficiency, E cost , is defined as E cost =E glob -K. Cost efficiency quantifies how a network is economically wired relative to the given network cost. If the network is economical, then E cost >0 (Achard & Bullmore, Reference Achard and Bullmore2007). We performed all of the graph theoretic analyses using the brain connectivity toolbox in MATLAB (Rubinov & Sporns, Reference Rubinov and Sporns2010; http://www.brain-connectivity-toolbox.net/).
Statistical Analyses
All statistical analyses were assessed in MATLAB R2013a. First, we performed the Shapiro-Wilk test at α=0.05 to assess the normality of distributions of each group’s demographics (age, years of education, BDI-II scores, PCL-S scores, percentage of motion-censored volumes, and average FD after motion censoring and trimming) and each network measure. Age did not pass the Shapiro-Wilk normality test. Thus, the Mann-Whitney U test was used to compare age between the groups. Two sample t tests were used to compare other demographics between the groups. The Fisher’s exact test was used to compare the gender distributions between the groups. Group comparisons on the neuropsychological test scores were carried out using the GLM with age, years of education, and within-group-centered BDI-II score covariates. Within-group-centered BDI-II score covariates were included to assess group differences in neuropsychological measures at average BDI-II scores of each respective group (Table 1).
All network measures for the control group passed the Shapiro-Wilk normality test, but most of the measures for the TBI group did not. Thus, for group comparison of the network measures, the Mann-Whitney U test was used.
Control Analyses
We assessed whether there were (1) the effects of demographics (motion, age, PTSD symptom severity, post-injury time) on our results, (2) “overall” neuropsychological deficits in the TBI group, (3) effects of estimated initial injury severity, (4) effects of depressive symptoms in the TBI group, (5) effects of a mixture of civilians and veterans in the TBI group, (6) group differences in whole-brain volumes, (7) sustained DAI in the TBI group, (8) effects of an initial threshold level on NBS, (9) discrepancies in our results when using partial-correlations, (10) effects of GSR, and (11) effects of short distance nodes (see the Supplementary Material for details of the control analyses).
Results
Demographics Comparisons between the Groups
The TBI participants were in the long-term chronic phase of TBI (approximately 8 years post-injury time on average). There were no statistically significant differences in age, education, or gender between the groups (Table 1). There was a trend in which the TBI group participants were older than the controls. The trend in age differences between the groups was not associated with group differences in the network measures (See control analysis results). The estimated initial injury severity of the TBI participants was primarily mild, but the types of injury were diverse. Although the TBI participants were not clinically diagnosed with depression or PTSD (per inclusion criteria), the TBI group was higher on depressive symptom severity than the controls (p<.05). The TBI group had mild depressive symptoms on average, as measured by the BDI-II manual (Beck et al., 1996).
The average PCL-S score of the TBI group fell within the borderline range of suggested cut-off scores for PTSD screening in specialized medical clinics according to the U.S. Department of Veterans Affairs guidelines for PCL usage (http://www.ptsd.va.gov/professional/assessment/adult-sr/ptsd-checklist.asp). The presence of depressive and PTSD-related symptoms of these TBI participants was not surprising as comorbid psychiatric disorders or symptoms are common in chronic TBI (e.g., Zgaljardic et al., Reference Zgaljardic, Seale, Schaefer, Temple, Foreman and Elliott2015, for review). Thus, we included within-group-centered BDI-II score covariates in all subsequent group analyses, and assessed whether the presence of comorbid depressive symptoms in the TBI group systematically altered our findings with the full TBI samples (see the Control Analysis Results and Limitations and Future Research sections).
Neuropsychological Measures
There were no statistically significant group differences in estimated current IQ between the groups (Table 3). The TBI group did not show statistically significant differences between premorbid and current IQ. The TBI group showed significantly lower performance (p<.05) on the word reading condition of the color-word interference test and on the satisfaction with life scale. More TBI individuals than expected by chance (N=1; 2.5%) showed deficits in matrix reasoning, digit span forward and backward, all sub-domains of color-word interference, card sorting, and trail making, category switching (total correct) of verbal fluency, delayed recall, and satisfaction with life scale.
Network-Based Statistics Results
Consistent with the findings of Spreng et al. (Reference Spreng, Sepulcre, Turner, Stevens and Schacter2013), group-averaged connectivity matrices demonstrated a dissociable structure of DMN-DAN-FPCN (Figure 1A, B). Group comparisons of connectivity matrices revealed that the TBI group exhibited reductions in connectivity among the three networks compared to controls (Figure 1C, D). An anatomical view of reduced connectivity within the TBI group relative to the controls (Figure 2) highlights the fact that disruptions occurred more frequently in connections between the three networks compared to those within the networks. Furthermore, the cumulative distribution of the number of reduced connections in TBI over controls skewed toward long-range connections (Figure 3A), and notable disruptions occurred in inter-hemispheric connections (Figure 3B). Among the three types of between-network connections, DMN-FPCN and DMN-DAN were prominently disrupted (Figure 3C).
Whole-Brain Network Properties
Overall, the TBI group showed relative reductions in efficiency measures at the whole-brain level (Figure 4). The TBI group showed statistically significant reductions in (1) global and cost efficiency occurring for 0.1 ≤ K ≤ 0.17 and K=0.22 at p<.05 and K=0.08, 0.09 at p<.1 and (2) local efficiency for K=0.15, 0.22 at p<.05 and K=0.16, 0.17 at p<.1 (Figure 4A–C). Importantly, reductions in global and local efficiency occurred when the network was highly economical. Scatter plots for global and local efficiency at K=0.12 and 0.15, respectively, (Figure 4D–E) revealed relative reductions of the TBI group in these measures at the single-subject level and TBI individuals with “abnormally” low global and local efficiency. Taken together, notable reductions in global and local efficiency over high network cost in the TBI group indicate the detrimental effects of TBI-related disruptions in weak but important long-range, inter-hemispheric, and between-network connectivity (Figures 1–4).
Regional Network Properties
Assessments of the regional global and local efficiency for K=0.12 and 0.15, respectively, exhibited relative reductions in the global and local efficiency in the TBI group at each of the regions (Figure 5A, B). Of the two efficiency measures, the TBI group showed more prominent reductions in regional local efficiency than in regional global efficiency. Specifically, statistically significant group differences in regional global efficiency (p<.05) occurred in three regions [l-SFG, l-SPL, and l-MFG (BA6)] and noticeable group differences in the regional global efficiency (p<.1) occurred in eight regions (l-HF, l-IFG, r-FEF, r-SOG, r-aIPL, daCC, l-dlFPC, and msPFC). As for the regional local efficiency, statistically significant group differences (p<.05) occurred in 9 regions [l-aTL, r-HF, pCC, l-pIPL, r-FEF, l-aINS, l-dlPFC, l-MFG (BA6), and r-MFG (BA6)] and noticeable group differences (p<.1) occurred in other 13 regions (amPFC, l-HF, l-IFG, r-pIPL, PCu, r-aIPL, daCC, r-dlPFC, msPFC, l-MFG (BA 9), r-MFG (BA9), l-rlPFC, and r-rlPFC).
Control Analysis Results
Effects of demographics
Our motion analysis (Table 1) revealed that there were no statistically significant group differences in the percentage of motion-censored volumes or average FD after motion censoring and trimming. The number of connections that were correlated with age at α=0.05 was less than 5%, indicating minimal effects of age on the connectivity matrices in our groups. The global and local efficiency were not associated with the amount of motion (Supporting Figure S1) or age (Supporting Figure S2A, D) in any of the groups. PCL-S scores and post-injury times did not show an influence on the global or local efficiency (Supporting Figure S2B–C, E–F).
Neuropsychological deficits in the TBI group
More TBI participants than would be expected by chance (12/40 vs. 1/40 expected, p=.0007) had abnormally poor neuropsychological performance relative to the controls in more than two neuropsychological measures (Figure S3A), indicating overall deficits in neuropsychological measures for the TBI group. Group comparisons of global and local efficiency in the TBI sub-group with neuropsychological deficits (N=26) versus the TBI sub-group without any neuropsychological deficits (N=14) did not show statistically significant group differences (Figure S3B).
Effects of estimated initial injury severity
There were no systematic effects of estimated initial injury severity on the TBI participants’ BDI-II or PCL-S scores, six selected neuropsychological measures, or global or local efficiency (Supporting Figure S4). There were also no systematic effects of estimated initial injury severity on our NBS findings (Supporting Figure S5; see Supplementary Materials for details).
Effect of depressive symptoms
We found no systematic effects of comorbid depressive symptoms on our findings on NBS (Supporting Figure S6) or network efficiency measures (see Supplementary Materials for details).
Effects of a mixture of civilians and veterans with TBI
There were no systematic effects of military status (civilian or veteran with TBI) on our NBS findings (Supporting Figure S7) or network efficiency measures (see Supplementary Materials for details).
Whole-brain volumes
There were no statistically significant group differences in whole-brain volumes (Table 1).
Sustained DAIs
The TBI group had relatively reduced fractional anisotropy in the anterior thalamic radiation, corpus callosum, corticospinal tract, forceps major and minor, inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus, and superior and inferior longitudinal fasciculi, confirming sustained DAIs among the TBI individuals (Supporting Figure S8).
Effects of an initial threshold level on NBS
NBS analyses with two additional threshold levels (Supporting Figure S9) replicated the patterns of relatively reduced connectivity of the TBI group in Figure 2.
Partial correlation
Consistent with the full correlation analysis results, group comparisons of direct connectivity using partial correlation coefficients revealed relative reductions in connectivity for the TBI group (Supporting Figure S10). There were a few exceptions showing relatively increased connectivity (Supporting Figure S10). Again, NBS analyses with partial correlations allowed us to demonstrate disrupted patterns of the TBI group (Supporting Figure S11) consistent with the patterns in Figure 2.
Effects of global signal regression
Analyses of the global and local efficiency without GSR (Supporting Figure S12) essentially replicated the results after GSR in Figure 4 with a broader range of K.
Effects of short distance nodes
Retaining connectivity strengths between short distance nodes did not markedly alter disruption patterns of long-range connections (Supporting Figure S13).
Discussion
We characterized disrupted resting-state functional connectivity of DMN-DAN-FPCN in chronic TBI comparing to healthy individuals. We confirmed that there were disruptions in chronic TBI individuals not only within-network connectivity but also between-network connectivity (Figures 1–3). Of the three types of between-network connectivity, DMN-FPCN and DMN-DAN showed marked reductions in the TBI group (Figures 2–3). Furthermore, quantitative analysis revealed that the patterns of disruptions occurred predominantly in long-range and inter-hemispheric connections in the TBI group (Figure 3). Lastly, graph theoretic analyses demonstrated reduced efficiency in the TBI group over the controls at both whole-brain (Figure 4) and regional levels (Figure 5).
Advantages of Combining both Complex Network and Hypothesis-Driven Approaches to Identify Disrupted Networks in TBI
This study demonstrates the advantages of combining both complex network and hypothesis-driven approaches over either simple network or whole-brain-wide data-driven approaches only in identifying network disruptions in chronic TBI relevant to specific deficits. As such, comprehensive assessments of multiple networks-of-interest allowed us to identify markedly disrupted between-network connectivity of chronic TBI individuals (Figures 1–3). These pronounced disruptions in between-network connectivity in chronic TBI would have been missed if we had simply investigated the integrity of a single network or connectivity of a single-brain region with rest of the brain.
On the other hand, data-driven complex network approaches for TBI individuals at the whole-brain level (e.g., Han et al., Reference Han, Mac Donald, Johnson, Barnes, Wierzechowski, Zonies and Brody2014; Nakamura et al., Reference Nakamura, Hillary and Biswal2009) present challenges in interpreting behavioral consequences from the identified network disruptions. Our study translated the advantages of hypothesis-driven simple network approaches (i.e., more straightforward interpretations of the behavioral consequences of network disruptions) into complex network analysis by constraining the networks-of-interest in the context of goal-directed cognition. Combining the advantages of both approaches, we demonstrated that disrupted interactions of DMN-DAN-FPCN may be a potential neural mechanism underlying the deficits in goal-directed cognition observed in chronic TBI individuals. Note that this study does not provide direct evidence to support this hypothesis, thus further studies are needed to test it (see the Limitations and Future Research section). Given the demonstrated advantages of investigating multiple networks specific to goal-directed cognition, we encourage others to assess interactions among multiple networks-of-interest to better understand deficits in higher order cognitive functions such as reasoning, decision making, and selective attention in TBI and in other disorders involving white matter injuries or degeneration.
Findings Relative to Previous Studies
Reduced long-range connectivity of DMN-DAN-FPCN in chronic TBI
Our study demonstrated predominantly reduced strengths in long-range connectivity in the TBI group (Figures 2, 3A). This finding is consistent with the disruption-patterns reported in other functional connectivity studies in TBI (Castellanos et al., Reference Castellanos, Paul, Ordonez, Demuynck, Bajo, Campo and Maestu2010; Kumar, Rao, Chandramouli, & Pillai, Reference Kumar, Rao, Chandramouli and Pillai2009). Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) studies in TBI also revealed frequent abnormalities in long-range white matter tracts such as the superior and inferior longitudinal fasciculi, inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus (Messe et al., Reference Messe, Caplain, Paradot, Garrigue, Mineo, Soto Ares and Lehericy2011; Niogi et al., Reference Niogi, Mukherjee, Ghajar, Johnson, Kolster, Sarkar and McCandliss2008; Singh, Jeong, Hwang, Sungkarat, & Gruen, 2010; Smits et al., Reference Smits, Houston, Dippel, Wielopolski, Vernooij, Koudstaal and van der Lugt2011), cingulum bundles (Mac Donald et al., Reference Mac Donald, Johnson, Cooper, Nelson, Werner, Shimony and Brody2011; Niogi et al., Reference Niogi, Mukherjee, Ghajar, Johnson, Kolster, Sarkar and McCandliss2008), and fornix (Singh et al., 2010). Thus, observed disruptions in long-range functional connectivity among the three networks in chronic TBI may be attributed to abnormalities in long-range white matter tracts mediating multiple networks. However, future studies directly comparing structural and functional connectivity will be required to confirm this possibility as relationships between functional and structural connectivity may not be straightforward (Honey et al., 2009).
Reduced inter-hemispheric connectivity of DMN-DAN-FPCN in chronic TBI
Consistent with the reported whole-brain-wide reductions in inter-hemispheric functional connectivity of TBI individuals in previous studies (Kumar et al., Reference Kumar, Rao, Chandramouli and Pillai2009; Marquez de la Plata et al., Reference Marquez de la Plata, Garces, Shokri Kojori, Grinnan, Krishnan, Pidikiti and Diaz-Arrastia2011; Rigon, Duff, McAuley, Kramer, & Voss, Reference Rigon, Duff, McAuley, Kramer and Voss2015; Slobounov et al., Reference Slobounov, Gay, Zhang, Johnson, Pennell, Sebastianelli and Hallett2011; Sours et al., Reference Sours, Rosenberg, Kane, Roys, Zhuo, Shanmuganathan and Gullapalli2015), the TBI group showed relative reductions in inter-hemispheric connectivity of DMN-DAN-FPCN (Figures 2, 3B). Such reductions in inter-hemispheric functional connectivity may be attributable to damage to the corpus callosum, which is common in TBI (Niogi & Mukherjee, Reference Niogi and Mukherjee2010). Reductions in inter-hemispheric functional connectivity occur after corpus callosum resection in rhesus monkeys (O’Reilly et al., Reference O’Reilly, Croxson, Jbabdi, Sallet, Noonan, Mars and Baxter2013). However, further studies directly evaluating the effects of corpus callosum damage on connectivity in humans will be required to confirm this speculation. Nonetheless, our findings of reduced inter-hemispheric functional connectivity in chronic TBI indicate that damage to regions in one hemisphere impact intact regions in the other hemisphere, supporting the use of network approaches for chronic TBI individuals.
Disruptions in between-network connectivity of DMN-DAN-FPCN in chronic TBI
Marked reductions in between-network connectivity over within-network connectivity in our TBI group (Figures 1D, 2, 3C) are consistent with the disruption patterns reported in Han et al. (Reference Han, Mac Donald, Johnson, Barnes, Wierzechowski, Zonies and Brody2014) whereby veterans with sub-acute, concussive blast-related TBI showed marked reductions in between-module connectivity with minimal alterations in within-module connectivity. We extended efforts to identify altered between-network interactions following TBI. Bonnelle et al. (Reference Bonnelle, Ham, Leech, Kinnunen, Mehta, Greenwood and Sharp2012) demonstrated that the amount of white matter damage in the SN predicts the abnormality of the DMN in chronic TBI. In our study, we further revealed disrupted between-network interactions in TBI by directly measuring functional connectivity among the three networks.
Although our study was not able to directly show which cognitive deficits were associated with reduced DMN-FPCN connectivity in the TBI group, potentially the most relevant deficits may be autobiographical planning and future problem solving. Several studies (Baird, Smallwood, & Schooler, Reference Baird, Smallwood and Schooler2011; Gerlach, Spreng, Gilmore, & Schacter, Reference Gerlach, Spreng, Gilmore and Schacter2011; Gerlach, Spreng, Madore, & Schacter, Reference Gerlach, Spreng, Madore and Schacter2014; Spreng et al., Reference Spreng, Stevens, Chamberlain, Gilmore and Schacter2010) demonstrated that autobiographical planning and future problem solving tasks induce activity in the regions within both DMN and FPCN, presumably by engaging the autobiographical memory system and executive control process (Smallwood, Brown, Baird, & Schooler, Reference Smallwood, Brown, Baird and Schooler2012). In TBI, deficits in autobiographical content and their associations with everyday planning difficulties have also been reported (Dritschel, Kogan, Burton, Burton, & Goddard, Reference Dritschel, Kogan, Burton, Burton and Goddard1998).
Of interest, relative reductions in DMN-DAN connectivity in the TBI group occurred in connections where the controls retained positive connectivity (e.g., connections of the DAN with r-aTL, l-HF, and r-STS in the DMN). Prior literature indicated an antagonistic relationship in DMN-DAN, supported by observed anti-correlation between the DMN and DAN in Fox et al. (Reference Fox, Snyder, Vincent, Corbetta, Van Essen and Raichle2005) and Kelly, Uddin, Biswal, Castellanos, and Milham (Reference Kelly, Uddin, Biswal, Castellanos and Milham2008). Furthermore, disrupted anti-correlation between the DMN and task-related networks in TBI were demonstrated (Mayer et al., Reference Mayer, Mannell, Ling, Gasparovic and Yeo2011; Sours et al., Reference Sours, Zhuo, Janowich, Aarabi, Shanmuganathan and Gullapalli2013). Thus, relative reductions in positive DMN-DAN connectivity of the TBI group apparently contradicts previous findings of negative DMN-DAN connectivity (Fox et al., Reference Fox, Snyder, Vincent, Corbetta, Van Essen and Raichle2005; Kelly et al., Reference Kelly, Uddin, Biswal, Castellanos and Milham2008). However, Dixon, Fox, and Christoff (Reference Dixon, Fox and Christoff2014) proposed that activation within the DMN and DAN could co-occur with minimal interference if externally or internally directed cognition involves spontaneous processing such as in creative thinking (Ellamil, Dobson, Beeman, & Christoff, Reference Ellamil, Dobson, Beeman and Christoff2012) and the influence of self-evaluative thoughts on cognitive control (Bengtsson, Dolan, & Passingham, Reference Bengtsson, Dolan and Passingham2011).
Reduced connectivity versus elevated connectivity in TBI
We observed markedly reduced functional connectivity in the TBI individuals relative to the control participants; however, there is a body of literature that has reported elevated functional connectivity in TBI (Caeyenberghs et al., Reference Caeyenberghs, Leemans, Heitger, Leunissen, Dhollander, Sunaert and Swinnen2012, Reference Caeyenberghs, Leemans, Leunissen, Michiels and Swinnen2013; Hillary et al., Reference Hillary, Rajtmajer, Roman, Medaglia, Slocomb-Dluzen, Calhoun and Wylie2014; Nakamura et al., Reference Nakamura, Hillary and Biswal2009; Sharp et al., Reference Sharp, Beckmann, Greenwood, Kinnunen, Bonnelle, De Boissezon and Leech2011). Such discrepant reports in the literature may be attributable to several factors that differed across studies. First, Caeyenberghs et al. (Reference Caeyenberghs, Leemans, Heitger, Leunissen, Dhollander, Sunaert and Swinnen2012, Reference Caeyenberghs, Leemans, Leunissen, Michiels and Swinnen2013) and Hillary et al. (Reference Hillary, Rajtmajer, Roman, Medaglia, Slocomb-Dluzen, Calhoun and Wylie2014) reported their findings based on task-state functional connectivity. The directionality of task-state functional connectivity in our TBI group has not been established (see the Limitations and Future Research section in this regard).
Second, Caeyenberghs et al. (Reference Caeyenberghs, Leemans, Heitger, Leunissen, Dhollander, Sunaert and Swinnen2012, Reference Caeyenberghs, Leemans, Leunissen, Michiels and Swinnen2013) and Hillary et al. (Reference Hillary, Rajtmajer, Roman, Medaglia, Slocomb-Dluzen, Calhoun and Wylie2014) obtained partial-correlations, while we obtained both full- and partial-correlations. Although our TBI group showed relatively reduced full-correlation coefficients, relative increases in connectivity were also observed over some connections with the partial correlation analysis (Supporting Figures S10, S11).
Third, the directionality of abnormal connectivity in individuals with TBI can vary with seed regions, networks-of-interests, and different definition of nodes [e.g., within- vs. between-module connectivity in Han et al. (Reference Han, Mac Donald, Johnson, Barnes, Wierzechowski, Zonies and Brody2014)], and these parameters in our study were different than the other studies that have reported elevated connectivity (Caeyenberghs et al., Reference Caeyenberghs, Leemans, Heitger, Leunissen, Dhollander, Sunaert and Swinnen2012, Reference Caeyenberghs, Leemans, Leunissen, Michiels and Swinnen2013; Hillary et al., Reference Hillary, Rajtmajer, Roman, Medaglia, Slocomb-Dluzen, Calhoun and Wylie2014; Nakamura et al., Reference Nakamura, Hillary and Biswal2009).
Fourth, differences in sample characteristics could play a role in the directionality of abnormal functional connectivity in TBI. As such, Hillary et al. (Reference Hillary, Rajtmajer, Roman, Medaglia, Slocomb-Dluzen, Calhoun and Wylie2014) and Nakamura et al. (Reference Nakamura, Hillary and Biswal2009) assessed TBI individuals at 3 and 6 months post-injury, whereas our TBI individuals were sampled at several years post-injury.
Fifth, to assess connectivity in TBI, Sharp et al. (Reference Sharp, Beckmann, Greenwood, Kinnunen, Bonnelle, De Boissezon and Leech2011) performed ICA and dual regression, while we performed the Pearson correlation. Note that the ICA-plus-dual-regression and Pearson correlation-based approaches are conceptually different (e.g., Joel, Caffo, van Zijl, & Pekar, Reference Joel, Caffo, van Zijl and Pekar2011). The ICA-plus-dual-regression approach decomposes resting-state fMRI signal at a voxel according to the each of the networks (i.e., the within-network component only). Thus, the ICA-plus-dual-regression approach does not take account the between-network component of connectivity at the voxel-level (although ICA can yield between-network connectivity at the network-level using network-level time-courses).
Indeed, the same research group responsible for the study of Sharp et al. (Reference Sharp, Beckmann, Greenwood, Kinnunen, Bonnelle, De Boissezon and Leech2011) used the Pearson correlation which takes account for between-network components at the region-level and they have demonstrated overall reductions in resting-state functional connectivity in individuals with TBI (Pandit et al., Reference Pandit, Expert, Lambiotte, Bonnelle, Leech, Turkheimer and Sharp2013). Furthermore, the dual-regression controls for the time-courses of networks other than the given network-of-interest. This is conceptually similar to the partial correlation approach that controls for correlations of time-courses at other regions outside a pair of regions-of-interest. Note that, when we used the partial correlation approach, we observed relatively elevated connectivity for our TBI group primarily in within-network connections. Taken together, mixed reports on reduced and elevated connectivity in TBI literature demonstrate complex nature of TBI and a strong need for additional research across methods and populations with TBI. We did evaluate the potential effects of depressive symptoms and PTSD scores for TBI participants on our results, but did not find substantive differences in network connectivity attributable to these factors.
Reduced efficiencies of DMN-DAN-FPCN in chronic TBI
Graph theoretic analyses revealed the consequences of impaired long-range, inter-hemispheric and between-network connectivity of DMN-DAN-FPCN in chronic TBI (Figures 4, 5). In healthy subjects, functional brain networks are “economical” in that the brain exhibits high global and local efficiency of parallel information processing given low wiring cost (Achard & Bullmore, Reference Achard and Bullmore2007). In our TBI group, network disruptions led to individuals circumventing impaired weak-but-efficient long-range, inter-hemispheric and between-network connections by using less efficient and “noisier” alternative paths for communications. Such selections of alternative paths in DMN-DAN-FPCN implicates DMN-DAN-FPCN connectivity of the TBI group were inefficient for parallel information flow and less tolerant to additional injuries to brain regions, demonstrated by relatively low global and local efficiency, respectively (Latora & Marchiori, Reference Latora and Marchiori2001).
Our findings of reduced global and local efficiency in chronic TBI are consistent with a previous finding on reduced efficiency in chronic TBI (Pandit et al., Reference Pandit, Expert, Lambiotte, Bonnelle, Leech, Turkheimer and Sharp2013). However, our findings are inconsistent with the study results of Nakamura et al. (Reference Nakamura, Hillary and Biswal2009) demonstrating no group differences in efficiency measures at the chronic stage. Such inconsistencies may be explained by the different group characteristics (N=6 and severe TBI) and methodologies (whole-brain connectivity) in Nakamura et al. (Reference Nakamura, Hillary and Biswal2009). We extend upon previous studies (Nakamura et al., Reference Nakamura, Hillary and Biswal2009; Pandit et al., Reference Pandit, Expert, Lambiotte, Bonnelle, Leech, Turkheimer and Sharp2013) by providing critical new information on reduced efficiency of the brain networks in TBI in that statistically significant reductions in global and local efficiency of the TBI group occurred when cost efficiency was optimal. Such findings suggest that chronic TBI individuals would have less optimal axonal wiring relative to controls or differences in metabolic running costs to provide parallel information processing among DMN-DAN-FPCN even under the individual’s most economical network settings (Achard & Bullmore, Reference Achard and Bullmore2007).
Limitations and Future Research
The present study has several limitations. First, our TBI group was comprised of a mixture of individuals with probable mild, probable moderate, and probable severe TBI, which yields it less comparable to studies of individuals in the sub-acute (3–6 months post-injury) and short-term chronic (6 months–2 years post-injury) stages of TBI. However, at the long-term chronic stage of TBI (>2 years post-injury), initial injury severity often plays less critical role in characterizing TBI individuals at the time of study (Arciniegas et al., Reference Arciniegas, Olincy, Topkoff, McRae, Cawthra, Filley and Adler2000; Kinnunen et al., Reference Kinnunen, Greenwood, Powell, Leech, Hawkins, Bonnelle and Sharp2010) and chronic TBI studies occasionally have reported a mixture of different injury severity levels (Bonnelle et al., Reference Bonnelle, Ham, Leech, Kinnunen, Mehta, Greenwood and Sharp2012, Reference Bonnelle, Leech, Kinnunen, Ham, Beckmann, De Boissezon and Sharp2011; Ham et al., Reference Ham, Bonnelle, Hellyer, Jilka, Robertson, Leech and Sharp2014; Kinnunen et al., Reference Kinnunen, Greenwood, Powell, Leech, Hawkins, Bonnelle and Sharp2010; Schönberger et al., Reference Schönberger, Ponsford, Reutens, Beare, Clarke and O’Sullivan2011; Sharp et al., Reference Sharp, Beckmann, Greenwood, Kinnunen, Bonnelle, De Boissezon and Leech2011). Furthermore, the fifth edition of American Psychiatric Association’s Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-V) suggests that initial injury severity does not necessarily correspond to the severity of resulting neuro-cognitive disorders after TBI, and that the course of recovery depends not only on the specifics of the injury but also on co-factors such as age, prior history of brain damage, and history of substance abuse.
In our case, there were no systematic effects of estimated initial injury severity on the BDI-II or PCL-S scores, selected neuropsychological measures, efficiency measures, or patterns of disrupted connectivity (Supporting Figures S4, S5). Nonetheless, care should be taken in interpreting our findings with regard to TBI severity as initial injury severity was retrospectively estimated.
Second, a portion of the TBI sample showed comorbid depressive symptoms, which are common in chronic TBI populations. Although comorbid psychiatric symptoms are common in chronic TBI and the presence of depressive symptoms did not alter our findings (Supporting Figures S6, S7), care should be taken due to (1) potential bias in self-reported BDI-II scores driven by frequent impairments in self-awareness among TBI individuals (Malec, Testa, Rush, Brown, & Moessner, 2007) and (2) relatively small sample size of the TBI sub-groups.
Third, the behavioral consequences of the phenomena reported in this study are as yet unclear. To identify behavioral consequences of disrupted between-network connectivity in chronic TBI, task-based fMRI brain activation studies are of interest.
Fourth, it is unknown that how disrupted resting-state functional connectivity among DMN-DAN-FPCN in TBI could influence “reconfiguration” of functional connectivity in specific tasks.
Fifth, we excluded negative correlations in our network analyses due to ongoing debates about the meaning of negative correlations after GSR (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Druzgal, Lopez-Larson, Jeong, Desai and Yurgelun-Todd2011; Chai, Castanon, Ongur, & Whitfield-Gabrieli, Reference Chai, Castanon, Ongur and Whitfield-Gabrieli2012; Chang & Glover, Reference Chang and Glover2009; Fox, Zhang, Snyder, & Raichle, Reference Fox, Zhang, Snyder and Raichle2009; Murphy, Birn, Handwerker, Jones, & Bandettini, Reference Murphy, Birn, Handwerker, Jones and Bandettini2009; Saad et al., Reference Saad, Gotts, Murphy, Chen, Jo, Martin and Cox2012). Note that, even though alternative approaches to reliably estimate negative correlations have been proposed (e.g., Jo, Saad, Simmons, Milbury, & Cox, 2010), the meaning of negative correlations are unclear at the present time. As such, negative correlations in rsfMRI could reflect accumulated phase delay along the path that connects two regions with positively correlated multiple edges (Chen, Chen, Xie, & Li, Reference Chen, Chen, Xie and Li2011), rather than an antagonistic relationship between two regions exhibiting a negative correlation. Assessment of anti-correlations among the three networks would be of interest if future studies clarify the meaning of negative correlations.
Our future works include an assessment of relationship between disrupted networks and behavioral consequences in this cohort and efforts to address the other concerns discussed above. Furthermore, we will identify if and how disrupted connectivity among DMN-DAN-FPCN in chronic TBI could be reorganized following rehabilitation.
In conclusion, we demonstrated pronounced disruptions in DMN-DAN-FPCN connectivity. Our findings suggest that the three networks should be analyzed together to better understand deficits in goal-directed cognition and other higher order cognitive functions in chronic TBI. Further studies are required to explain the behavioral consequences of the phenomena reported in this study.
a See Spreng et al., Reference Spreng, Sepulcre, Turner, Stevens and Schacter2013, for coordinates and network affiliations for the listed regions.
Note: WASI, Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence; FSIQ, Full Scale Intelligent Quotient; WTAR, Wechsler Test of Adult Reading.
a Mean and standard deviation values were reported.
b p-Values were obtained with age, years of education and within-group-centered BDI-II score covariates.*, † indicate p<0.05, p<0.1, respectively.
c The number of “abnormal” TBI participants whose neuropsychological measures were outside (either above for the color-word and the trail making tests or below for the other measures) the two-standard deviation-band from the mean of the controls. Data in boldface indicate that the number of TBI participants who performed “abnormally” poor in a given neuropsychological measure was higher than the number of “abnormal” TBI participants expected to occur by chance [i.e., 1 out of 40 (2.5%)].
d WASI FSIQ-2 versus WTAR FSIQ within the TBI group.
e Unavailable for one healthy individual due to timer malfunction.
Acknowledgments
We thank all study participants and their families who supported this study. This work has been supported by Department of Defense CDMRP grants W81XWH-11-2-0194 to DCK and W81XWH-11-2-0195 to S.B.C. and a grant from the Meadows Foundation to D.C.K. and S.B.C. The authors declare no conflicts of interest.
Supplementary material
To view supplementary material for this article, please visit http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1355617715001393