Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rcrh6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-27T14:00:55.930Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Variety in fruit and vegetable intake and cognitive function in middle-aged and older Puerto Rican adults

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 May 2012

Xingwang Ye
Affiliation:
Department of Health Sciences, Northeastern University, 316 Robinson Hall, 360 Huntington Avenue, Boston, MA02115, USA
Shilpa N. Bhupathiraju
Affiliation:
Department of Health Sciences, Northeastern University, 316 Robinson Hall, 360 Huntington Avenue, Boston, MA02115, USA
Katherine L. Tucker*
Affiliation:
Department of Health Sciences, Northeastern University, 316 Robinson Hall, 360 Huntington Avenue, Boston, MA02115, USA
*
*Corresponding author: K. L. Tucker, fax +1 617 373 2968, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Higher variety in fruit and vegetable intake has been associated with a lower risk of several chronic diseases. It remains unclear whether such associations exist relating to cognition. The authors examined associations between total quantity and variety in fruit and vegetable intake and cognitive function in a cross-sectional sample of 1412 Puerto Rican adults, aged 45–75 years from the Boston Puerto Rican Health Study, 2004–9. Fruit and vegetable intake was assessed with a FFQ. Cognitive function was measured with a battery of seven tests; the Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) was administrated to assess global cognitive function. Greater variety, but not total quantity, of fruit and vegetable intake was associated with a higher MMSE score after multivariate adjustment (P for trend = 0·012). This association remained significant after further adjusting for total quantity of fruit and vegetable intake (P for trend = 0·018). High variety of fruit and vegetable intake was also associated with individual cognitive domains, including executive function, memory and attention (all P for trend < 0·05). Variety, more than total quantity, of fruit and vegetable intake may offer cognitive protection in middle-aged and older adults, but longitudinal studies are needed to clarify direction of causality.

Type
Full Papers
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2012

Greater fruit and/or vegetable intake has been associated with a lower risk of several chronic diseases, including CHD(Reference Dauchet, Amouyel and Hercberg1, Reference He, Nowson and Lucas2), stroke(Reference Dauchet, Amouyel and Dallongeville3, Reference He, Nowson and MacGregor4) and several types of cancers(Reference Koushik, Hunter and Spiegelman5Reference Pavia, Pileggi and Nobile7). A higher intake of vegetables has also been associated with less cognitive decline(Reference Kang, Ascherio and Grodstein8, Reference Morris, Evans and Tangney9); and a lower intake of fruit and vegetables has been related to poorer executive function and memory(Reference Sabia, Nabi and Kivimaki10) and a higher risk of dementia and Alzheimer's disease(Reference Hughes, Andel and Small11).

Fruit and vegetables are generally rich in antioxidants (vitamin C, carotenoids, etc.) and other bioactive compounds. Dietary antioxidants have been associated with a lower risk of dementia and Alzheimer's disease in some studies(Reference Engelhart, Geerlings and Ruitenberg12, Reference Morris, Evans and Bienias13), but not others(Reference Laurin, Masaki and Foley14, Reference Luchsinger, Tang and Shea15). However, several intervention studies with antioxidant vitamins have not shown beneficial effects on cognition in older adults(Reference Yaffe, Clemons and McBee16Reference Grodstein, Kang and Glynn19). The inconsistency among these studies could be due to differences in study design, or to limitations in the supplements provided – in that other nutrients or bioactive compounds in fruit and vegetables, rather than investigated specific antioxidant vitamins, may be protective against cognitive decline. Further, different fruits and vegetables have varied nutrients and bioactive compounds. The combination of different types of fruits and vegetables may provide additive and/or synergistic beneficial effects relative to individual fruits and vegetables(Reference Liu20).

To our knowledge, no studies have examined the association between variety in fruit and vegetable intake and cognitive function. We aimed to simultaneously study the associations between variety and total quantity of fruit and vegetable intake and cognitive function in a middle-aged and older population.

Materials and methods

Participants

Participants in the baseline interview of the Boston Puerto Rican Health Study, an ongoing study to explore sociological, environmental and genetic factors contributing to ageing-related chronic diseases and quality of life, were included in these analyses(Reference Tucker, Mattei and Noel21). Details of this study design have been reported previously(Reference Tucker, Mattei and Noel21). In brief, baseline data collection was completed between 2004 and 2009. Census tracts with twenty-five or more Puerto Rican adults, aged 45–75 years, were selected from the year 2000 census in the greater Boston, Massachusetts area, and all blocks with ten or more Hispanic adults, aged 45–75 years, were enumerated. If more than one household member qualified for the study, one was randomly selected. Approximately 77·4 % of our sample was identified with this method, with additional participants solicited randomly at major community events (9·8 %), from referral (7·2 %) or individual calls to the study (5·6 %). Of the 2170 individuals identified, seventy-seven were excluded because of serious health conditions that would preclude answering questions, a planned move away within 2 years or a lack of a permanent address. Of those remaining, 1811 agreed to participate and 1500 (83·2 %) completed the baseline survey: 302 participants did not finish the survey due to difficulty in scheduling or unsuccessful follow-up; and nine were excluded because of their low Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) scores ( ≤ 10, indicating severe cognitive impairment)(Reference Mungas22). For the present analysis, we further excluded individuals with implausible energy intake ( < 2510 kJ/d (600 kcal/d) or >20 083 kJ/d (4800 kcal/d)) and those with incomplete cognitive tests or blood lipids, resulting in a sample of 1412. The present study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki and all procedures involving human subjects were approved by the Institutional Review Board at Tufts Medical Center. Written informed consent was obtained from all subjects.

Variety and quantity of fruit and vegetable intake

Information about dietary intake was collected for the previous 12 months with a validated semi-quantitative FFQ with 223 items, interviewer-administered in the home(Reference Tucker, Mattei and Noel21). Nutrient intake was calculated using the Nutrition Data System for Research software version 2007 (Nutrition Coordinating Center, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, USA, Food and Nutrient Database 2007). The number of servings of fruit and vegetables per d was calculated using reference serving sizes from the Pyramid Servings Database for USDA Survey Food Codes version 2.0(Reference Cook and Friday23). The FFQ includes questions for the intake of specific fruits, vegetables, 100 % fruit juice and 100 % vegetable juice(Reference Bhupathiraju and Tucker24). Dried beans and starchy vegetables (potatoes and potato products, plantains, tannier and cassava) were not included for the calculation of total quantity and variety in fruit and vegetable intake. Each specific type of fruit, vegetable, 100 % fruit juice or 100 % vegetable juice that was consumed at least once per month was counted towards the variety score. The possible ranges for the summed variety scores were from 0 to 27 for fruit intake (including 100 % fruit juice) and from 0 to 26 for vegetable intake (including 100 % vegetable juice). The FFQ fruit and vegetable items are shown in Table S1 (available online).

Assessment of cognitive function

A trained research assistant administered a battery of seven neuropsychological tests with each participant, in his or her home, before the FFQ data were collected. The MMSE was used as a measure of global cognitive function (ranged from 12 to 30 in the present sample)(Reference Folstein, Folstein and McHugh25). Other cognitive tests included: a word-list learning test to assess verbal memory, with sub-scores for learning and immediate recall, recognition, and percentage retention(Reference Artiola Fortuny, Hermosillo Romo and Heaton26); digit span forward and backward tests to measure attention and working memory(Reference Artiola Fortuny, Hermosillo Romo and Heaton26); the Stroop test for cognitive flexibility, response inhibition and processing speed(Reference Tucker, Mattei and Noel21); the verbal fluency test to measure the speed at which one can provide examples to a category (initial letter of a word)(Reference Artiola Fortuny, Hermosillo Romo and Heaton26); clock-drawing(Reference Wolf-Klein, Silverstone and Levy27) and figure-copying(Reference Beery28) tests to measure visual–spatial organisation. Scores for figure copying were weighted for the complexity of the figure, by assigning 1 point for easy figures and to 4 points for the most difficult. Greater scores on each of these seven tests reflect better cognition.

The MMSE score was our primary outcome because the MMSE measures global cognitive function. Scores from each neuropsychological test were standardised with a mean of 0 and a standard deviation of 1. A factor analysis for data reduction was then conducted with standardised scores. After performing varimax rotation, three meaningful cognitive function factors were obtained: executive function; memory; attention (Table S2, available online). The standardised factor-scoring coefficients for converting individual test scores to factor scores are shown in Table S3 (available online).

Assessment of covariates

Educational attainment was categorised into four categories: none or primary school; secondary school; high school; college or higher. Participants were classified as never smoker ( < 100 cigarettes in entire life), former smoker or current smoker. Alcohol use was classified as not current, current moderate ( ≤ 1 drink/d for women or  ≤ 2 drinks/d for men) or current heavy (>1 drink/d for women or >2 drinks/d for men). Physical activity was evaluated with a modified Paffenbarger questionnaire from the Harvard Alumni Activity Survey(Reference Tucker, Mattei and Noel21). Poverty was defined ‘yes’ if a participant's total annual household income was below the poverty threshold released each year by the US Department of Health and Human Services, relative to family size. For example, the poverty threshold for a family with two members was $13 690 for Massachusetts residents in 2007(29). An acculturation score was calculated based on answers to seven questions regarding the extent of use of English and/or Spanish for work and daily life(Reference Tucker, Mattei and Noel21), ranging from 0 (only using Spanish) to 100 (only using English). Physical disability was assessed with a modified Activities of Daily Living (ADL) scale with twelve items with total scores ranging from 0 to 36(Reference Tucker, Falcon and Bianchi30). Greater ADL scores indicate lower physical function.

During the home interview, height and weight were measured with standard methods(Reference Tucker, Mattei and Noel21), and BMI was calculated. Blood pressure was measured at three time points during the home interview with the latter two measures averaged for data analysis. Hypertension was defined as systolic blood pressure  ≥ 140 mmHg or diastolic blood pressure  ≥ 90 mmHg, or use of antihypertensive medications. Fasting blood samples were collected and concentrations of glucose, total cholesterol, HDL-cholesterol and TAG were determined with standardised methods(Reference Tucker, Mattei and Noel21). Diabetes was defined as ‘yes’ if a participant had fasting glucose  ≥ 7·0 mmol/l or was taking medication for diabetes(31).

Statistical analyses

Participants were placed in quintiles according to the total number of servings and variety score of fruit and vegetable intake, respectively. General linear models were used to compare continuous variables and logistic regression models were used to compare categorical outcome variables across quintiles of total intake quantity and variety scores. Means in the last four quintiles were compared with means in the first quintile, after Tukey's adjustment for multiple comparisons. Cognitive performance scores for each quintile were calculated after multivariate adjustment with general linear models. In the first model, we adjusted for age (years), sex, educational attainment (none or primary school, secondary school, high school, or college or higher), income below the poverty threshold (yes/no), acculturation score (continuous), BMI (kg/m2) and total energy intake (kJ/d). In a second model, we further adjusted for potential confounders and mediators, including smoking status (never, past smoker or current smoker), alcohol use (never, moderate or high), physical activity score (continuous), supplement use (yes/no), use of five or more types of medications within the past 12 months (yes/no), ADL score (continuous), hypertension (yes/no) and diabetes (yes/no). In a final full model, we further adjusted for the variety score for the total serving variable, and vice versa. Original continuous total servings and variety score were used for trend tests. We also investigated whether lipids (total cholesterol, HDL-cholesterol and TAG) or the dietary intake of PUFA and MUFA would attenuate the associations between total servings and variety score and cognitive function. In a secondary analysis, we repeated all analyses to examine whether fruit intake (servings v. variety) or vegetable intake (servings v. variety) was individually associated with cognitive function. All statistical analyses were conducted with SAS version 9.2 (SAS Institute, Inc.). A value of P< 0·05 (two-sided) was considered statistically significant.

Results

Participants in the highest quintile of fruit and vegetable intake had a 4·6-fold higher intake than those in the first quintile (median 6·5 v. 1·4 servings/d; Table 1), and those in the highest quintile of variety in fruit and vegetable intake consumed twice the variety of those in the lowest quintile. Those with higher fruit and vegetable intake, or with higher fruit and vegetable variety, tended to have education at the college level or higher, to be not current smokers, to drink alcohol moderately, to have a higher physical activity score, a lower ADL score, and to have a higher intake of total energy and a higher use of supplements, relative to those with the lowest quantity or variety in fruit and vegetable intake, respectively. Participants with the highest variety scores also tended to be female, to be more acculturated, above the poverty line, to have higher total and HDL-cholesterol concentrations, and were less likely to have diabetes and to use medications, than those with the lowest variety scores. The total quantity of fruit and vegetable intake was moderately associated with the variety score (Spearman's correlation coefficient 0·34, P< 0·001).

Table 1 Characteristics of the participants by quintiles of total quantity and variety in fruit and vegetable intake from the Boston Puerto Rican Health Study, 2004–9 (Mean values with their standard errors; number of participants; percentages; medians and ranges)

ADL, Activities of Daily Living.

Mean values were significantly different compared with the lowest quintile after Tukey's adjustment for multiple comparisons: *P< 0·05, **P< 0·01, ***P< 0·001.

P values for ANOVA for continuous variables across quintiles of total quantity and variety in fruit and vegetable intake were calculated by PROC GLM with SAS 9.2, respectively, and χ2 test for categorical variables across quintiles was performed by PROC FREQ.

Medication use was defined as yes if a participant had taken more than five types of medications within the past 12 months.

Higher variety in fruit and vegetable intake (P for trend < 0·001), but not quantity (P for trend = 0·13), was associated with a greater MMSE score after adjustment for age, sex, educational attainment, household income, acculturation score, BMI and total energy intake (model 1; Table 2). The association between variety in fruit and vegetable intake and the MMSE score remained significant after further adjustment for smoking, alcohol use, physical activity score, supplement use, medication use, ADL score, hypertension and diabetes (model 2, P for trend = 0·012), and total quantity of fruit and vegetable intake (model 3, P for trend = 0·018). Each of the seven points of the fruit and vegetable variety score (β= 0·17, se= 0·07, P= 0·012) was also inversely similar to 5 years of age on MMSE scores (β=  − 0·16, se= 0·07, P= 0·018), after multivariate adjustment in model 3. Higher variety score was also associated with better executive function, memory and attention (model 1–3, all P values for trend < 0·05). Further adjustment for blood lipids (total cholesterol, HDL-cholesterol and TAG) and dietary intake of PUFA and MUFA did not change the observed associations mentioned above (data not shown).

Table 2 Cognitive performance by quintile of total quantity and variety in fruit and vegetable intake from the Boston Puerto Rican Health Study, 2004–9 (Mean values with their standard errors)

MMSE, Mini Metal State Examination.

Mean values were significantly different compared with the lowest quintile after Tukey's adjustment for multiple comparisons: *P< 0·05, **P< 0·01, ***P< 0·001.

Results of the omitted quintiles generally follow the pattern of those of the shown quintiles, but are not presented for simplicity; adjusted means (with their standard errors) of cognitive performance scores according to quintiles of total quantity and variety in fruit and vegetable intake were calculated using general linear models (PROC GLM in SAS 9.2) after adjustment for covariates, respectively.

P values for trend were calculated using total quantity and variety in fruit and vegetable intake with PROC GLM in SAS 9.2, respectively.

§ Model 1, adjusted for age (years), sex, educational attainment (none or primary school, secondary school, high school, or college or higher), household income below the threshold (yes/no), acculturation score (continuous), BMI (kg/m2) and total energy intake (kJ/d).

Model 2, further adjusted for smoking status (never, past smoker or current smoker), alcohol use (never, moderate or high), physical activity score (continuous), supplement use (yes/no), taking more than five types of medications within the past 12 months (yes/no), activities of daily living scores (continuous), hypertension (yes/no) and diabetes (yes/no).

Model 3, further adjusted for fruit and vegetable variety for fruit and vegetable intake and vice versa.

We further examined the associations between total quantity and variety in fruit and vegetable intake and the scores of individual cognitive tests (Table 3). A higher fruit and vegetable variety score was significantly associated with better scores for word-list immediate recall, word-list percentage recognition and retention, letter fluency, digit span forward and backward, and clock drawing, after multivariate adjustment including total intake of fruits and vegetables (all P values for trend < 0·05).

Table 3 Individual cognitive test performance by quintile of total quantity and variety in fruit and vegetable intake from the Boston Puerto Rican Health Study, 2004–9 (Mean values with their standard errors)

Mean values were significantly different compared with the lowest quintile after Tukey's adjustment for multiple comparisons: *P< 0·05, ***P< 0·001.

Results of the omitted quintiles generally follow the pattern of those of the shown quintiles, but are not presented for simplicity; adjusted means (with their standard errors) of cognitive performance scores (standardised with a mean of 0 and a standard deviation of 1) according to quintiles of total quantity and variety in fruit and vegetable intake were calculated using general linear models (PROC GLM in SAS 9.2), respectively, after adjustment for each other (original continuous values) and age (years), sex, educational attainment (none or primary school, secondary school, high school, or college or higher), household income below the threshold (yes/no), acculturation score (continuous), BMI (kg/m2), total energy intake (kJ/d), smoking status (never, past smoker or current smoker), alcohol use (never, moderate or high), physical activity score (continuous), supplement use (yes/no), taking more than five types of medications within the past 12 months (yes/no), activities of daily living scores (continuous), hypertension (yes/no), diabetes (yes/no) and fruit and vegetable variety for fruit and vegetable intake and vice versa.

Sample mean and standard deviation of the corresponding original cognitive test score.

§ P values for trend were calculated using total quantity and variety in fruit and vegetable intake with PROC GLM in SAS 9.2, respectively.

In secondary analyses, higher variety, but not quantity, of fruit intake was significantly associated with a higher MMSE score, and with the executive function, memory and attention factors, after multivariate adjustment (model 1–3, all P values for trend < 0·05; Table S4, available online). Higher quantity and variety in vegetable intake were both significantly associated with a greater MMSE score after adjustment for age, sex, education, household income below the threshold, acculturation score, BMI and total energy intake (P for trend < 0·05; Table S5, available online). Variety, but not quantity, of vegetable intake remained significantly associated with the MMSE score after further adjustment for lifestyle factors, supplement and medication uses, ADL score, diabetes and hypertension (P for trend = 0·024). Higher variety in vegetable intake was marginally significantly associated with the MMSE score (P for trend = 0·065) and memory (P for trend = 0·067), and significantly associated with executive function and attention (both P for trend < 0·05) after further adjustment for total quantity of vegetable intake.

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first study to show that greater variety in fruit and vegetable intake was significantly associated with better cognitive function in middle-aged and older adults. Further, this association was maintained after adjustment for total quantity of fruit and vegetable intake.

Available studies about the association between fruit and vegetable intake and cognitive function are limited. In the Nurses' Health Study, intake of some types of vegetables, but neither of fruit, nor total fruit and vegetable intake, was associated with less cognitive decline after a 2-year follow-up(Reference Kang, Ascherio and Grodstein8). In the Chicago Health and Aging Project, with adults aged  ≥ 65 years, vegetable intake, but not citrus fruit intake, was associated with a slower rate of cognitive decline after a 6-year follow-up(Reference Morris, Evans and Tangney9). We did not observe significant associations between the total intake of fruit, the combination of fruit and vegetables and the MMSE score or individual cognitive domains (executive function, memory and attention). The total quantity of vegetable intake was significantly associated with the MMSE score only before adjustment for lifestyle factors, supplement and medication use, ADL score, diabetes and hypertension.

In contrast, higher variety in fruit and vegetable intake was associated with a better MMSE score, executive function, attention, memory function and several individual tests. These associations remained significant or continued to approach significance, even after adjustment for total quantity. The present results support the Dietary Guidelines for Americans, 2010(32) recommendation to increase variety in fruit and vegetable intake.

Fruit and vegetables are rich sources of antioxidant vitamins, B vitamins, minerals and other bio-compounds such as polyphenols. Our previous analysis has shown that higher variety in fruit and vegetable intake was significantly correlated with greater blood concentrations of carotenoids and ascorbic acid(Reference Bhupathiraju and Tucker24). Oxidative stress has been indicated in the pathophysiological process of cognitive impairment and dementia(Reference Pratico, Clark and Liun33, Reference Lovell and Markesbery34). Antioxidants in fruit and vegetables may protect against neurodegeneration by scavenging free radicals. Nonetheless, previous studies have yielded inconsistent results about the associations between the intake of conventional antioxidant vitamins and cognitive decline and dementia(Reference Yaffe, Clemons and McBee16Reference Grodstein, Kang and Glynn19). Limited data suggest that non-vitamin antioxidants may have higher neuroprotective capacity than antioxidant vitamins(Reference Dai, Borenstein and Wu35). Further, different fruits and vegetables have varied bioactive components. Therefore, the combination of multiple types of fruits and vegetables may have additive and/or synergistic effects on physiology above that of isolated nutrients or individual foods. Wang et al. (Reference Wang, Meckling and Marcone36) recently demonstrated that there were additive or synergistic effects on the total antioxidant capacity of the combination of specific foods within food categories (fruit or vegetable). More importantly, the combination of specific foods across categories was more likely to have synergistic effects on the total antioxidant capacity than the combinations within categories(Reference Wang, Meckling and Marcone36). Their data support our present findings that higher variety in fruit and vegetable intake, relative to total quantity, was more strongly associated with better cognitive function.

The observed associations between higher variety in fruit and vegetable intake and cognitive function could also be due to other nutrients and/or bio-compounds. For example, low folate concentrations have been observed among those with neurodegenerative disorders(Reference Mattson and Shea37). A higher dietary folate intake has been linked with a slower decline of cognitive function(Reference Tucker, Qiao and Scott38). In addition, inflammatory factors have been associated with poor cognitive function, cognitive impairment and dementia(Reference Komulainen, Lakka and Kivipelto39). Higher variety in fruit and vegetable intake was also associated with lower C-reactive protein (a chronic low-grade inflammatory biomarker) in our previous analysis(Reference Bhupathiraju and Tucker24).

Strengths of the present study include a large sample size and our ability to adjust for a variety of covariates. We were able to examine the independent associations between variety in fruit and vegetable intake and cognitive function because the correlation between total quantity and variety in fruit and vegetable intake was moderate. We administrated a series of cognitive tests to examine several domains of cognition. However, due to the cross-sectional nature of the present analysis, we cannot infer a casual direction to our observed association. It is possible that those with lower cognitive function may change their dietary intake, or may recall their dietary intake in a less accurate way than others. However, we have previously shown that interviewer-administrated FFQ reporting may be valid in those with cognitive impairment, due to the superior retention of generic memory, relative to episodic memory for specific events, in most individuals(Reference Arsenault, Matthan and Scott40). Additionally, residual confounding remains a possibility. While our sample is restricted to the Puerto Rican population, there is no reason to suspect that the present results cannot be generalised to other populations or minority groups.

In conclusion, greater variety in fruit and vegetable intake was associated with better cognitive function in middle-aged and older adults. An emphasis on increasing variety, rather than only quantity of fruits and vegetables, may be advisable for this and other populations, to delay or prevent the onset of cognitive impairment and related chronic diseases.

Acknowledgements

This study was supported by the National Institute on Aging of the National Institutes of Health (NIH) (P01AG023394, R01AG02708), the National Heart Lung and Blood Institute of NIH (P50HL105185), and the US Department of Agriculture Agricultural Research Service (contract no. 58–1950–7–707). X. Y. contributed to the data analysis, the interpretation of the data and the writing of the manuscript. S. N. B. and K. L. T. contributed to the interpretation of the data and the critical revision of the manuscript. K. L. T. obtained the funding, conceived the study and supervised the data collection. All authors read and approved the final manuscript. None of the authors had any conflicts of interest.

Supplementary tables are available online at http://www.journals.cambridge.org/bjn

References

1Dauchet, L, Amouyel, P, Hercberg, S, et al. (2006) Fruit and vegetable consumption and risk of coronary heart disease: a meta-analysis of cohort studies. J Nutr 136, 25882593.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2He, FJ, Nowson, CA, Lucas, M, et al. (2007) Increased consumption of fruit and vegetables is related to a reduced risk of coronary heart disease: meta-analysis of cohort studies. J Hum Hypertens 21, 717728.Google Scholar
3Dauchet, L, Amouyel, P & Dallongeville, J (2005) Fruit and vegetable consumption and risk of stroke: a meta-analysis of cohort studies. Neurology 65, 11931197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
4He, FJ, Nowson, CA & MacGregor, GA (2006) Fruit and vegetable consumption and stroke: meta-analysis of cohort studies. Lancet 367, 320326.Google Scholar
5Koushik, A, Hunter, DJ, Spiegelman, D, et al. (2007) Fruits, vegetables, and colon cancer risk in a pooled analysis of 14 cohort studies. J Natl Cancer Inst 99, 14711483.Google Scholar
6Lunet, N, Valbuena, C, Vieira, AL, et al. (2007) Fruit and vegetable consumption and gastric cancer by location and histological type: case–control and meta-analysis. Eur J Cancer Prev 16, 312327.Google Scholar
7Pavia, M, Pileggi, C, Nobile, CG, et al. (2006) Association between fruit and vegetable consumption and oral cancer: a meta-analysis of observational studies. Am J Clin Nutr 83, 11261134.Google Scholar
8Kang, JH, Ascherio, A & Grodstein, F (2005) Fruit and vegetable consumption and cognitive decline in aging women. Ann Neurol 57, 713720.Google Scholar
9Morris, MC, Evans, DA, Tangney, CC, et al. (2006) Associations of vegetable and fruit consumption with age-related cognitive change. Neurology 67, 13701376.Google Scholar
10Sabia, S, Nabi, H, Kivimaki, M, et al. (2009) Health behaviors from early to late midlife as predictors of cognitive function: the Whitehall II study. Am J Epidemiol 170, 428437.Google Scholar
11Hughes, TF, Andel, R, Small, BJ, et al. (2010) Midlife fruit and vegetable consumption and risk of dementia in later life in Swedish twins. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 18, 413420.Google Scholar
12Engelhart, MJ, Geerlings, MI, Ruitenberg, A, et al. (2002) Dietary intake of antioxidants and risk of Alzheimer disease. JAMA 287, 32233229.Google Scholar
13Morris, MC, Evans, DA, Bienias, JL, et al. (2002) Dietary intake of antioxidant nutrients and the risk of incident Alzheimer disease in a biracial community study. JAMA 287, 32303237.Google Scholar
14Laurin, D, Masaki, KH, Foley, DJ, et al. (2004) Midlife dietary intake of antioxidants and risk of late-life incident dementia: the Honolulu-Asia Aging Study. Am J Epidemiol 159, 959967.Google Scholar
15Luchsinger, JA, Tang, MX, Shea, S, et al. (2003) Antioxidant vitamin intake and risk of Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol 60, 203208.Google Scholar
16Yaffe, K, Clemons, TE, McBee, WL, et al. (2004) Impact of antioxidants, zinc, and copper on cognition in the elderly: a randomized, controlled trial. Neurology 63, 17051707.Google Scholar
17Petersen, RC, Thomas, RG, Grundman, M, et al. (2005) Vitamin E and donepezil for the treatment of mild cognitive impairment. N Engl J Med 352, 23792388.Google Scholar
18Kang, JH, Cook, NR, Manson, JE, et al. (2009) Vitamin E, vitamin C, beta carotene, and cognitive function among women with or at risk of cardiovascular disease: the Women's Antioxidant and Cardiovascular Study. Circulation 119, 27722780.Google Scholar
19Grodstein, F, Kang, JH, Glynn, RJ, et al. (2007) A randomized trial of beta carotene supplementation and cognitive function in men: the Physicians’ Health Study II. Arch Intern Med 167, 21842190.Google Scholar
20Liu, RH (2003) Health benefits of fruit and vegetables are from additive and synergistic combinations of phytochemicals. Am J Clin Nutr 78, 517S520S.Google Scholar
21Tucker, K, Mattei, J, Noel, S, et al. (2010) The Boston Puerto Rican Health Study, a longitudinal cohort study on health disparities in Puerto Rican adults: challenges and opportunities. BMC Public Health 10, 107.Google Scholar
22Mungas, D (1991) In-office mental status testing: a practical guide. Geriatrics 46, 5458, 66.Google Scholar
23Cook, AJ & Friday, JE (2004) Pyramid servings database for USDA survey food codes. Version 2.0. Beltsville, MD: US Department of Agriculture, Agricultural Research Service, Community Nutrition Research Group.http://www.ars.usda.gov/Services/docs.htm?docid = 8634.Google Scholar
24Bhupathiraju, SN & Tucker, KL (2011) Greater variety in fruit and vegetable intake is associated with lower inflammation in Puerto Rican adults. Am J Clin Nutr 93, 3746.Google Scholar
25Folstein, MF, Folstein, SE & McHugh, PR (1975) “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 12, 189198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
26Artiola Fortuny, L, Hermosillo Romo, D, Heaton, RK, et al. (2000) Manual de Normas y Procedimientos para la Batería Neuropsicológica en Español (Standards and Procedures Manual for Neuropsychological Battery in Spanish). Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.Google Scholar
27Wolf-Klein, GP, Silverstone, FA, Levy, AP, et al. (1989) Screening for Alzheimer's disease by clock drawing. J Am Geriatr Soc 37, 730734.Google Scholar
28Beery, K (1989) The Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration Manual, revised ed.Cleveland: Modern Curriculum Press.Google Scholar
29Department of Health and Human Services (2007) Annual update of the HHS poverty guidelines.Federal Register 72, 31473148.Google Scholar
30Tucker, KL, Falcon, LM, Bianchi, LA, et al. (2000) Self-reported prevalence and health correlates of functional limitation among Massachusetts elderly Puerto Ricans, Dominicans, and non-hispanic white neighborhood comparison group. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 55, M90M97.Google Scholar
31American Diabetes Association (2008) Diagnosis and classification of diabetes mellitus. Diabetes Care 31, Suppl. 1, S55S60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
32U.S. Department of Agriculture and U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (2010) Dietary Guidelines for Americans, 2010. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.Google Scholar
33Pratico, D, Clark, CM, Liun, F, et al. (2002) Increase of brain oxidative stress in mild cognitive impairment: a possible predictor of Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol 59, 972976.Google Scholar
34Lovell, MA & Markesbery, WR (2007) Oxidative damage in mild cognitive impairment and early Alzheimer's disease. J Neurosci Res 85, 30363040.Google Scholar
35Dai, Q, Borenstein, AR, Wu, Y, et al. (2006) Fruit and vegetable juices and Alzheimer's disease: the Kame Project. Am J Med 119, 751759.Google Scholar
36Wang, S, Meckling, KA, Marcone, MF, et al. (2011) Synergistic, additive, and antagonistic effects of food mixtures on total antioxidant capacities. J Agric Food Chem 59, 960968.Google Scholar
37Mattson, MP & Shea, TB (2003) Folate and homocysteine metabolism in neural plasticity and neurodegenerative disorders. Trends Neurosci 26, 137146.Google Scholar
38Tucker, KL, Qiao, N, Scott, T, et al. (2005) High homocysteine and low B vitamins predict cognitive decline in aging men: the Veterans Affairs Normative Aging Study. Am J Clin Nutr 82, 627635.Google Scholar
39Komulainen, P, Lakka, TA, Kivipelto, M, et al. (2007) Serum high sensitivity C-reactive protein and cognitive function in elderly women. Age Ageing 36, 443448.Google Scholar
40Arsenault, LN, Matthan, N, Scott, TM, et al. (2009) Validity of estimated dietary eicosapentaenoic acid and docosahexaenoic acid intakes determined by interviewer-administered food frequency questionnaire among older adults with mild-to-moderate cognitive impairment or dementia. Am J Epidemiol 170, 95103.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Characteristics of the participants by quintiles of total quantity and variety in fruit and vegetable intake from the Boston Puerto Rican Health Study, 2004–9 (Mean values with their standard errors; number of participants; percentages; medians and ranges)

Figure 1

Table 2 Cognitive performance by quintile of total quantity and variety in fruit and vegetable intake from the Boston Puerto Rican Health Study, 2004–9† (Mean values with their standard errors)

Figure 2

Table 3 Individual cognitive test performance by quintile of total quantity and variety in fruit and vegetable intake from the Boston Puerto Rican Health Study, 2004–9† (Mean values with their standard errors)

Supplementary material: PDF

Ye supplementary material

Appendix

Download Ye supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 451.9 KB