Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dsjbd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T18:29:53.171Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Long-term effects of the terror attack in Berlin in 2016 on paranoid ideation in female emergency personnel

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 August 2020

Ulrich Wesemann*
Affiliation:
Psychotrauma Center, German Armed Forces Hospital Berlin, Germany
Manuel Mahnke
Affiliation:
Psychotrauma Center, German Armed Forces Hospital Berlin, Germany; and Fire and Rescue Station Wedding, Voluntary Fire Brigade, Germany
Sarah Polk
Affiliation:
Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Germany
Gerd Willmund
Affiliation:
Psychotrauma Center, German Armed Forces Hospital Berlin, Germany
*
Correspondence: Ulrich Wesemann. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

In a pilot study, female emergency personnel showed increased paranoid ideation following a terror attack. This newly designed confirmatory study aims to replicate these previously found gender-specific results and investigate the progression of effects after 2 years. Participants were exposed and unexposed emergency personnel (n = 120). Exposed female versus exposed male personnel showed higher paranoid ideation at both time points. There was a group × time interaction effect in paranoid ideation: paranoid ideation increased over time in the exposed versus the unexposed female group. The same effect was observed with exposed female emergency personnel showing a significant 2-year post-deployment increase compared with the total group including unexposed female as well as exposed and unexposed male emergency personnel. There is, as yet, no conclusive explanation for this difference. Sexual harassment in a male-dominated profession may be a vulnerability factor. Differentiated preparation and follow-up for emergency responders is recommended moving towards health-related equality.

Type
Short Report
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s) 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Royal College of Psychiatrists

Background

Responses to a potentially traumatic event span a wide range from increased stress to mental disorders, such as depression, post-traumatic stress disorder, other anxiety disorders or addiction.Reference Zvolensky, Farris, Kotov, Schechter, Bromet and Gonzalez1 Numerous publications focus on the psychological effects of disasters on emergency responders, but the systematic investigation of differences between genders or occupational groups is comparatively limited.Reference Neria, Nandi and Galea2 Indeed, the psychological reactions of emergency personnel exposed to terror attacks in Europe have until now rarely been examined.Reference Skogstad, Heir, Hauff and Ekeberg3Reference Motreff, Baubet, Pirard, Rabet, Petitclerc and Stene5 However, understanding these differences is necessary for the development of specific preparation and follow-up tools. In particular, gender differences are of interest in addressing health inequalities in pre- and post-mission outcomes.

In military contexts, computer-based prevention programs that are target group-specific can be used during preparation for foreign missions; in the USA, the ‘Stress Resilience in Virtual Environments Program’ (STRIVE) was developed for this purpose.Reference Rizzo, Buckwalter, John, Newman, Parsons and Kenny6 STRIVE is specific to target group and deployment, although not adaptive because of its fixed programmed scenarios. In Germany, the ‘Chaos Driven Situation Management Retrieval System’ (CHARLY) was recently launched. CHARLY is a blended learning program that has already been made available for certain military professions, such as paramedics or ordnance technicians, and has demonstrated a specific preventive effect on post-traumatic stress disorder.Reference Wesemann, Kowalski, Jacobsen, Beudt, Jacobs and Fehr7 Similarly, specific programs would be useful for emergency personnel.

Research findings after the 2016 terrorist attack in Berlin

In order to examine whether the efforts to investigate occupation- and gender-specific differentiation is warranted, an explorative pilot study was conducted after the 2016 terrorist attack in Berlin, when a tractor-trailer was steered into a crowd at the Breitscheidplatz Christmas market. This was the first Islamist terrorist attack in Germany in which more people died than the perpetrator himself. In total, 12 people were killed and another 55 injured. Initial cross-sectional analyses found occupational and gender differences; notably, female personnel showed higher paranoid ideation.Reference Wesemann, Bühler, Mahnke, Butler, Polk and Willmund8 Although the results regarding the occupational groups have been replicated (U. Wesemann, personal communication, 2020), those regarding gender-specific differences in paranoid ideation are still provisional and no conclusive explanation for this difference is given. A meta-analysis showed a robust association between paranoid ideation and violence in the general population, and this association remained stable when controlling for drug misuse or other psychiatric comorbidities.Reference Coid, Ullrich, Bebbington, Fazel and Keers9 Regarding gender differences, the theory of tokenism predicts gender stereotypes and harassment in organisations where women are underrepresented.Reference Kanter10 This theory seems to be of particular relevance in organisations where length of employment is shorter for women,Reference Wilson11 which is often the case in emergency services such as fire brigades. Indeed, sexual assaults on women are more common in male-dominated occupations,Reference Kabat-Farr and Cortina12 which could increase vulnerability to paranoid ideation. Therefore, this confirmation study aims to repeat these gender-specific findings, with the hypothesis of more pronounced paranoid ideation among exposed female emergency personnel.

Method

All emergency personnel from the fire brigade, police, non-governmental organisations and civil rescue services deployed to respond to the terror attack at Breitscheidplatz in Berlin were given the opportunity to participate in the study (exposed group). A comparison group comprised personnel from the same units who had not responded to the attack. The recruitment was conducted with the consent of the organisations. Task force leaders were informed about the study and invited to participate. If task force leaders agreed, brief information sessions were held for the respective units, and attendees were informed about the purpose of the study. Participation in the information sessions and the study was voluntary, and written informed consent was given by all participants. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Charité. The first survey was distributed 3–4 months after the attack, and a second measurement was conducted 18 to 21 months after the first survey. Participants received the questionnaires in the mail.

A total of n = 120 volunteers participated at the first time point, 60 in the exposed group and 60 in the comparison group. At the second time point, 39 individuals from the exposed group and 33 from the comparison group responded. All deployed personnel had been on site with the exception of a dispatcher. Gender and occupational group distributions are shown in Table 1.

Table 1 Distribution of emergency service personnel according to gender, occupation and deployment to the terror attack

NGO, non-governmental organisation; PSNV, Psychosocial Emergency and Crisis Network (Psychosoziale Notfallversorgung);

a. Excluded from occupational affiliation because of data protection reasons.

Data collection consisted of questionnaires including demographics and the Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI) to evaluate paranoid ideation. The BSI consists of five items that assess aspects of mistrust up to strong paranoid ideation on a five-point Likert scale.Reference Derogatis and Melisaratos13

Statistical analyses were conducted with SPSS (Version 21). Missing values at the second time point were imputed using the baseline-observation-carried-forward method. To test for drop-out bias, two-sample t-tests were conducted for metric parameters and Pearson χ2 was calculated for categorial parameters. Using linear regression, the exposed group and comparison group were tested separately to test whether gender had an influence on paranoid ideation. Participants who indicated their gender as ‘other’ were excluded, as the small number (n = 1) would not have allowed for meaningful evaluation. In order to check whether the occupational group had an influence on paranoid ideation, a Kruskal–Wallis H-test was carried out within deployed female personnel. Additionally, differences in change in paranoid ideation between the exposed group and comparison group were examined using a single-factor repeated measures ANOVA (rmANOVA) with a Bonferroni post hoc test.

Results

There were no significant differences between the completers and those who dropped out (Table 2). Linear regression analysis revealed a significant influence of gender on paranoid ideation within the exposed group, F(1,57) = 11.6, P = 0.001, R 2 = 0.17, β = 0.41, with female personnel scoring higher. This influence was not seen within the comparison group, F(1,58) = 3.1 (not significant). Similar results were found 2 years after the attack: female personnel in the exposed group continued to report higher paranoid ideation, F(1,57) = 8.9, P = 0.004, R 2 = 0.14, β = 0.37. This influence was absent in the comparison group, F(1,57) = 1.6 (not significant). rmANOVA revealed an interaction effect in paranoid ideation, F(1, 115) = 5.0, P = 0.027, ƞ² = 0.04; compared with all other participants, paranoid ideation increased significantly among exposed female personnel. This interaction effect remained when directly comparing only female personnel of each group, F(1, 37) = 4.2, P = 0.047, ƞ² = 0.103, with a significant increase in paranoid ideation in exposed female personnel. Finally, a Kruskal–Wallis H-test carried out within deployed female personnel showed that there was no statistically significant difference in paranoid ideation score between the different occupation groups, χ²(3) = 1.92, P = 0.589.

Table 2 t-test showing no significant differences between the participants who responded at both time points and those who responded at baseline only

Cisgender: χ²(1, n = 119) <1; not significant.

Discussion

The current study replicated the results of the pilot study. The finding that gender influences paranoid ideation following terror attacks or catastrophes has, to our knowledge, not been replicated in other studies since, but studies relating high-risk occupations to paranoid ideation are so far inconsistent. For example, medical prison staff reported higher paranoid ideation than the general population,Reference Liu, Jiang, Hou, He, Lu and Mao14 but veterans deployed to the Gulf War did not differ from a matched comparison group of soldiers.Reference Ishoy, Knop, Suadicani, Guldager, Appleyard and Gyntelberg15 However, these studies did not control for gender, and although in the sample of medical prison staff, the proportion of female employees (53.6%) may have driven this difference, among the sample of Gulf War veterans, only 3.1% were women. Thus, future research may consider gender-specific evaluations, or controlling for gender when comparing different studies.

As a result of limitations of this study, the results should be interpreted carefully. The sample size for the subgroup analysis was small, and because of a drop-out rate of 40%, some data had to be imputed at the second time point, which notably restricts interpretation of the effects seen after 2 years. Further, because of the small sample size, direct comparison between female personnel in the exposed group and comparison group was only carried out over time. Also, approximately 90% of the female control group are police, limiting representativeness. Nevertheless, occupational group differences for paranoid ideation were ruled out for deployed female emergency responders.

Despite these limitations, the replication of the results of the pilot study provides an indication of gender-specific effects. The current study suggests that there is an elevated need for improvement in health-related gender equality. Our results could be used for the development of a gender-specific module before and after the deployment.

Data availability

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Charlyn Löpke from the Bundeswehr Hospital Berlin for supporting our work.

Author contribution

U.W.: conception of the work, analysis and interpretation of data for the work, drafting the work, approval of the final version and agreement to be accountable for all aspects of the work. M.M.: acquisition of data for the work, revising the work, approval of the final version and agreement to be accountable for all aspects of the work. S.P.: interpretation of data for the work, revising the work, approval of the final version and agreement to be accountable for all aspects of the work. G.W: conception of the work, analysis and interpretation of data for the work, drafting the work, approval of the final version and agreement to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

Declaration of interest

None.

ICMJE forms are in the supplementary material, available online at https://doi.org/10.1192/bjo.2020.57.

References

Zvolensky, MJ, Farris, SG, Kotov, R, Schechter, CB, Bromet, E, Gonzalez, A, et al. World Trade Center disaster and sensitization to subsequent life stress: a longitudinal study of disaster responders. Prev Med 2015; 75: 70–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neria, Y, Nandi, A, Galea, S. Post-traumatic stress disorder following disasters: a systematic review. Psychol Med 2008; 38: 467–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skogstad, L, Heir, T, Hauff, E, Ekeberg, Ø. Post-traumatic stress among rescue workers after terror attacks in Norway. Occup Med 2016; 66: 528535.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vandentorren, S, Pirard, P, Sanna, A, Aubert, L, Motreff, Y, Dantchev, N, et al. Healthcare provision and the psychological, somatic and social impact on people involved in the terror attacks in January 2015 in Paris: cohort study. Br J Psychiatry 2018; 212: 207214.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Motreff, Y, Baubet, T, Pirard, P, Rabet, G, Petitclerc, M, Stene, LE, et al. Factors associated with PTSD and partial PTSD among first responders following the Paris terror attacks in November 2015. J Psychiatr Res 2019; 121: 143–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rizzo, A, Buckwalter, JG, John, B, Newman, B, Parsons, T, Kenny, P, et al. STRIVE: Stress Resilience In Virtual Environments: a pre-deployment VR system for training emotional coping skills and assessing chronic and acute stress responses. Stud Health Technol Inform 2012; 173: 379–85.Google ScholarPubMed
Wesemann, U, Kowalski, JT, Jacobsen, T, Beudt, S, Jacobs, H, Fehr, J, et al. Evaluation of a technology-based adaptive learning and prevention program for stress response - a randomized controlled trial. MilMed 2016; 181: 863871.Google ScholarPubMed
Wesemann, U, Bühler, A, Mahnke, M, Butler, O, Polk, S, Willmund, G. Burdens on emergency responders after a terrorist attack in Berlin. Occup Med (Lond) 2018; 68: 60–3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coid, JW, Ullrich, S, Bebbington, P, Fazel, S, Keers, R. Paranoid ideation and violence: meta-analysis of individual subject data of 7 population surveys. Schizophr Bull 2016; 42: 907915.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kanter, RM. Some effects of proportions on group life: skewed sex ratios and responses to token women. AJS 1977; 82: 965990.Google Scholar
Wilson, LC. The prevalence of military sexual trauma: a meta-analysis. Trauma Violence Abuse 2018; 19: 584597.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kabat-Farr, D, Cortina, LM. Sex-based harassment in employment: new insights into gender and context. Law Hum Behav 2014; 38: 5872.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Derogatis, LR, Melisaratos, N. The Brief Symptom Inventory: an introductory report. Psychol Med 1983; 13: 595605.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, X, Jiang, D, Hou, Z, He, M, Lu, Y, Mao, Z. Mental health of the prison medical workers (PMWs) and influencing factors in Jiangxi, China. Int J Environ Res Public Health 2017; 14: 1459.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ishoy, T, Knop, J, Suadicani, P, Guldager, B, Appleyard, M, Gyntelberg, F. Increased psychological distress among Danish gulf war veterans - without evidence for a neurotoxic background. Danish Med Bul 2004; 51: 108–13.Google ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1 Distribution of emergency service personnel according to gender, occupation and deployment to the terror attack

Figure 1

Table 2 t-test showing no significant differences between the participants who responded at both time points and those who responded at baseline only

Supplementary material: PDF

Wesemann et al. supplementary material

Wesemann et al. supplementary material

Download Wesemann et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 1.2 MB
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.