Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-dh8gc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-16T09:23:35.109Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A life-course approach to healthy ageing: maintaining physical capability

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 January 2014

Diana Kuh*
Affiliation:
MRC University Unit for Lifelong Health and Ageing at UCL, 33 Bedford Place, London, UK
Sathya Karunananthan
Affiliation:
Department of Epidemiology, Biostatistics and Occupational Health, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada
Howard Bergman
Affiliation:
Department of Family Medicine, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada
Rachel Cooper
Affiliation:
MRC University Unit for Lifelong Health and Ageing at UCL, 33 Bedford Place, London, UK
*
*Corresponding author: Professor D. Kuh, fax +44 (0) 20 7580 1501, email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Research on healthy ageing lacks an agreed conceptual framework and has not adequately taken into account the growing evidence that social and biological factors from early life onwards affect later health. We conceptualise healthy ageing within a life-course framework, separating healthy biological ageing (in terms of optimal physical and cognitive functioning, delaying the onset of chronic diseases, and extending length of life for as long as possible) from changes in psychological and social wellbeing. We summarise the findings of a review of healthy ageing indicators, focusing on objective measures of physical capability, such as tests of grip strength, walking speed, chair rises and standing balance, which aim to capture physical functioning at the individual level, assessing the capacity to undertake the physical tasks of daily living. There is robust evidence that higher scores on these measures are associated with lower rates of mortality, and more limited evidence of lower risk of morbidity, and of age-related patterns of change. Drawing on a research collaboration of UK cohort studies, we summarise what is known about the influences on physical capability in terms of lifetime socioeconomic position, body size and lifestyle, and underlying physiology and genetics; the evidence to date supports a broad set of factors already identified as risk factors for chronic diseases. We identify a need for larger longitudinal studies to investigate age-related change and ethnic diversity in these objective measures, the dynamic relationships between them, and how they relate to other component measures of healthy ageing. Robust evidence across cohort studies, using standardised measures within a clear conceptual framework, will benefit policy and practice to promote healthy ageing.

Type
Conference on ‘Nutrition and healthy ageing’
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
The online version of this article is published within an Open Access environment subject to the conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution licence http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2014

Abbreviations:
HALCyon

Healthy Ageing across the Life Course

NIH

National Institutes of Health

MRC

Medical Research Council

Rationale for studying healthy ageing

Research on the factors that influence healthy ageing has become a priority of governments and funding agencies to inform strategies for reducing societal and individual costs of an ageing population. For example, living a long and healthy life is a key research priority theme in the current UK Medical Research Council (MRC) strategic plan( 1 ). Improvements in life expectancy that underlie population ageing should be heralded as a success but the societal costs of population ageing, particularly at a time of economic austerity, are more often the focus of debate. This is partly because there is little evidence, or inconsistent evidence, about whether improving trends in life expectancy are matched by similar trends in health gain, or the compression of morbidity. Mortality and morbidity trends do not necessarily change in parallel( Reference Omran 2 ). Whether population cohorts now reaching older ages are healthier than earlier born cohorts, and if they are healthier, what the main reasons for this are remain major areas of debate, and the answer is likely to be highly dependent on which aspect of health is being compared.

There are a number of conceptual and measurement challenges in the field of healthy ageing. One major challenge is the lack of an agreed conceptual framework, and no standard operational definition. A commonly cited review of ‘successful ageing’ notes twenty-nine different operational definitions across twenty-eight different studies up to 2005( Reference Depp and Jeste 3 ). Since then there has been much more research in this area but little progression on the conceptual framework. The term ‘successful ageing’ is problematic: it encourages unattainable ideals of success and inappropriate ideas of failure; takes little account of the variation in environmental challenges that individuals face; appears to promote the idea that older people should act like younger people for as long as possible; and questions whether functional decline is inevitable, placing prime responsibility to delay decline on the individual. Measures used in studies of healthy ageing commonly use criteria that distinguish the least healthy individuals rather than identifying those in the best of health, and often do not investigate variability across the whole spectrum. For example, physical functioning, one of the most commonly included components in definitions of healthy ageing( Reference Depp and Jeste 3 ), is often operationalised using self-reported measures of physical disability. Finally, we know little about whether the factors across life that promote healthy ageing are the mirror image of risk factors for the development of chronic disease, nor to what extent early life factors may be influential, as has been increasingly recognised for chronic disease risk. Systematic reviews and meta-analyses have become common in the chronic disease literature; a few exist for healthy ageing, limited in part by the lack of standard definitions of the outcome.

The first purpose of the present paper is to provide a life-course conceptual framework for healthy ageing, identifying the key components and inter-relationships that the research agenda needs to address. We then focus on one of these key components, physical capability, the capacity to undertake the physical tasks of daily living. The second purpose is to summarise our findings on physical capability measures from an ongoing MRC-funded review to produce guidelines on markers of healthy ageing. This review had two main objectives: to identify objectively assessed indicators of healthy ageing that are commonly used in population-based studies, easily applied in a range of settings, and exhibit meaningful variation from midlife onward; and to review the evidence for the suitability of these measures as markers of healthy ageing in terms of the patterns of age-related change in later life and prospective associations with important health outcomes. The third purpose of the present paper is to briefly summarise what is known about the lifetime determinants of physical capability, building on our recent reviews of the literature( Reference Kuh, Cooper and Hardy 4 ). This includes the findings on physical capability from the Healthy Ageing across the Life Course (HALCyon) cross-cohort research programme, funded by the UK New Dynamics of Ageing cross council programme 2008–2013( Reference Kuh, Cooper and Richards 5 ). We conclude with a discussion of the most fruitful areas for further research on physical capability in order to be able to compare results across studies and build up a strong evidence base to inform policy and practice to promote healthy ageing.

Conceptualising health, ageing and healthy ageing in a life-course framework

Health is a multi-dimensional concept, capturing how people feel, and how they function from the individual to the cellular level. Evolving classifications define diseases and disease risk using a constellation of signs, symptoms and assessments of impaired function. Health and disease reflect the ability of an organism to respond adaptively to environmental challenges( Reference Dubos 6 ). Thus, a dynamic concept of health across life is needed, not just an assessment of health status at one point in time. Health can be seen as the ‘ability to adapt and self-manage’( Reference Huber, Knottnerus and Green 7 ), based on resilience to cope and maintain and restore one's integrity, equilibrium, and sense of wellbeing in three areas: biologically, in terms of physiological resilience; mentally, in terms of capacity to cope; and socially, in terms of the capacity to fulfil potential and obligations, manage independent living, and social participation.

Development and ageing refer to the changes in health with age. Health capital or ‘reserve’ is built up during development and reaches a peak or plateau at maturity. The progressive, generalised deterioration in function post maturity can be thought of as ‘biological ageing’ or ‘senescence’; the generally accepted disposable soma theory of ageing suggests that this is caused by the accumulation of molecular and cellular damage from environmental insults and chance( Reference Kirkwood and Austad 8 ).

A life-course perspective extends these ideas by investigating the biological and behavioural pathways that link physical and social exposures during gestation, childhood, adolescence and adult life, to changes in health and disease risk later in life( Reference Kuh, Cooper and Hardy 4 , Reference Kuh, Ben-Shlomo and Tilling 9 Reference Ben-Shlomo, Mishra, Kuh, Ahrens and Pigeot 11 ). Life-course functional trajectories (as illustrated in Fig. 1) for body functions (e.g. muscle, lung) or structures (e.g. bone mass), are used as a dynamic way of studying lifetime influences on health and disease risk; these trajectories capture the natural history of biological systems which display rapid growth and development during the prenatal, pre-pubertal, and pubertal periods, reaching a peak or plateau at maturity (‘structural or functional reserve’) and then a decline with age; there is much variation between individuals in the patterns and rate of decline that may be gradual or accelerated; and the age at onset of decline. Exposures in early life, particularly during a critical developmental window, may leave imprints on the structure or function of body systems; and epigenetic mechanisms may contribute to these processes. This developmental plasticity may affect reserve without appreciable effects on the rate of decline, or may interact with biological ageing processes to accelerate functional decline. Exposures after the developmental period can only affect the timing of onset and patterns and rate of decline.

Fig. 1. Life-course functional trajectories. A, normal development and decline; B, sub-optimal development resulting in reduced functional reserve at maturity; C, accelerated age-related decline; D, a combination of trajectories B and C.

Defining healthy ageing

In our recent work( Reference Kuh, Cooper and Hardy 4 ), we separated healthy ageing into healthy biological ageing and wellbeing. One essential component of healthy biological ageing was defined as the maintenance, post maturity, of optimal physical and cognitive functioning for as long as possible, delaying the onset and rate of functional decline. Other essential components are survival to old age, and delaying the onset of clinical disorders (i.e. health conditions where there is a consensus that medical treatment in terms of monitoring, and usually treatment, is required) and chronic diseases that accompany functional decline. In discussions of healthy ageing there is a tendency to forget that over 10% of individuals even from high income countries still do not survive to age 65 years and therefore do not have the chance to ‘age healthily’; rather the focus is on the declining proportion of the population in this group, down from about 40% at the beginning of the 20th century. Evidence from Europe and the USA( Reference Christensen, Doblhammer and Rau 12 ), suggests that overall the prevalence of chronic diseases is rising among older people due to some combination of the following factors that increase duration of time with disease: earlier diagnosis, decreased case fatality and diagnostic creep. Trends in functional limitations and disability are inconsistent across Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development countries( Reference Lafortune and Balestat 13 ), although overall there is some evidence that people are living longer without severe disability( Reference Christensen, Doblhammer and Rau 12 ). Recent comparisons of data on cohorts born in the first half of the 20th century is promising, showing, across cohorts, a decline in dementia in England and Wales( Reference Matthews, Arthur and Barnes 14 ), and an improvement in cognitive functioning in Denmark( Reference Christensen, Thinggaard and Oksuzyan 15 ). As yet there is less evidence of cohort effects in later born cohorts, although cohort differences in obesity trends give cause for concern( Reference Li, Hardy and Kuh 16 ).

Functional ageing may be assessed at the individual, body system or cellular levels. Sustaining performance for most physical tasks (such as rising from a chair or walking at normal speed) requires several body functions to operate. At the individual level we use the terms physical and cognitive capability to refer to the capacity to undertake the physical and mental tasks of daily living, to distinguish them from the physiological functions of the underlying body systems, and to focus equal attention on what individuals can, and cannot, do. At any of these levels, healthy biological ageing is about optimising function for as long as possible.

Wellbeing is distinct from healthy biological ageing, and evidence suggests that the lifetime trajectory of wellbeing is U-shaped, perhaps levelling off at the oldest ages, in contrast to the trajectory of development and ageing (Fig. 1). Wellbeing generally covers positive emotional health, and participation in valued social roles, engaging with others, leading meaningful lives, maintaining autonomy and independence.

Recently, we have proposed an integrated life-course model of ageing that suggests how these measures of healthy ageing inter-relate with each other and may be linked to physical, cognitive and emotional development in early life and to lifetime environmental factors and lifetime lifestyles( Reference Kuh, Ben-Shlomo and Tilling 9 , Reference Ben-Shlomo, Mishra, Kuh, Ahrens and Pigeot 11 , Reference Kuh, Richards, Cooper, Kuh, Cooper and Hardy 17 ). This model elaborates our original life-course models( Reference Kuh, Ben-Shlomo and Lynch 18 , Reference Ben-Shlomo and Kuh 19 ), taking more fully into account the heterogeneity in biological ageing, and incorporating two sources of resilience post maturity. The first is ‘compensatory reserve’, the ability of body systems to compensate physiologically or repair damage with varying degrees of success when faced with acute or chronic low-level challenges. The second source of resilience refers to the adaptations that individuals make to their behaviour or the environment, when faced with these challenges, in order to modify the effect on, or slow the rate of, functional decline.

Physical capability measures as indicators of healthy biological ageing

Objective tests of physical capability with standardised assessment criteria have been developed since the 1980s; these measure capability along a continuum and have been incorporated into a growing number of population-based studies, particularly in the USA( Reference Guralnik, Branch and Cummings 20 , Reference Guralnik and Ferrucci 21 ). They include tests of grip strength, walking speed, chair rising and standing balance. These complement self-reports, improve validity and reproducibility, may more accurately capture change over time, and potentially reduce the influence of cognitive function, culture, language and education that can affect self-reports and make comparisons across studies problematic( Reference Guralnik, Branch and Cummings 20 ). These tests also facilitate the study of variation across the full spectrum of function.

The focus of much of the research using these tests of physical capability, beyond their use as indicators of the current levels of functioning, is to provide evidence of their associations with subsequent morbidity, mortality and other health outcomes. A key aim is to identify thresholds or cut points that could be used in screening for those whose future health is likely to be compromised and who may require earlier intervention than others; however, most studies look at risk rather than prediction( Reference Sourial, Bergman and Karunananthan 22 ). There is also a growing literature on the lifetime determinants of these tests of physical capability. In this research field, robust assessments of the evidence from systematic reviews and meta-analyses are still somewhat rare, but are increasingly being undertaken (see the ‘Lifetime determinants of physical capability’ section).

Selection of physical capability measures

To select objective measures of physical capability for inclusion in the MRC review of markers of healthy ageing, we first identified all potentially relevant studies worldwide that had objectively assessed physical capability in adulthood and documented the measures they had each used. This was based on a review that one of the authors (R. C.) with a colleague undertook in 2008, as part of the HALCyon research collaboration that drew on sources including: a review paper on longitudinal studies of ageing( Reference Seematter-Bagnoud and Santos-Eggimann 23 ); relevant websites; and discussions with experts in the field of gerontology. We then identified other potentially relevant measures from the motor function domain of the National Institutes of Health (NIH) toolbox (http://www.nihtoolbox.org). For the latter, we consulted the NIH toolbox motor function team who, through a process of literature reviews, field surveys, and in-depth interviews with experts in the field, had identified five sub-domains that were considered critical for motor function( Reference Reuben, Magasi and McCreath 24 ). This team had identified the most appropriate measure to objectively assess each of these subdomains across the full range of ages from 3 to 85 years and proposed a standardised method of assessment for each measure for inclusion in the toolbox.

We chose four of the five subdomains identified for use in the motor function domain of the NIH toolbox: locomotor function, strength, balance and dexterity. These accurately described the underlying functions that the most commonly used objective measures of physical capability assess. Locomotor function was assessed by tests of walking speed, timed ‘get up and go’, and chair rising. These measures also involve strength and balance and so assess aspects of multiple subdomains. Strength was assessed by tests of hand grip strength; standing balance by the one leg stand or tandem stands; and dexterity by the pegboard test. In the Supplementary material, we provide information on measurement protocols for these different tests, summarise common variations in protocols, equipment required, exclusion criteria commonly used and underlying body functions captured.

In summary, the Supplementary material provides evidence that all the tests of physical capability chosen are relatively quick, easy and cheap to perform with only grip strength and the pegboard test requiring special instruments; the other tests require some or all of the following: chair, tape measure, tape and stopwatch. There is also evidence that each of the tests is usually found to be valid and reliable. There was considerable variability between studies in the protocols of assessment( Reference Roberts, Denison and Martin 25 Reference Ceria-Ulep, Grove and Chen 30 ); however, attempts at standardisation across studies are now being made through initiatives such as the NIH toolbox.

All the tests have exclusion criteria, and one challenge is how to handle the increasing proportion of people unable to perform these tests at older ages. Many studies simply exclude individuals without test results, but if data are missing because participants were unable to complete the test this is informative and, where possible, should be taken into account. For example, in the MRC National Survey of Health and Development, alternatively known as the 1946 British birth cohort study, grip strength, chair rise time and standing balance time have been measured at 53 years and again, where possible, at 60–64 years in 2500 study participants. It has been found that among those assessed at 53 years, those not assessed 10 years later because they had died in the intervening period, or were unable to do the tests for health reasons at 60–64 years, had significantly lower performance scores at 53 years than those measured at both ages (among whom a decline in mean levels of performance was observed). This is illustrated in Fig. 2 for chair rise speed. The mean level of performance at 53 years of other subsequent non-responders did not differ from the level of those with repeat data.

Fig. 2. Mean chair rise speed (stands/min) at ages 53 and 60–64 years by response status in the Medical Research Council National Survey of Health and Development.

Patterns of age-related change

As part of the MRC review of markers of healthy ageing, we assessed the evidence for age-related declines in mean levels of each of the selected measures of physical capability. As it was anticipated that we would not find systematic reviews examining this we aimed to identify individual studies of interest. Where possible we focused on studies with measures of longitudinal change but where insufficient studies with such data were available we also examined cross-sectional studies. The search terms used are provided in the Appendix; this search was supplemented by expert knowledge and web searches to identify relevant material, including unpublished work.

We found that the strength of the evidence varied widely between measures with the most evidence available for grip strength. Longitudinal data on grip strength across adult life show that mean levels of grip strength are at their peak in the fourth decade of life( Reference Kallman, Plato and Tobin 31 Reference Nahhas, Choh and Lee 34 ), and then start to decline from the fifth or sixth decades( Reference Kallman, Plato and Tobin 31 , Reference Metter, Conwit and Tobin 33 , Reference Nahhas, Choh and Lee 34 ). Timing of the onset of decline in grip strength is similar for men and women( Reference Cooper, Hardy and Aihie Sayer 26 , Reference Kallman, Plato and Tobin 31 , Reference Metter, Conwit and Tobin 33 Reference Forrest, Zmuda and Cauley 40 ), but men start from a higher level and their rate of decline is faster( Reference Metter, Conwit and Tobin 33 Reference Frederiksen, Hjelmborg and Mortensen 35 , Reference Oksuzyan, Maier and McGue 41 Reference Vianna, Oliveira and Araujo 44 ). Data on other physical capability measures is more limited and generally cross-sectional, which restricts inferences about age-related change but suggests a steady rate of age-related decline with men usually performing somewhat better than women at each age( Reference Cooper, Hardy and Aihie Sayer 26 Reference Michikawa, Nishiwaki and Takebayashi 29 , Reference Bohannon 45 Reference Wang, Magasi and Bohannon 62 ).

Associations with health outcomes

The final task of the MRC review of markers of healthy ageing was to assess the strength of the evidence for associations between our chosen physical capability measures and subsequent specified health outcomes, namely survival, disability, CVD, dementia, dependence and wellbeing, by systematic searches of the published literature to identify existing systematic reviews and meta-analyses. We focused on reviews of studies which had aimed to examine the specified associations longitudinally, excluding cross-sectional studies due to the possibility of reverse causality, and using community-dwelling samples, given likely confounding by morbidity in clinical/disease-specific samples. The Appendix provides the search terms used.

To date, two systematic reviews have shown an overall association between slower walking speed and all-cause mortality rates in older community dwelling populations, independent of age( Reference Cooper, Kuh and Hardy 63 , Reference Studenski, Perera and Patel 64 ). The first review also showed similar associations between grip strength, chair rise and standing balance performance and all-cause mortality rates( Reference Cooper, Kuh and Hardy 63 ). Three other systematic reviews provide evidence that lower performance on these tests is also associated with greater risk of subsequent disability in terms of restrictions in activities of daily living( Reference Michikawa, Nishiwaki and Takebayashi 29 , Reference Vermeulen, Neyens and van Rossum 65 , Reference den Ouden, Schuurmans and Arts 66 ). A systematic review of the prospective associations of grip strength, walking speed, chair rise and standing balance performance with CVD, dementia and institutionalisation (as a marker of loss of independence) found some evidence that reduced physical capability was associated with higher risk of these health outcomes, but there were too few studies with comparable data for meta-analyses, thus limiting any definitive conclusions( Reference Cooper, Kuh and Cooper 67 ). At the time of the literature search there were no systematic reviews of the prospective associations between measures of physical capability and wellbeing.

Lifetime determinants of physical capability

The HALCyon research collaboration focused on the lifetime determinants of physical (and cognitive) capability and wellbeing. Here, we summarise the findings for the lifetime determinants of physical capability, drawing on a series of HALCyon publications, and the forthcoming HALCyon book on a life-course approach to healthy ageing( Reference Kuh, Cooper and Hardy 4 ), in particular chapters 2, 10, 14 and 16. Investigating the lifetime determinants of physical capability is a continuing aim of what is now the HALCyon network, which brings together studies for cross-cohort comparisons, systematic reviews (according to the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses guidelines) and meta-analyses where appropriate, alongside in-depth studies from single cohorts. The focus of the original HALCyon research programme was on the relationships between physical capability and the following factors: lifetime socioeconomic position, body size and nutrition, and indicators of the underlying hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (axis), telomere length and genetic factors. To do this, measures of physical capability (grip strength, walking speed, chair rising and standing balance) from different HALCyon studies were harmonised( Reference Cooper, Hardy and Aihie Sayer 26 ).

One HALCyon systematic review and meta-analysis showed modest associations between lower childhood socioeconomic position and slower walking and chair rise speeds that remained after adjustment for adult risk factors( Reference Birnie, Cooper and Martin 68 ); the mean difference found in walking speed translates into an 11% difference in mortality rates between those who were most and least deprived in childhood. Two in-depth studies in the National Survey of Health and Development, complemented this analysis. One showed that lifetime area level characteristics, as well as individual level characteristics, influence physical capability( Reference Murray, Ben-Shlomo and Tilling 69 ). The second suggested that associations of childhood socioeconomic position with physical capability operate through neurodevelopmental and physical growth pathways( Reference Strand, Cooper and Hardy 70 ).

Another HALCyon systematic review and meta-analysis showed robust evidence that birth weight is positively related to subsequent grip strength; in children, young adults and in those at older ages( Reference Dodds, Denison and Ntani 71 ), suggesting that factors in utero or early postnatal life may leave long-term biological imprints on later life physical capability, in particular muscle. Studies from single cohorts show that growth in the pre-pubertal and pubertal periods is also important( Reference Kuh, Hardy and Butterworth 72 Reference Aihie Sayer, Syddall and Gilbody 74 ).

There was cross-sectional evidence from eight of the HALCyon cohorts that greater adiposity was associated with worse physical performance( Reference Hardy, Cooper and Aihie Sayer 75 ); the detrimental impact was greatest in the highest two-fifths of BMI and generally stronger in women than men. Weak grip strength was associated with poorer performance on tests of walking speed, chair rising and standing balance: again, associations were generally stronger in women than men; and particularly poor performance was seen in those in the lowest fifth of grip strength. BMI and grip strength were independently associated with these performance tests and had additive rather than interactive effects.

Across the HALCyon cohorts and other relevant studies, we showed no consistent evidence of associations between physical capability and common polymorphisms of: (1) TERT, a telomere maintenance gene( Reference Alfred, Ben-Shlomo and Cooper 76 ); (2) ACTN3, a genotype related to athletic status( Reference Alfred, Ben-Shlomo and Cooper 77 ); (3) genetic variants on the growth hormone and insulin-like growth factor-1 axis( Reference Alfred, Ben-Shlomo and Cooper 78 ). A HALCyon study has also shown that associations between change in telomere length and physical capability were weak and inconsistent( Reference Gardner, Martin-Ruiz and Cooper 79 ).

Somewhat more positive were the results of the relationships between markers of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis and physical capability. Across four HALCyon cohorts and two other studies, cross-sectional data showed a larger diurnal drop in cortisol was associated with faster walking and chair rise speeds( Reference Gardner, Lightman and Aihie Sayer 80 ); however, there was little evidence of associations with grip strength or standing balance. In a single-cohort study (Caerphilly prospective study), higher cortisol levels measured 20 years earlier, were associated with walking speed, in ways that suggested that the ability to mount a good stress-induced response may be a marker of a more reactive and healthier hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis( Reference Gardner, Lightman and Gallacher 81 , Reference Ben-Shlomo, Gardner, Lightman, Kuh, Cooper and Hardy 82 ).

HALCyon investigators also reviewed the literature on diet and physical capability and found that evidence from cross-cohort studies has been inconsistent and limited( Reference Mishra, Richards, Mihrshahi, Kuh, Cooper and Hardy 83 ), and not supported by trial evidence. There are major challenges of providing harmonised food or nutrient measures for cross-cohort studies. In addition, a recent literature review describing studies of the associations between nutritional factors and muscle strength( Reference Robinson, Cooper and Aihie Sayer 84 ) highlighted that most observational studies to date have been cross-sectional, and existing longitudinal studies tend to have small sample sizes and relatively short follow-up. However, these studies do provide some evidence to suggest that diet quality and the intake of specific food groups and macronutrients are related to some measures of physical capability in later life( Reference Robinson, Jameson and Batelaan 85 Reference Mulla, Cooper and Mishra 88 ), although findings are not consistent. Furthermore, there is some evidence that these associations may operate across life; for example, evidence from the Boyd Orr study suggests that greater childhood milk intake was associated with better physical performance in old age, and there was some evidence that childhood intake of calcium, fat and protein were also important( Reference Birnie, Ben-Shlomo and Gunnell 89 ).

There is more robust evidence from observational studies and randomised controlled trials of the importance of physical activity( 90 ).

Suggestions for future research

While the extent of publications referenced attest to the considerable amounts of research undertaken on each of the physical capability measures, the variability between studies in the specific protocols employed often makes it difficult to compare and combine findings from different studies. For example, estimating the expected rate of decline in physical capability by age using data drawn from several sources is problematic. Very few studies have formally compared the different measures of physical capability in relation to a set of outcomes; so it is currently difficult to establish what the added value of assessing each additional measure is, and to recommend any order of priority. In a number of studies, a set of different objective tests of physical capability are often administered together( Reference Freiberger, de Vreede and Schoene 91 ), such as the short physical performance battery( Reference Guralnik, Simonsick and Ferrucci 92 ), and a total performance score is derived. Further work on the added value of each measure should establish whether the strategy of deriving an overall score of physical capability is of more prognostic value than considering each measure separately. The most appropriate approach is likely to depend on the specific research question being addressed.

We identified a number of studies that had examined age-related changes in population levels of objective measures of physical capability. However, there is a need for larger studies with longitudinal data in which these age-related patterns can be investigated further and variations in within-individual changes over time can also be characterised. This is likely to be especially important given that declines in mean levels of physical capability at the population level disguises the fact that not all people in a population are necessarily declining. Those people who are maintaining their physical capability despite their increasing age are likely to be an important group when studying healthy ageing.

Although several systematic reviews have examined associations of objective measures of physical capability with subsequent health outcomes, some measures have been studied more often than others. For example, grip strength and walking speed have been studied much more often than performance in the pegboard test, which has not been included in any of the reviews identified.

No reviews were identified that had examined the associations of physical capability with wellbeing, another component of healthy ageing. In light of this, we collected a common measure of wellbeing across five HALCyon cohorts, the Warwick Edinburgh Mental Wellbeing Scale. In a recent meta-analysis of the associations between physical capability and subsequent wellbeing in these cohorts( Reference Cooper, Stafford and Hardy 93 ), we found that higher levels of physical capability were modestly associated with higher levels of subsequent mental wellbeing. Adjustment for age, gender, socioeconomic position, living alone, health status and neuroticism partially attenuated these associations. We have also found reasonable evidence that wellbeing may be protective for physical and cognitive capability, as well as being affected by functional decline( Reference Gale, Deary, Stafford, Kuh, Cooper and Hardy 94 ). However, this is still an area where further research is needed, as discussed elsewhere( Reference Gale, Deary, Stafford, Kuh, Cooper and Hardy 94 ).

Within HALCyon we investigated the associations of factors across life with physical capability using a number of well-established longitudinal studies in the UK, which have collected data at different life stages. As there is little or no ethnic diversity within the majority of HALCyon study populations, we were unable to explore the role of ethnicity, despite recognising its potential importance. From a national and international policy perspective it is going to be important in the future to explore differences by ethnicity and perform cross-national comparisons to identify which life-course associations are universal and which are contextual. This is especially important given emerging evidence from the UK, building on a larger body of work undertaken in the USA, that ethnic differences in physical capability and their determinants are clearly present in older populations( Reference Williams, Tillin and Whincup 95 ).

Another area for future research is to investigate the dynamic relationships between different measures of physical capability, between physical and cognitive capability, and between physical capability and the onset of clinical disorders or chronic diseases. Because of this gap in the literature, researchers from HALCyon and the Integrative Analysis of Longitudinal Studies on Aging network carried out a systematic review and meta-analyses of prospective associations between physical and cognitive capability( Reference Clouston, Brewster and Kuh 96 ). Although many cross-sectional studies suggest that physical and cognitive capability are strongly correlated, we found far fewer longitudinal studies; indeed we identified only seven that had investigated ‘change’ in fluid cognition with ‘change’ in physical capability or lung function. Overall, findings were not sufficiently strong or consistent to support a common cause mechanism. Operationalisation and measurement challenges limited comparability, again identifying the need for common protocols and approaches.

In the National Survey of Health and Development, we plan to capture the unfolding relationships between physical and cognitive capability, and between capability and all the other components of healthy ageing, and the lifetime risk factors, mediators, and modifiers of these relationships. We recently showed that, on average, study participants had at least two out of fifteen common clinical disorders assessed at age 60–64 years, and less than one in six were disorder free( Reference Pierce, Silverwood and Nitsch 97 ). Using cross-sectional data at 60–64 years, associations between each measure of physical capability and many of these fifteen disorders are observed; Fig. 3 illustrates this for chair rise performance. We have also recently published findings on the consistent associations in the National Survey of Health and Development between childhood socioeconomic circumstances and four measures of physical capability (walking speed, grip strength, chair rise and standing balance performance), two measures of lung function (forced expiratory volume 1 and forced vital capacity), three measures of cognitive capability (verbal memory, processing speed and reaction time) and an overall standardised measure of functional ageing at 60–64 years( Reference Hurst, Stafford and Cooper 98 ). Fig. 4 shows a striking gradient between number of clinical disorders and this standardised composite measure of functional ageing.

Fig. 3. Patterns of association between common clinical disorders and chair rise performance at 60–64 years in the Medical Research Council National Survey of Health and Development. ECG, electrocardiogram; FEV1, forced expiratory volume in 1 second; FVC, forced vital capacity.

Fig. 4. Differences in mean functional ageing score by number of clinical disorders in the Medical Research Council National Survey of Health and Development at age 60–64 years (sex adjusted).

In summary, what is required is longitudinal research that can compare findings on the different components of healthy ageing across studies and over time, and which can identify the key modifiable lifetime determinants to be targeted. The evidence to date on physical capability (and cognitive capability and wellbeing) is insufficient in these respects( Reference Kuh, Cooper, Hardy, Kuh, Cooper and Hardy 99 ), but so far largely supports the broad set of modifiable risk factors already identified for chronic diseases. Given the growing evidence that the majority of older people are ageing with chronic conditions, research on maximising physiological resilience, capacity to cope and maintaining social participation should become priorities. More robust evidence, using standardised measures within a clear conceptual framework, will benefit policy and practice to promote healthy ageing.

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0029665113003923.

Acknowledgements

Literature searches were undertaken following the standard format outlined in the Appendix by: Rachel Cooper, Sathya Karunananthan, and Renata Bryce. The authors are grateful to Susan Magasi of Northwestern University who provided information on the methods employed for the NIH toolbox. Experts who contributed to the development of the motor function domain of the NIH toolbox are: Dallas Anderson (NIH/National Institute on Aging), Richard Bohannon (University of Connecticut), Deborah Bubela (University of Connecticut), Diane Damiano (NIH/NIH Clinical Center), Marjorie Garvey (NIH/National Institute of Mental Health), Jin-Shei Lai (Northwestern University), Susan Magasi (Northwestern University), Heather McCreath (UCLA), David Reuben (UCLA), Rose Marie Rine (Specialty Therapy LLC and Marshall University), W. Zev Rymer (Rehabilitation Institute of Chicago) and Inga Wang (Northwestern University).

Financial support

D. K., R. C. and this work were supported by the UK Medical Research Council (MC_UU_12019/4, MC_UU_12019/1, G09006861). H. B. and S. K. are funded by the Dr Joseph Kaufmann Chair in Geriatric Medicine. S. K. is also supported by a Canadian Institutes of Health Research Doctoral Research Award.

The funders had no role in the design, analysis or writing of this article.

Conflicts of interest

None.

Authorship

D. K. drafted the manuscript, R. C. reviewed the literature on physical capability measures and carried out the analyses, R. C. and S. K. undertook the literature searches, D. K., R. C., S. K. and H. B. all reviewed and revised the manuscript.

Appendix: Search terms

  • A. Search terms used for each physical capability measure:

  • Walking speed and timed get up and go

  • (gait speed OR walking speed OR walk speed OR ‘get up and go’ OR TUG OR ‘timed get up and go’)

  • Chair rising

  • (chair ris* OR chair stand OR sit to stand)

  • Grip strength

  • (grip strength OR handgrip strength OR hand strength)

  • Balance (One leg stand and tandem stands)

  • (standing balance OR flamingo stand OR tandem stand OR postural control OR postural sway OR postural balance)

  • Pegboard test

  • (pegboard OR peg-board OR ‘peg test’ OR dexterity)

  • B. Search terms used for patterns of age-related change:

  • Physical capability measure (see terms in section A) AND (age difference OR trajectory OR age related change), limited to papers published since 1/1/1980

  • C. Search teams used for associations with subsequent health outcomes:

    1. 1) Survival

      Physical capability measure (see terms in section A) AND (death OR mortality OR survival)

    2. 2) Disability

      Physical capability measure (see terms in section A) AND disability

    3. 3) Cardiovascular disease

      Physical capability measure (see terms in section A) AND (cardiovascular OR stroke OR CHD OR IHD)

    4. 4) Dementia (restricted to clinically diagnosed outcomes)

      Physical capability measure (see terms in section A) AND (dementia OR Alzheimer's)

    5. 5) Dependence

      Physical capability measure (see terms in section A) AND (dependency OR loss of independence OR dependence OR institutionalisation)

    6. 6) Wellbeing

      Physical capability measure (see terms in section A) AND (wellbeing OR quality of life).

References

1. Medical Research Council (2009) MRC Strategic Plan 2009–2014. Research changes lives. http://www.mrc.ac.uk/Utilities/Documentrecord/index.htm?d=MRC006090) Google Scholar
2. Omran, AR (1998) The epidemiologic transition revisited thirty years later. World Health Stat Q 53, 99119.Google Scholar
3. Depp, CA & Jeste, DV (2006) Definitions and predictors of successful aging: a comprehensive review of larger quantitative studies. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 14, 620.Google Scholar
4. Kuh, D, Cooper, R, Hardy, R, et al. (editors) (2014) A Life Course Approach to Healthy Ageing. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google ScholarPubMed
5. Kuh, D, Cooper, R, Richards, M et al. (2012) A life course approach to healthy ageing: the HALCyon programme. Public Health 126, 193195.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6. Dubos, R (1965) Man Adapting. New Haven and London: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
7. Huber, M, Knottnerus, JA, Green, L et al. (2011) How should we define health? BMJ 343, d4163.Google Scholar
8. Kirkwood, TB & Austad, SN (2000) Why do we age? Nature 408, 233238.Google Scholar
9. Kuh, D, Ben-Shlomo, Y, Tilling, K et al. (2013) Life course epidemiology and analysis. In The Oxford Textbook of Public Health, 6th ed. (In the Press).Google Scholar
10. Kuh, D & Ben-Shlomo, Y(editors) (2004) A Life Course Approach to Chronic Disease Epidemiology. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
11. Ben-Shlomo, Y, Mishra, G & Kuh, D (2013) Life course epidemiology. In Handbook of Epidemiology, 2nd ed. pp. 1521–1550 [Ahrens, W and Pigeot, I, editors]. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
12. Christensen, K, Doblhammer, G, Rau, R et al. (2009) Ageing populations: the challenges ahead. Lancet 374, 11961208.Google Scholar
13. Lafortune, G, Balestat, G & the disability study expert group members (2007) Trends in severe disability among elderly people: assessing the evidence in 12 OECD countries and the future implications. OECD Health Working Papers 26 ed. http://www.oecd-ilibrary.org/social-issues-migration-health/trends-in-severe-disability-among-elderly-people_217072070078 Google Scholar
14. Matthews, FE, Arthur, A, Barnes, LE et al. (2013) A two-decade comparison of prevalence of dementia in individuals aged 65 years and older from three geographical areas of England: results of the Cognitive Function and Ageing Study I and II. Lancet 382, 14051412.Google Scholar
15. Christensen, K, Thinggaard, M, Oksuzyan, A et al. (2013) Physical and cognitive functioning of people older than 90 years: a comparison of two Danish cohorts born 10 years apart. Lancet 382, 15071513.Google Scholar
16. Li, L, Hardy, R, Kuh, D et al. (2008) Child-to-adult body mass index and height trajectories: a comparison of 2 British birth cohorts. Am J Epidemiol 168, 10081015.Google Scholar
17. Kuh, D, Richards, M, Cooper, R et al. (2014) Life course epidemiology, ageing research and maturing cohort studies: a dynamic combination for understanding healthy ageing. In A Life Course Approach to Healthy Ageing pp. 3–15 [Kuh, D, Cooper, R, Hardy, R, et al. , editors]. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
18. Kuh, D, Ben-Shlomo, Y, Lynch, J et al. (2003) Life course epidemiology. J Epidemiol Commun Health 57, 778783.Google Scholar
19. Ben-Shlomo, Y & Kuh, D (2002) A life course approach to chronic disease epidemiology: conceptual models, empirical challenges and interdisciplinary perspectives. Int J Epidemiol 31, 285293.Google Scholar
20. Guralnik, JM, Branch, LG, Cummings, SR et al. (1989) Physical performance measures in aging research. J Gerontol Med Sci 44, M141M146.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
21. Guralnik, JM & Ferrucci, L (2003) Assessing the building blocks of function utilizing measures of functional limitation. Am J Prev Med 25, 112121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
22. Sourial, N, Bergman, H, Karunananthan, S et al. (2013) Implementing frailty into clinical practice: a cautionary tale. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 68, 15051511.Google Scholar
23. Seematter-Bagnoud, L & Santos-Eggimann, B (2006) Population-based cohorts of the 50 s and over: a summary of worldwide previous and ongoing studies for research on health in ageing. Eur J Ageing 3, 4159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
24. Reuben, DB, Magasi, S, McCreath, HE et al. (2013) Motor assessment using the NIH Toolbox. Neurology 80,11 Suppl. 3, S65S75.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
25. Roberts, HC, Denison, HJ, Martin, HJ et al. (2011) A review of the measurement of grip strength in clinical and epidemiological studies: towards a standardised approach. Age Ageing 40, 423429.Google Scholar
26. Cooper, R, Hardy, R, Aihie Sayer, A et al. (2011) Age and gender differences in physical capability levels from mid-life onwards: the harmonisation and meta-analysis of data from eight UK cohort studies. PLoS ONE 6, e27899.Google Scholar
27. Bohannon, RW (2006) Reference values for the timed up and go test: a descriptive meta-analysis. J Geriatr Phys Ther 29, 6468.Google Scholar
28. Bohannon, RW (2006) Single limb stance times – a descriptive meta-analysis of data from individuals at least 60 years of age. Top Geriatr Rehabil 22, 7077.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
29. Michikawa, T, Nishiwaki, Y, Takebayashi, T et al. (2009) One-leg standing test for elderly populations. J Orthop Sci 14, 675685.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
30. Ceria-Ulep, CD, Grove, J, Chen, R et al. (2010) Physical aspects of healthy aging: assessments of three measures of balance for studies in middle-aged and older adults. Curr Gerontol Geriatr Res 2010, 849761.Google Scholar
31. Kallman, DA, Plato, CC & Tobin, JD (1990) The role of muscle loss in the age-related decline of grip strength – cross-sectional and longitudinal perspectives. J Gerontol 45, M82M88.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
32. Lindle, RS, Metter, EJ, Lynch, NA et al. (1997) Age and gender comparisons of muscle strength in 654 women and men aged 20–93 yr. J Appl Physiol 83, 15811587.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
33. Metter, EJ, Conwit, R, Tobin, J et al. (1997) Age-associated loss of power and strength in the upper extremities in women and men. J Gerontol Biol Sci Med Sci 52, B267B276.Google Scholar
34. Nahhas, RW, Choh, AC, Lee, M et al. (2010) Bayesian longitudinal plateau model of adult grip strength. Am J Hum Biol 22, 648656.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
35. Frederiksen, H, Hjelmborg, J, Mortensen, J et al. (2006) Age trajectories of grip strength: cross-sectional and longitudinal data among 8,342 Danes aged 46 to 102. Ann Epidemiol 16, 554562.Google Scholar
36. Rantanen, T, Masaki, K, Foley, D et al. (1998) Grip strength changes over 27 yr in Japanese-American men. J Appl Physiol 85, 20472053.Google Scholar
37. Rantanen, T, Era, P & Heikkinen, E (1997) Physical activity and the changes in maximal isometric strength in men and women from the age of 75 to 80 years. J Am Geriatr Soc 45, 14391445.Google Scholar
38. Andersen-Ranberg, K, Petersen, I, Frederiksen, H et al. (2009) Cross-national differences in grip strength among 50+year-old Europeans: results from the SHARE study. Eur J Ageing 6, 227236.Google Scholar
39. Bassey, EJ (1998) Longitudinal changes in selected physical capabilities: muscle strength, flexibility and body size. Age Ageing 27, 1216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
40. Forrest, KYZ, Zmuda, JM & Cauley, JA (2007) Patterns and correlates of muscle strength loss in older women. Gerontology 53, 140147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
41. Oksuzyan, A, Maier, H, McGue, M et al. (2010) Sex differences in the level and rate of change of physical function and grip strength in the Danish 1905-Cohort study. J Aging Health 22, 589610.Google Scholar
42. Oksuzyan, A, Crimmins, E, Saito, Y et al. (2010) Cross-national comparison of sex differences in health and mortality in Denmark, Japan and the US. Eur J Epidemiol 25, 471480.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
43. Goodpaster, BH, Park, SW, Harris, TB et al. (2006) The loss of skeletal muscle strength, mass, and quality in older adults: the Health, Aging and Body Composition study. J Gerontol Biol Sci Med Sci 61, 10591064.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
44. Vianna, LC, Oliveira, RB & Araujo, CG (2007) Age-related decline in handgrip strength differs according to gender. J Strength Cond Res 21, 13101314.Google Scholar
45. Bohannon, RW (1997) Comfortable and maximum walking speed of adults aged 20–79 years: reference values and determinants. Age Ageing 26, 1519.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
46. Bohannon, RW (2006) Reference values for the five-repetition sit-to-stand test: a descriptive meta-analysis of data from elders. Percept Mot Skills 103, 215222.Google Scholar
47. Steffen, TM, Hacker, TA & Mollinger, L (2002) Age- and gender-related test performance in community-dwelling elderly people: Six-Minute Walk Test, Berg Balance Scale, Timed Up & Go Test, and gait speeds. Phys Ther 82, 128137.Google Scholar
48. Tsubaki, A, Kubo, M, Kobayashi, R et al. (2010) Age-related changes in physical function in community-dwelling people aged 50–79 years. J Phys Ther Sci 22, 2327.Google Scholar
49. Butler, AA, Menant, JC, Tiedemann, AC et al. (2009) Age and gender differences in seven tests of functional mobility. J Neuroeng Rehabil 6, 31.Google Scholar
50. Proctor, DN, Fauth, EB, Hoffman, L et al. (2006) Longitudinal changes in physical functional performance among the oldest old: insight from a study of Swedish twins. Aging Clin Exp Res 18, 517530.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
51. Himann, JE, Cunningham, DA, Rechnitzer, PA et al. (1988) Age-related-changes in speed of walking. Med Sci Sports Exerc 20, 161166.Google Scholar
52. Springer, BA, Marin, R, Cyhan, T et al. (2007) Normative values for the unipedal stance test with eyes open and closed. J Geriatr Phys Ther 30, 815.Google Scholar
53. Balogun, JA, Akindele, KA, Nihinlola, JO et al. (1994) Age-related changes in balance performance. Disabil Rehabil 16, 5862.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
54. Kimura, M, Mizuta, C, Yamada, Y et al. (2012) Constructing an index of physical fitness age for Japanese elderly based on 7-year longitudinal data: sex differences in estimated physical fitness age. Age (Dordr) 34, 203214.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
55. Stevens, KN, Lang, IA, Guralnik, JM et al. (2008) Epidemiology of balance and dizziness in a national population: findings from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing. Age Ageing 37, 300305.Google Scholar
56. Onder, G, Penninx, BW, Lapuerta, P et al. (2002) Change in physical performance over time in older women: the Women's Health and Aging Study. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 57, M289M293.Google Scholar
57. Bohannon, RW, Andrews, AW & Thomas, MW (1996) Walking speed: reference values and correlates for older adults. J Orthop Sports Phys Ther 24, 8690.Google Scholar
58. Wagert, PV, Gustafson, Y & Lundin-Olsson, L (2009) Large variations in walking, standing up from a chair, and balance in women and men over 85 years: an observational study. Aust J Physiother 55, 3945.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
59. Pondal, M & del Ser, T (2008) Normative data and determinants for the timed “up and go” test in a population-based sample of elderly individuals without gait disturbance. J Geriatr Phys Ther 31, 5763.Google Scholar
60. Bohannon, RW, Bubela, DJ, Magasi, SR et al. (2010) Sit-to-stand test: performance and determinants across the age-span. Isokinet Exerc Sci 18, 235240.Google Scholar
61. Ruff, RM & Parker, SB (1993) Gender- and age-specific changes in motor speed and eye-hand coordination in adults: normative values for the Finger Tapping and Grooved Pegboard Tests. Percept Mot Skills 76, 12191230.Google Scholar
62. Wang, YC, Magasi, SR, Bohannon, RW et al. (2011) Assessing dexterity function: a comparison of two alternatives for the NIH Toolbox. J Hand Ther 24, 313320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
63. Cooper, R, Kuh, D, Hardy, R et al. (2010) Objectively measured physical capability levels and mortality: systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ 341, c4467.Google Scholar
64. Studenski, S, Perera, S, Patel, K et al. (2011) Gait speed and survival in older adults. JAMA 305, 5058.Google Scholar
65. Vermeulen, J, Neyens, JC, van Rossum, E et al. (2011) Predicting ADL disability in community-dwelling elderly people using physical frailty indicators: a systematic review. BMC Geriatr 11, 33.Google Scholar
66. den Ouden, ME, Schuurmans, MJ, Arts, IE et al. (2011) Physical performance characteristics related to disability in older persons: a systematic review. Maturitas 69, 208219.Google Scholar
67. Cooper, R, Kuh, D, Cooper, C et al. (2011) Objective measures of physical capability and subsequent health: a systematic review. Age Ageing 40, 1423.Google Scholar
68. Birnie, K, Cooper, R, Martin, RM et al. (2011) Childhood socioeconomic position and objectively measured physical capability levels in adulthood: a systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS ONE 6, e15564.Google Scholar
69. Murray, ET, Ben-Shlomo, Y, Tilling, K et al. (2013) Area deprivation across the life course and physical capability in midlife: findings from the 1946 british birth cohort. Am J Epidemiol 178, 441450.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
70. Strand, BH, Cooper, R, Hardy, R et al. (2011) Lifelong socioeconomic position and physical performance in midlife: results from the British 1946 birth cohort. Eur J Epidemiol 26, 475483.Google Scholar
71. Dodds, R, Denison, HJ, Ntani, G et al. (2012) Birth weight and muscle strength: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Nutr Health Aging 16, 609615.Google Scholar
72. Kuh, D, Hardy, R, Butterworth, S et al. (2006) Developmental origins of midlife physical performance: evidence from a British birth cohort. Am J Epidemiol 164, 110121.Google Scholar
73. Kuh, D, Hardy, R, Butterworth, S et al. (2006) Developmental origins of midlife grip strength: findings from a British cohort study. J Gerontol Med Sci 61A, 702706.Google Scholar
74. Aihie Sayer, A, Syddall, HE, Gilbody, HJ et al. (2004) Does sarcopenia originate in early life? Findings from the Hertfordshire cohort study. J Gerontol Med Sci 59A, 930934.Google Scholar
75. Hardy, R, Cooper, R, Aihie Sayer, A et al. (2013) Body mass index, muscle strength and physical performance in older adults from eight cohort studies: the HALCyon programme. PLoS ONE 8, e56483.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
76. Alfred, T, Ben-Shlomo, Y, Cooper, R et al. (2011) Absence of association of a single-nucleotide polymorphism in the TERT-CLPTM1L locus with age-related phenotypes in a large multicohort study: the HALCyon programme. Aging Cell 10, 520532.Google Scholar
77. Alfred, T, Ben-Shlomo, Y, Cooper, R et al. (2011) ACTN3 genotype, athletic status, and life course physical capability: meta-analysis of the published literature and findings from nine studies. Hum Mutat 32, 10081018.Google Scholar
78. Alfred, T, Ben-Shlomo, Y, Cooper, R et al. (2012) A multi-cohort study of polymorphisms in the GH/IGF axis and physical capability: the HALCyon programme. PLoS ONE 7, e29883.Google Scholar
79. Gardner, MP, Martin-Ruiz, C, Cooper, R et al. (2013) Telomere length and physical performance at older ages: an individual participant meta-analysis. PLoS ONE 8, e69526.Google Scholar
80. Gardner, MP, Lightman, S, Aihie Sayer, A et al. (2013) Dysregulation of the hypothalamic pituitary adrenal (HPA) axis and physical performance at older ages: an individual participant meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38, 4049.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
81. Gardner, MP, Lightman, SL, Gallacher, J et al. (2011) Diurnal cortisol patterns are associated with physical performance in the Caerphilly Prospective Study. Int J Epidemiol 40, 16931702.Google Scholar
82. Ben-Shlomo, Y, Gardner, MP & Lightman, S (2014) A life course approach to neuroendocrine systems: the example of the HPA axis. In A Life Course Approach to Healthy Ageing pp. 133–145 [Kuh, D, Cooper, R, Hardy, R, et al. , editors]. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
83. Mishra, G, Richards, M, Mihrshahi, S et al. (2014) Chapter 16: lifetime lifestyles I: diet, the life course and ageing. In A Life Course Approach to Healthy Ageing [Kuh, D, Cooper, R, Hardy, R, et al. , editors]. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Google Scholar
84. Robinson, S, Cooper, C & Aihie Sayer, A (2012) Nutrition and sarcopenia: a review of the evidence and implications for preventive strategies. J Aging Res 2012, 510801.Google Scholar
85. Robinson, SM, Jameson, KA, Batelaan, SF et al. (2008) Diet and its relationship with grip strength in community-dwelling older men and women: the Hertfordshire cohort study. J Am Geriatr Soc 56, 8490.Google Scholar
86. Martin, H, Aihie Sayer, A, Jameson, K et al. (2011) Does diet influence physical performance in community-dwelling older people? Findings from the Hertfordshire Cohort Study. Age Ageing 40, 181186.Google Scholar
87. Xu, B, Houston, DK, Locher, JL et al. (2012) Higher Healthy Eating Index-2005 scores are associated with better physical performance. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 67, 9399.Google Scholar
88. Mulla, UZ, Cooper, R, Mishra, GD et al. (2013) Adult macronutrient intake and physical capability in the MRC National Survey of Health and Development. Age Ageing 42, 8187.Google Scholar
89. Birnie, K, Ben-Shlomo, Y, Gunnell, D et al. (2012) Childhood milk consumption is associated with better physical performance in old age. Age Ageing 41, 776784.Google Scholar
90. Physical activity guidelines advisory committee (2008) Physical activity guidelines advisory committee report. Washington, DC: US Department of Health and Human Services.Google Scholar
91. Freiberger, E, de Vreede, P, Schoene, D et al. (2012) Performance-based physical function in older community-dwelling persons: a systematic review of instruments. Age Ageing 41, 712721.Google Scholar
92. Guralnik, JM, Simonsick, EM, Ferrucci, L et al. (1994) A short physical performance battery assessing lower extremity function: association with self-reported disability and prediction of mortality and nursing home admission. J Gerontol 49, M85M94.Google Scholar
93. Cooper, R, Stafford, M, Hardy, R et al. Physical capability and subsequent positive mental wellbeing in older people: findings from five HALCyon cohorts. Age (Dordr) [Epublication ahead of print].Google Scholar
94. Gale, CR, Deary, IJ & Stafford, M (2014) A life course approach to psychological and social wellbeing. In A life Course Approach to Healthy Ageing pp. 46–61 [Kuh, D, Cooper, R, Hardy, R, et al. , editors]. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
95. Williams, ED, Tillin, T, Whincup, P et al. (2012) Ethnic differences in disability prevalence and their determinants studied over a 20-year period: a cohort study. PLoS ONE 7, e45602.Google Scholar
96. Clouston, SAP, Brewster, P, Kuh, D et al. (2013) The dynamic relationship between physical function and cognition in longitudinal aging cohorts. Epidemiol Rev 35, 3350.Google Scholar
97. Pierce, MB, Silverwood, RJ, Nitsch, D et al. (2012) Clinical disorders in a post war British cohort reaching retirement: evidence from the first National Birth Cohort Study. PLoS ONE 7, e44857.Google Scholar
98. Hurst, L, Stafford, M, Cooper, R et al. (2013) Lifetime socioeconomic inequalities in physical and cognitive aging. Am J Public Health 103, 16411648.Google Scholar
99. Kuh, D, Cooper, R, Hardy, R et al. (2014) What have we learnt for future research and knowledge exchange? In A Life Course Approach to Healthy Ageing pp. 261–272 [Kuh, D, Cooper, R, Hardy, R, et al. , editors]. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Life-course functional trajectories. A, normal development and decline; B, sub-optimal development resulting in reduced functional reserve at maturity; C, accelerated age-related decline; D, a combination of trajectories B and C.

Figure 1

Fig. 2. Mean chair rise speed (stands/min) at ages 53 and 60–64 years by response status in the Medical Research Council National Survey of Health and Development.

Figure 2

Fig. 3. Patterns of association between common clinical disorders and chair rise performance at 60–64 years in the Medical Research Council National Survey of Health and Development. ECG, electrocardiogram; FEV1, forced expiratory volume in 1 second; FVC, forced vital capacity.

Figure 3

Fig. 4. Differences in mean functional ageing score by number of clinical disorders in the Medical Research Council National Survey of Health and Development at age 60–64 years (sex adjusted).

Supplementary material: File

Kuh Supplementary Material

Supplementary Material

Download Kuh Supplementary Material(File)
File 114 KB