Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-tf8b9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T00:50:55.260Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Protocol for a quasi-experimental study examining the effect of a ball skills intervention on four domains of preschooler development

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 November 2021

Hua Wu
Affiliation:
Faculty of Associated Medical Sciences, Khon Kaen University, Khon Kaen, Thailand Faculty of Physical Education, Hainan Normal University, Haikou, China
Wichai Eungpinichpong*
Affiliation:
Division of Physical Therapy, Faculty of Associated Medical Sciences, BNOJHP Research Center, Khon Kaen University, Khon Kaen, Thailand
Hui Ruan
Affiliation:
Faculty of Physical Education, Hainan Normal University, Haikou, China
Xinding Zhang
Affiliation:
Faculty of Physical Education, Hainan Normal University, Haikou, China
Sansan Wang
Affiliation:
Central Kindergarten of Haikou, Haikou, China
Caijin Ding
Affiliation:
Central Kindergarten of Haikou, Haikou, China
*
Author for correspondence: Wichai Eungpinichpong, Division of Physical Therapy, Khon Kaen University, Khon Kaen, Thailand. E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background:

Preschooler inactivity and insufficient motor development have serious long-term consequences. The Chinese Ministry of Education launched a nationwide football-focused pilot project aimed at kindergartens in 2019 and issued the policy “Notice on the Establishment of National Football Kindergartens” in 2020. However, the impact of fundamental movement skills (FMS) interventions on other aspects of child development is unclear.

Aim:

This study will evaluate the effects of ball skills physical education projects on the development of Chinese preschoolers’ physical, motor, cognitive, and social competencies and examine the influencing factors.

Method:

This is a quasi-experimental study evaluating how well the “Hello Sunshine” curriculum project promotes children’s development over 10 weeks. The trial will be conducted from September 2021 to November 2021 in 12 classes from 3 kindergartens with a total of 249 children aged 4 to 6 years in Haikou, China. Pre- and posttest analyses will include tests on participants’ physical fitness, FMS, cognitive self-regulation, and emotional competence. Participants’ background information will be collected through questionnaires answered by parents and teachers. The intervention will focus on game-based basic ball skills.

Findings:

If this intervention provides evidence that these skills improve children’s multidimensional development, it will support the promotion of similar programs in China. We will also outline the social-ecological factors affecting the intervention’s results, providing further information for improving pedagogical methods related to preschool ball skills.

Type
Research
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

The preschool years form an important stage in the development of children’s physical, motor, socioemotional, and cognitive skills (Chinese Ministry of Education, 2012; Leppänen et al., Reference Leppänen, Nyström, Henriksson, Pomeroy, Ruiz, Ortega, Cadenas-Sánchez and Löf2016; Van Capelle et al., Reference Van Capelle, Broderick, van Doorn, Ward and Parmenter2017; Zeng et al., Reference Zeng, Ayyub, Sun, Wen, Xiang and Gao2017). From Piaget’s theory, we know that motor skill development in early childhood enables children to perceive the world, gain self-knowledge, and interact with others, which further facilitates improvement in other developmental domains (Goodway et al., Reference Goodway, Ozmun and Gallahue2011). Thelen (Reference Thelen1992) also stated that motor skills develop interactively with perceptual, social, and cognitive abilities. However, some evidence indicates that once children enter kindergarten, this interactive relationship seems reduce or become limited to specific domains (Hagmann-von Arx et al., Reference Hagmann-von Arx, Manicolo, Lemola and Grob2016; Kenny et al., Reference Kenny, Hill and Hamilton2016; Libertus and Hauf, Reference Libertus and Hauf2017; Cadoret et al., Reference Cadoret, Bigras, Duval, Lemay, Tremblay and Lemire2018). While, little is known about the relationship between preschoolers’ motor skills and the development of other critical domains (such as physical fitness, and socioemotional and cognitive skills). Stodden et al. (Reference Stodden, Goodway, Langendorfer, Roberton, Rudisill, Garcia and Garcia2008) suggested that physical activity and motor skill competence might promote each other in early childhood. Furthermore, some systematic reviews have shown evidence that higher physical activity is associated with improved physical and psychosocial health indicators in preschool (Timmons et al., Reference Timmons, Leblanc, Carson, Connor Gorber, Dillman, Janssen, Kho, Spence, Stearns and Tremblay2012; Carson et al., Reference Carson, Hunter, Kuzik, Wiebe, Spence, Friedman, Tremblay, Slater and Hinkley2016, Reference Carson, Lee, Hewitt, Jennings, Hunter, Kuzik, Stearns, Unrau, Poitras, Gray, Adamo, Janssen, Spence, Timmons, Sampson and Tremblay2017).

Currently, in the area of health care, intervention projects designed to prompt physical activity and prevent obesity in children share three characteristics: 1) preschoolers are targeted; 2) children are provided with structured interventions carried out by teachers and experts, mostly in kindergartens or other child-centric contexts (Logan et al., Reference Logan, Robinson, Wilson and Lucas2012; Van Capelle et al., Reference Van Capelle, Broderick, van Doorn, Ward and Parmenter2017; Hesketh et al., Reference Hesketh, O’Malley, Paes, Moore, Summerbell, Ong, Lakshman and van Sluijs2017); and 3) they focus on fundamental movement skills (FMS) development, especially objective control skill interventions, like the CHAMP project (Robinson et al., Reference Robinson, Veldman, Palmer and Okely2017; Veldman et al., Reference Veldman, Palmer, Okely and Robinson2017; Palmer et al., Reference Palmer, Chinn and Robinson2019), the Head Start curriculum (Robinson and Goodway, Reference Robinson and Goodway2009), the SKIP project (Mulvey et al., Reference Mulvey, Taunton, Pennell and Brian2018; Taunton et al., Reference Taunton, Mulvey and Brian2018), and other structured skill classes implemented by physical education (PE) teachers (Wadsworth et al., Reference Wadsworth, Johnson, Carroll, Pangelinan, Rudisill and Sassi2020) or non-experts (Palmer et al., Reference Palmer, Miller, Meehan and Robinson2020).

Learning and developing FMS competence, as supported by theoretical motor development models, is critical in preschool-age children. FMS is considered the building block of more complex and advanced skills, which means that mastering these abilities makes it easier for children to learn more specialized skills later on (Seefeldt et al., Reference Seefeldt, Nadeau, Halliwell, Newell and Roberts1980; Clark and Metcalf, Reference Clark and Metcalf2002; Gallahue and Donnelly, Reference Gallahue and Donnelly2007).

FMS terms such as “motor performance,” “motor proficiency,” “motor ability,” and “motor coordination” (Cattuzzo et al., Reference Cattuzzo, Henrique, Ré, Oliveira, Melo and Moura2017) are often used interchangeably and inconsistently within the literature. In our study, we used the FMS definition: “basic learned movement patterns that do not occur naturally and are suggested to be foundational for more complex physical and sporting activities” (Barnett, Stodden, et al., Reference Barnett, Stodden, Cohen, Smith, Lubans, Lenoir, Iivonen, Miller, Laukkanen, Dudley, Lander, Brown and Morgan2016). Thus, FMS includes locomotor (e.g., run, hop, slide), object control (e.g., dribble, throw, kick, also known as manipulative or ball skills) (Okely et al., Reference Okely, Booth and Patterson2001), and stability skills (e.g., beam walk and one-foot balance) (Goodway et al., Reference Goodway, Ozmun and Gallahue2011).

Evidence suggests that FMS has a positive association with multiple aspects of health in children, such as their physical activity levels, cardiorespiratory fitness, muscular strength and endurance, and healthy weight maintenance (Stodden et al., Reference Stodden, Goodway, Langendorfer, Roberton, Rudisill, Garcia and Garcia2008; Lubans et al., Reference Lubans, Morgan, Cliff, Barnett and Okely2010; Robinson et al., Reference Robinson, Stodden, Barnett, Lopes, Logan, Rodrigues and D’Hondt2015; Cattuzzo et al., Reference Cattuzzo, Henrique, Ré, Oliveira, Melo and Moura2017), which are the foundations for the health and well-being of children. Studies have explored the association between FMS and variables of children in kindergartens, namely, social-emotional behaviors (Piek et al., Reference Piek, Bradbury, Elsley and Tate2008; Robinson et al., Reference Robinson, Palmer and Bub2016; Ma et al., Reference Ma, Lin and Song2019) and cognitive and academic performance (Planinsec, Reference Planinsec2002; Niederer et al., Reference Niederer, Gut, Hartmann, Schindler, Barral and Puder2011; van der Fels et al., Reference van der Fels, te Wierike, Hartman, Elferink-Gemser, Smith and Visscher2015; Fang et al., Reference Fang, Wang, Zhang and Qin2017; Cadoret et al., Reference Cadoret, Bigras, Duval, Lemay, Tremblay and Lemire2018; Michel et al., Reference Michel, Molitor and Schneider2019).

In most countries, however, FMS competence is insufficiently developed in preschoolers (Taggart and Keegan, Reference Taggart and Keegan1997; Hardy et al., Reference Hardy, King, Farrell, Macniven and Howlett2010; Robinson et al., Reference Robinson, Veldman, Palmer and Okely2017), with this deficit continuing into primary school (van Beurden et al., Reference van Beurden, Zask, Barnett and Dietrich2002; Hardy et al., Reference Hardy, Reinten-Reynolds, Espinel, Zask and Okely2012; Spessato et al., Reference Spessato, Gabbard, Valentini and Rudisill2013; Mukherjee et al., Reference Mukherjee, Ting Jamie and Fong2017; Wu et al., Reference Wu, Ruan and Zhang2017). Recently, a systematic review (Bolger et al., Reference Bolger, Bolger, O’Neill, Coughlan, O’Brien, Lacey, Burns and Bardid2020) of studies in typically developing children (3–10 years) worldwide demonstrated that FMS levels were “below average” to “average” according to the Test of Gross Motor Development-2 and that ball skills proficiency was less than locomotor skill. It was, therefore, recommended that in order to address these limitations, FMS interventions should be taught, practiced, and reinforced to children (Robinson and Goodway, Reference Robinson and Goodway2009).

To date, multiple intervention projects have been implemented, but their effectiveness in healthy preschoolers remains controversial. There are many issues around this topic. First, some interventions lack a rigorous design and only record the outcomes directly after the intervention, with a lack of medium- and long-term follow-up (Wick et al., Reference Wick, Leeger-Aschmann, Monn, Radtke, Ott, Rebholz, Cruz, Gerber, Schmutz, Puder, Munsch, Kakebeeke, Jenni, Granacher and Kriemler2017). Second, some interventions have limited assessment variables and focus narrowly either on a certain aspect of children’s health or risk development. Third, differential outcomes are often ignored in intragroup cases. Finally, there is a general lack of exploration of the social-ecological factors correlating with FMS competence (Barnett et al., Reference Barnett, Lai, Veldman, Hardy, Cliff, Morgan, Zask, Lubans, Shultz, Ridgers, Rush, Brown and Okely2016). It can be argued that FMS intervention projects should evaluate the interconnection of motor skill development with physical fitness, cognitive, and social abilities (subject to physical and environmental constraints), as these are the main aspects determining preschoolers’ health (Westendorp et al., Reference Westendorp, Houwen, Hartman, Mombarg, Smith and Visscher2014; Leonard, Reference Leonard2016; True et al., Reference True, Pfeiffer, Dowda, Williams, Brown, O’Neill and Pate2017; Zeng et al., Reference Zeng, Johnson, Boles and Bellows2019).

Background

In recent years, the Chinese government has promulgated policies to promote the development of basketball (Hu fengshen, 2017) and football (Ministry of Education of the People’s Republic of China, 2019) in kindergartens in order to develop students’ physique and talents. Although there is a rise in sport-centric characteristics among kindergarteners, and some successful examples, the question of how to use ball activities to promote early childhood development still needs exploration, along with a scientifically comprehensive evaluation of children’s developmental benefits resulting from participation in these games.

Based on the current cultural context and the potential importance of children’s ball skills for the long-term promotion and maintenance of health-related fitness (Stodden et al., Reference Stodden, Gao, Goodway and Langendorfer2014), our research will use ball activities to promote FMS, cognitive performance, social skills development, and other health benefits. The “Hello Sunshine” (HS) project is a PE intervention project for preschoolers that uses various ball activities (football, basketball, badminton, table tennis, baseball, golf, handball, bowling, and snooker) as a strategy to promote multiple aspects of children‘s development. The HS project not only teaches football and basketball but also guides children to learn and experience different ball skills and have more fun with a variety of tasks and environmental constraints. It is likely that not every child will enjoy ball games and this gives us an opportunity to explore how social-ecological factors may influence intervention outcomes. It is hoped that this study will provide a basis for a pedagogical method to improve preschooler’s ball skills.

Theoretical framework

It is well known that theories and models used in the health promotion and educational fields provide a framework for developing, understanding, and carrying out interventions (Glanz et al., Reference Glanz, Rimer and Viswanath2008). Our theoretical framework, including its concepts, models, and theories, is described below (Figure 1).

Figure 1. Theoretical framework of this project

Our theoretical framework utilized Bronfenbrenner’s ecological systems theory (Bronfenbrenner, Reference Bronfenbrenner1979) and Stokols’ social ecology model for health promotion (Stokols, Reference Stokols1992) to guide the targeting of specific factors when designing this intervention project and explaining its outcomes. According to the proposed model, children’s behavior is influenced by the individual, their affiliations to people, organizations, and the community, and at the public policy level, and these are collectively known as social-ecological factors. Cool et al. (Reference Cools, De Martelaer, Samaey and Andries2011) identified that parental education, father’s physical activity, and parental rearing pattern had a relationship with preschooler’s FMS performance. In a meta-analysis, Barnett et al. (Reference Barnett, Lai, Veldman, Hardy, Cliff, Morgan, Zask, Lubans, Shultz, Ridgers, Rush, Brown and Okely2016) found that FMS competence was correlated with individual factors, such as age, healthy, weight, range, sex (male), and family socioeconomic status (higher). Zeng et al. (Reference Zeng, Johnson, Boles and Bellows2019) suggested the different components of a preschooler’s FMS have multidimensional relationships with social-ecological factors at the child, family, and environmental levels, and Niemistö et al. (Reference Niemistö, Finni, Cantell, Korhonen and Sääkslahti2020) considered individual factors (age, physical, and specific temperament) to be the most important predictors of FMS.

Teachers and teaching strategies at the kindergarten level are hypothesized to be important factors when initiating children’s FMS programs. At this point, multiple human–environment interactions take place, which, in addition to children’s characteristics, influence their FMS levels and the effects of interventions.

Our intervention project will be based on the concept of cultivating physically literate individuals. Physical literacy was first proposed by Whitehead (Reference Whitehead2013) and has now become globally accepted as a key element in quality PE, according to the United Nations Educational, Cultural, and Scientific Organization (McLennan and Thompson, Reference McLennan and Thompson2015). The ball skills teaching method is game-based and pursues the interaction of different constraints – task, environment, and performer (Newell Reference Newell1986).

The outcomes of this project will allow us to explore the effects of this intervention on the physical, motor, cognitive, and social development of preschoolers, according to the motor development process and dynamic systems theory (Thelen, Reference Thelen1992), as well as the relationship among physical activity, FMS perceived competence, and health-related fitness as Stodden’s model (Stodden et al., Reference Stodden, Goodway, Langendorfer, Roberton, Rudisill, Garcia and Garcia2008). Additionally, based on existing studies, we used the social-ecological model to explain individual differences in the intervention’s outcomes (Glanz et al., Reference Glanz, Rimer and Viswanath2008; Bellows et al., Reference Bellows, Johnson, Davies, Anderson, Gavin and Boles2013; Taunton et al., Reference Taunton, Mulvey and Brian2018; Zeng et al., Reference Zeng, Johnson, Boles and Bellows2019).

Materials and methods

The study protocol complies with the guidelines of the SPIRIT 2013 statement (Chan et al., Reference Chan, Tetzlaff, Altman, Laupacis, Gøtzsche, Krleža-Jerić, Hróbjartsson, Mann, Dickersin, Berlin, Doré, Parulekar, Summerskill, Groves, Schulz, Sox, Rockhold, Rennie and Moher2013). The trial was registered with the Chinese Clinical Trial Registry (registration number: ChiCTR2000035414) on 10 August 2020 and will be carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Children, aged 4–6 years, from three predetermined kindergartens will participate. The three kindergartens are categorized based on three different PE programs provided for the children: a Hello Sunshine (HS), an ordinary PE (OPE), and a free play (FP) depending on their existing PE curriculum.

Aim

This study aims to conduct a “Hello Sunshine” curriculum project and evaluate its effects on the development of physical, motor, cognitive, and social competence in preschoolers. It also aims to determine the social-ecological factors influencing the intervention’s effect.

Objectives

  • To assess the effects of the HS project on the development of children’s physical (anthropometric, physical fitness), motor (FMS), cognitive (perception, self-regulation), and social (emotional comprehension) development and to contrast intervention effects between different PE course groups.

  • To use a social-ecological perspective to explore the factors affecting different intragroup outcomes in the intervention groups.

  • To identify the main factors impacting the efficiency of the HS intervention at an intragroup level.

Research questions

The following research questions are proposed in this study:

  1. 1. What are the effects of the HS project interventions on the physical fitness and motor development of preschool children, as well as on their cognitive and social competencies?

  2. 2. How do intervention effects differ among the HS group, the OPE group, and the FP group?

  3. 3. To what extent are the intervention gains on the HS groups mediated by personal factors?

  4. 4. What are the main social-ecological variables related to differences in the intervention outcomes?

  5. 5. To what extent are the outcome variables related to each other?

Hypotheses

Based on these five research questions, we hypothesize that children in all groups will exhibit improvements in developmental outcomes across the 10-week period. We predict participants in the HS groups will report significantly higher scores on physical fitness, FMS, and cognitive self-regulation tests at follow-up, compared to baseline. We also expect improvements in these indicators to be significantly greater when compared to the alternative programs (OPE and FP).We also hypothesize that differential intervention effects within the HS groups will be associated with the individual, family, and environmental factors.

Methods and design

Study design

A quasi-experimental design study will be employed.

At the beginning of the September 2021 school semester, preparations and baseline data collection will be carried out over 2 weeks, with an intervention period running for 10 weeks. This will be followed by a posttest period and a 6-week follow-up. At the beginning of the February 2022 semester, the third round of testing will be conducted. Given the uncertainty of the pandemic, if the school temporarily closes again, the project will be postponed until the next semester or the next opportune time.

This study’s procedures are illustrated in Figure 2.

Figure 2. Study procedure. HS: Hello Sunshine PE curriculum; OPE: Ordinary PE curriculum; FP: Free play PE

Participants

The study will be conducted in three types of kindergartens in the center of Haikou city, China. Sport-featured kindergartens, public kindergartens, and inclusive private kindergartens in the same district will be assigned. Two classes in K2–K3 grade (children aged 4–6 years) will be randomly selected from these kindergartens and will be allocated as HS group (from sport-featured kindergarten), OPE group (from public kindergarten), and FP group (from inclusive private kindergarten).

The inclusion criteria are: 1) healthy and typically developing children willing to participate, 2) aged 4–6 years, 3) with the informed consent of their primary caregivers, 4) the primary caregiver must have a smartphone to answer the questionnaire. Children 1) who have difficulty in communication, unexplained health problem, 2) who have a contraindication to exercise such as congenital heart disease, asthma, fever, malaise, diarrhea, and 3) who have additional training from a sports club will be excluded.

Sample size determination

We used WinPepi software to calculate the sample size with reference to Westendop’s research (2014) while ensuring it would be larger than those used in other comparative studies (Robinson et al., Reference Robinson, Veldman, Palmer and Okely2017; Taunton, Mulvey and Brian, Reference Taunton, Mulvey and Brian2018). The following assumptions were used: significance level of 5%; power of 80%; and confidence level of 95%, inflated to compensate for a 5% non-inclusion rate. As a result, at least 249 children (83 in the HS group, 83 in the OPE group, and 83 in the FP group) will participate in this study.

Randomization

The children aged 4–6 years within four classes (two classes selected from K2 and K3 grade, separately) will be randomized from the designated kindergartens using Research Randomizer software. The preschoolers in each class who meet the eligibility criteria will be screened by the kindergarten teachers and staff. To avoid changes in teachers and classmates affecting the results of this course, we will intervene in the form of a nature class. In total, 12 nature classes in grades K2 and K3 from the 3 kindergartens will be assigned to a PE curriculum in their respective schools, that is, 4 classes in the sport-featured kindergarten with “Hello sunshine” PE curriculum, the OPE group will have ordinary PE classes, the PE class in the FP group will be free play. This sequence will then be concealed in an opaque envelope.

Blinding

Trained graduate students and senior undergraduate students will act as researcher. The researcher will not know which children will have had which intervention until the end of the study. However, each of the participants will know which group he/she is belongs to. Thus, only single blind (blind the researcher) will be applied in this study. The data will be renamed and converted into statistics by the second author.

Interventions

In the study, the participants will be divided into three groups. The PE teacher will use various kinds of ball game-based curriculum in the HS group. The OPE group will have the ordinary preschool PE class that uses the kindergarten-based syllabus with a PE teacher (ball skills are not the main teaching contents). The FP group will play freely in outdoor activities with no guidance from teacher. The three groups will have the same daily life at kindergarten, except for the content of the PE class and outdoor time. PE classes will be 25 or 30 min, one or two times per week (depending on the student grade). Outdoor time will be implemented for 30 min per day, 5 days a week for 10 weeks during the intervention period. All three methods (HS, PE, and FP) are believed to improve motor skills and physical activity. The differences in the three methods are described below. This description follows the Template for Intervention Description and Replication checklist and guidelines for better reporting of interventions (Hoffmann et al., Reference Hoffmann, Glasziou, Boutron, Milne, Perera, Moher, Altman, Barbour, Macdonald, Johnston, Lamb, Dixon-Woods, McCulloch, Wyatt, Chan and Michie2014).

HS group

The HS project’s design is based on the physical literacy framework (Whitehead, Reference Whitehead2013). It is a well-structured, age-demarcated, PE curriculum that includes five modules: knowledge, motor skills, physical quality, psychological quality, and health behavior. Each module has different course objectives and content in accordance with age. The HS curriculum consists mainly of ball skills activities including teaching or practicing ball skills in PE class, ball club time, and autonomous regional area in outdoor time. It consists of nine kinds of ball activities, such as football, basketball, badminton, table tennis, baseball, golf, handball, bowling, and snooker. K2 students will have a PE class (one ball topic for 25 min) and a ball clubs’ class (children volunteer to join any club for 60 min) once a week. In addition, they will have one football activity (autonomous regional play) in the afternoon outdoor time, daily. K3 children will have PE class (football or basketball) twice a week and ball club activities once a week, and football topic activity two times and golf topic activity in outdoor activity time once. In the PE class, ball game-related general knowledge, ball skills, and so on will be taught. In ball clubs’ time, children will participate and practice in ball games with the PE teacher. During outdoor activity, children will play in an autonomous regional ball area with supervision by the vice-class teacher for 30min.

An experienced PE teacher will teach basic ball skills in the context of a game or story and motivate the children to complete the task, while also enjoying themselves. The teacher will guide the children through the preset game scenarios, while demonstrating and explaining basic ball skills and their task or activity execution via prompt words that remain consistent with the teaching plan. Timely encouragement and feedback will also be provided to the children. During the outdoor class, high-autonomy interventions, wherein the children have the freedom to self-navigate through a ball skills game station are to be conducted.

Taking K3 for example, size 3 footballs or size 4 basketballs will be used during the nature classes to help the children learn and practice in the PE class. Hoops, traffic cones, disk cones, inclined plates, and mini-goals will be used as teaching aids. Participants will undergo 30-min classes, including jogging and playing games for a warm-up of 5–8 min, followed by 20 min of learning ball skills (PE class) or playing ball activities (outdoor times), and finally, 2–5 min of cooldown (using the ball to stretch and lightly pat themselves along with music). The teachers will monitor the intensity of play by observation of facial expression and color, and respiratory rate as recommended by the physical activity guidelines for Chinese preschool (Working Group on Physical Activity Guideline for Chinese Preschoolers Aged 3–6 Years, 2020) and assist children to engage in energetic play (NHS GGC, 2017; WHO Team, 2019).

OPE group

Children will have PE class with the PE teacher according to the assigned kindergarten textbook (Hainan edition) for monthly theme teaching, not focused on ball skills. K2 and K3 will have one PE class in the afternoon, five times/week and the same outdoor time as the HS group, but structured as FP.

FP group (control group)

The kindergarten will have a space for children to play freely with all kinds of sports equipment (slide, sand, climbing frame, etc.) They will be supervised by vice-class teachers without PE backgrounds.

Fidelity

The fidelity measurements of the HS and OPE interventions are based on the curriculum lesson plan and curriculum summary provided by kindergarten teachers, which record the theme, goal, design, and other contents of the course. Research group experts will attend one class per week in each kindergarten at random to check that reporting from teachers accurately reflects the activities delivered.

Outcome measures

The assessment process will use a variety of methodologies (scales, field tests, questionnaires, and ability-based tasks) and multiple informants (teachers and parents). An overview of the measurement process is shown in Table 1.

Table 1. Domains and factors measured by the tests

Anthropometric measures

The height and weight of the children will be measured in the morning by the research assistants. The children will wear light clothing, without shoes, and their standing height and body weight will then be measured using an electronic scale. Body mass index will be calculated as weight/height squared (kg/m2), and the weight status will be classified by Chinese preschooler overweight and obesity criteria (Li et al., Reference Li, Zong, Ji and Mi2010).

Physical fitness measures

We will choose items from the National Physical Fitness Measurement Standards Manual (State Sport General Administration, 2003). Tennis ball throwing, sitting, and reaching tasks represent aspects of health-related fitness (muscle strength, flexibility) and standing long jump, walking on a balance beam, and the 10-m SRT test represent skill-related physical fitness (explosive strength, balance, and agility).

Movement measures

Gross motor skills

FMS will be assessed using Dr Ulrich’s Test of Gross Motor Development-3 (TGMD-3) (2019), a validated and reliable assessment tool for measuring gross motor skill competence in children aged 3–10 years (Dale A, Ulrich, Reference Ulrich2013; Brian et al., Reference Brian, Taunton, Lieberman, Haibach-Beach, Foley and Santarossa2018). The TGMD-3 is subdivided into two subscales: locomotor (running, galloping, 1-legged hopping, skipping, jumping, and sliding) and ball skills (2-hand striking, 1-hand striking, dribbling, kicking, catching, overhand throw, and underhand throw). Each skill is evaluated based on three to five performance criteria. If a child demonstrates the correct performance criteria, a score of 1 for each criterion is given; otherwise, they receive a score of 0. Children complete one practice and then two formal trials. Coding is only completed for the two formal trials, which are summed to calculate the raw score.

Cognition measures

Cognitive self-regulation

Executive function (EF) is used to describe “higher or meta” cognitive function (Miyake et al., Reference Miyake, Friedman, Emerson, Witzki, Howerter and Wager2000) and is critically important in the overall neuropsychological functioning of the developing child (Isquith et al., Reference Isquith, Crawford, Espy and Gioia2005). The Head–Toes–Knees–Shoulders (HTKS) test (Ponitz et al., Reference Ponitz, McClelland, Matthews and Morrison2009; Meador et al., Reference Meador, Turner, Lipsey and Farran2013) assesses children’s behavioral self-regulation, which reflects their ability to transform executive functioning into explicit behaviors (Becker et al., Reference Becker, Miao, Duncan and McClelland2014), including a measure of inhibitory control, working memory, and attention focusing (Ponitz et al., Reference Ponitz, McClelland, Jewkes, Connor, Farris and Morrison2008). The test has strong interrater reliability and is a good evaluator of the behavioral self-regulation of children aged 4–8 years from different cultural backgrounds (Ponitz et al., Reference Ponitz, McClelland, Jewkes, Connor, Farris and Morrison2008). The test is divided into two parts. The first task is Head to Toes (HTT) and the child is required to react in the opposite way to two oral instructions from the examiner (when the child hears “touch your head” they should respond by touching their toes, and vice versa). The child will perform six practice trials with feedback followed by 10 test trials. If the child passes the HTT section, they will complete an advanced task, which mixes the knee and shoulder commands. In this task, the child should respond to the new commands (“touch your shoulders” and “touch your knees”) by performing the opposite action and touch their knees or touch their shoulders, respectively. This section concludes with 10 test trials that mix touch your head or shoulders, knees or toes oral prompts. HTKS is composed of 26 irregular instructions (16 trials of HTT and 10 trials of HTKS). Children get 2 points for correct responses, 0 points for incorrect responses, and if they immediately correct a wrong action, they score 1 point. Thus, the score range is 0 to 52 points (Meador et al., Reference Meador, Turner, Lipsey and Farran2013). This test was previously used in the study of Chinese preschool children (Ma et al., Reference Ma, Lin and Song2019).

Social competence

Emotional ability is crucial to building interpersonal relationships (Parke, Reference Parke1994). Even during the preschool period, emotional competence contributes to social competency and has long-term implications (Denham et al., Reference Denham, Blair, DeMulder, Levitas, Sawyer, Auerbach-Major and Queenan2003).

Emotional competence measure

The shortened version of the Social Competence and Behavior Evaluation Scale (SCEB-30) (LaFreniere and Dumas, Reference LaFreniere and Dumas1996) will be used for measuring participants’ emotional competency levels and will be utilized in each class with the classroom teacher. The test consists of three subscales: social competence, anger aggression, and anxious-withdrawal. It has high interrater and test–retest reliability, internal consistency, and over 6-month temporal stability (LaFreniere and Dumas, Reference LaFreniere and Dumas1996). The Chinese revised version has proven to have good reliability and validity when applied for Chinese preschoolers (Liu et al., Reference Liu, Song and Liang2012).

Covariate and mediator

Personal factors

Age will be calculated as the children’s month-based ages, as it is going to affect the intervention’s outcome. Participants’ perceived motor competence will be measured with a pictorial scale for perceived movement skill competence (PMSC) that builds upon the TGMD-2 (Barnett et al., Reference Barnett, Ridgers, Zask and Salmon2015). It consists of 13 pictures illustrating cartoon boys (boy version) or girls (girl version) performing TGMD-3 skills. It shows middle to high instrument reliability and face validity in Chinese children (Diao et al., Reference Diao, Dong, Barnett, Estevan, Li and Ji2018). Children will be shown illustrations by the examiner and judge whether the cartoon character is good at doing the skills, then choose between four Likert scale options (1 to 4, from “not too good” to “really good”).

Individual temperaments are expected to be predictive of the differential effects of this motor skills intervention (Taunton et al., Reference Taunton, Mulvey and Brian2018), and they will be assessed using the 36-item Children’s Behavioral Questionnaire-Very Short Form (CBQ-VSF) (Putnam and Rothbart Reference Putnam and Rothbart2006). The test is divided into three broad dimensions: surgency, negative affect, and effortful control and it demonstrates acceptable internal consistency. It is a very short form based on the well-established parent report measure of temperament CBQ questionnaire for children aged 3 to 8 years (Rothbart et al., Reference Rothbart, Ahadi, Hershey and Fisher2001) and it has medium to high reliability and validity confirmed by studies in multiple countries. The CBQ-VSF provides moderate reliability of scale scores for assessing temperamental characteristics in preschoolers (de la Osa et al., Reference de la Osa, Granero, Penelo, Domènech and Ezpeleta2014).

Family factors

Parents of the participants will fill out questionnaires to provide additional information about their sociocultural characteristics. For example, occupational status will be defined according to the following categories ranging from low to high: unemployed, worker, education and teaching, self-employed, management, executive position, and professional (Vandendriessche et al., Reference Vandendriessche, Vandorpe, Vaeyens, Malina, Lefevre, Lenoir and Philippaerts2012) and education level will be classified from low to high level: 1) secondary school or less, 2) trade certificate/diploma, and 3) university qualification (Hacke et al., Reference Hacke, Ketelhut, Wendt, Müller, Schlesner and Ketelhut2019). Family income will not be asked for privacy reasons.

Environment factors

Parental report of physical activity

Children’s physical activities promote the development of their FMS (Fisher et al., Reference Fisher, Reilly, Kelly, Montgomery, Williamson, Paton and Grant2005; Stodden et al., Reference Stodden, Goodway, Langendorfer, Roberton, Rudisill, Garcia and Garcia2008). Parental reports of their preschool-aged children’s physical activity levels are feasible in studying the home (available recreational space inside the home) and environment (community) of population-based samples (Burdette et al., Reference Burdette, Whitaker and Daniels2004).Our survey will explore this factor using self-designed parent reports of physical activity based on Palou’s (Reference Palou, Muntaner-Mas, Cantallops, Borràs, Labayen, Jiménez-Pavón, Dorado García, Moliner-Urdiales, Rodríguez Pérez, Rojo-Tirado, Cadenas-Sanchez, Ortega and Vidal-Conti2019) study. The parents will be asked to answer the following questions:

  1. (1) “Your children’s physical activity time every day after school is …?” The response options are 1 = 30 min or less, 2 = between 30 min and 1 h, 3 = between 1 h and 2 h, 4 = between 2 h and 3 h, 5 = more than 3 h.

  2. (2) “Your children’s screen time after school is …? The options are: 1 = less than 30 min, 2 = between 30 min and 1 h, 3 = between 1 h and 2 h, 4 = between 2 h and 3 h, 5 = more than 3 h. (WHO Team, 2019; Working Group on Physical Activity Guideline for Chinese Preschoolers Aged 3–6 Years, 2020).

  3. (3) “How many times do you perform moderate or vigorous exercise for at least 30 min per week ….?” Optional answers are: 1 = 0 times, 2 = 1 to 2 times, 3 = 3 to 4 times, 4 = 5–6 times, 5 = more than 6 times (Li et al., Reference Li, Wang, Zhang, Zhao, Huang, Zhou, Wu and Zhang2020; WHO 2020).

  4. (4) “How much space in your house for child recreational play?” The size options are: 1= < 20 m2, 2 = 20 to 50 m2, and 3 = > 50m2 (Bardid et al., Reference Bardid, Deconinck, Descamps, Verhoeven, De Pooter, Lenoir and D’Hondt2013).

  5. (5) “How often is your child allowed to play in the community playground or sports facilities ?” Optional answers are 0 = never, 1 = during weekends, 2 = every now, and 3 = nearly daily (Niemistö et al., Reference Niemistö, Finni, Cantell, Korhonen and Sääkslahti2020).

Study procedure

After conducting the preliminary research work, the researchers will contact three kinds of kindergartens in this district and communicate with the directors to enquire whether they are interested in research on the health benefits of physical activity in young children. It was agreed that once the study has been approved by the ethics committee, the researchers will recruit participants from selected kindergartens. The kindergartens will send invitations to parents, along with information about the project and an informed consent form. After the collection of the signed consent forms, eligible participants will be screened by the kindergarten teachers, and the researchers. The baseline measurements will be conducted in the initial 2 weeks and all the participants will experience the same assessment at the end of the 10 weeks intervention. The experimental process will be administered as shown in the flow diagram (Figure 2). The physical fitness test, TGMD-3 test, and psychological test (PMCS and HTKS) tasks will be tested in random order in each kindergarten and based on their schedules and space availabilities both at baseline and after 10 weeks intervention, should finish within 4 days in each kindergarten with trained research students responsible for grouping the test items. All children will be encouraged with snacks or stickers to complete all tests. Video recordings will be used for later scoring and checking. The participants will be given a separate and quiet space to take the psychological test. Parents will receive their digital-based questionnaires at a parents’ meeting. They will fill these out and submit them on the day. The head teacher of each class will fill in the SCEB-30 questionnaire in 1 week.

Results

Data will be imported from Excel 2019 to IBM SPSS version 26.0 for inferential statistical analysis. The normal distribution of the data will be checked before determining the descriptive statistics for all variables (means and standard deviations). The participants’ basic information will be descriptive, including age, gender, and BMI. The primary outcomes will be expressed as continuous. If data are approximately normal distribution, the effect of the PE curriculum intervention on dependent variable (FMS, or TGMD, or HTKS, or SCEB score) will be investigated with repeated ANOVAs within factor: time (pre and post), between factors: group (HS, OPE, and FP) and gender (boy and female) and Bonferroni post hoc tests. Further, the relationship between FMS and other domains will be identified by regression analysis. Finally, the potential influence of individual factors (such as age, gender, and temperament) on the intervention effect will be investigated by comparing the gain in FMS score and subscale. Multiple regression equation is used to further explore the extent to which social-ecological factors contribute to the FMS score level at baseline. Statistical significance will be set at P < 0.05.

Discussion

There is currently a diverse collection of interventions for improving PA and FMS in young children, but a dose–response relationship and consequent intervention effects are uncertain and incomplete. For example, some studies have outlined immediate effects (without follow-up), showing a significant increase in PA (Puder et al., Reference Puder, Marques-Vidal, Schindler, Zahner, Niederer, Bürgi, Ebenegger, Nydegger and Kriemler2011; Gao et al., Reference Gao, Zeng, Pope, Wang and Yu2019) and FMS (Wick et al., Reference Wick, Leeger-Aschmann, Monn, Radtke, Ott, Rebholz, Cruz, Gerber, Schmutz, Puder, Munsch, Kakebeeke, Jenni, Granacher and Kriemler2017) in intervention groups. Other studies indicate no significant changes in either PA (Cliff et al., Reference Cliff, Okely, Morgan, Steele, Jones, Colyvas and Baur2011; Palmer et al., Reference Palmer, Chinn and Robinson2019) or FMS (Bonvin et al., Reference Bonvin, Barral, Kakebeeke, Kriemler, Longchamp, Schindler, Marques-Vidal and Puder2013). Hence, any causal relationship between PA, FMS (Van Capelle et al., Reference Van Capelle, Broderick, van Doorn, Ward and Parmenter2017), and perceived motor skills in preschool children remains unclear (Stodden et al., Reference Stodden, Goodway, Langendorfer, Roberton, Rudisill, Garcia and Garcia2008). Moreover, few studies have focused on the cognitive, social, behavioral, and other developmental changes of preschoolers and the influence of certain covariates (e.g., social-ecological factors) on the maximization of the intervention’s effect.

Although football and basketball classes tend to be popular in Chinese kindergartens, there is a lack of systematic scientific research on their effects on children’s overall health. This study is the first quasi-experimental study to explore three kinds of PE education in terms of their effects on the physical, motor, cognitive, and social development of preschoolers. The purpose of this project is to explore whether these ball game interventions have differential influences on the domains of children’s development, compared to a control condition (traditional PE class). It will further explore the influence of children’s social-ecological backgrounds and the intragroup effects on the intervention’s results.

Conclusions

Physical inactivity and low FMS competence in preschool children is a global challenge. This project has been designed to be implemented within a realistic context with a policy-advocating aim, to explore the effectiveness of improving ball skills on children’s multidimensional development. Our findings will also attempt to explain the problems in this intervention and provide evidence for the development of effective PE for children.

Strengths and limitations

One of the strengths of this project is that the quasi-experimental study comparing three parallel groups of PE in kindergartens using blinded assessors. This study design is implemented in realistic environment where the kindergartens are likely to be more cooperative as they can keep their existing curriculum arrangements. The HS group is mainly focused on ball skills, the other two teaching methods represent the mainstream PE activities employed in most kindergartens; the OPE group covers multiple types of physical activity taught by PE teachers, and the control group’s main characteristic is FP. Our research will involve a comprehensive quantitative analysis of the immediate and follow-up effects of the interventions on four domains of children’s development and will collect information from multiple informants including from children, their parents, and their teachers. Finally, the impact of social-ecological factors on the interventions’ effects will be analyzed.

A limitation of this research will be the large number of items tested in this project, as these will take more time to collect and may stress the children’s patience levels. The short forms of each of the established tests have been selected. Exercise interventions in outdoor environments are also affected by weather conditions, including rain, and/or overheating during high temperatures, and the effects of the COVID-19 pandemic. Such factors may affect both delivery and compliance. Further randomized controlled trial studies conducted in more kindergartens will broaden our understanding of the impact of ball skills learning on children’s development, and a cohort study will be established to explore the long-term effects of ball skill learning on children.

Author contributions

H.W. was responsible for developing the study design, study protocol development, and writing; W.E. provided guidance for developing the study design and for writing the study protocol. H. R. and D. Z. participated in developing the study design and preparation of the manuscript; S.W. and C.D. were responsible for monitoring the implementation of the project and preparation of the manuscript. All authors read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Financial support

This work was supported by National Office for Philosophy and Social Science project (16BTY058).

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the teachers and staff at the kindergartens and their students who participated in this pre-research survey study. The authors thank Dr Glenn Borlace for English language assistance via the Khon Kaen University Publication Clinic.

Conflicts of interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Ethical standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional guidelines on human experimentation (Khon Kaen University Ethics Committee for Human Research and Hainan normal university Ethics Committee) and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

References

Bardid, F, Deconinck, FJA, Descamps, S, Verhoeven, L, De Pooter, G, Lenoir, M and D’Hondt, E (2013) The effectiveness of a fundamental motor Skill Intervention in pre-schoolers with motor problems depends on gender but not environmental context. Research in Developmental Disabilities 34, 45714581.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barnett, LM, Lai, SK, Veldman, SLC, Hardy, LL, Cliff, DP, Morgan, PJ, Zask, A, Lubans, DR, Shultz, SP, Ridgers, ND, Rush, E, Brown, HL and Okely, AD (2016) Correlates of Gross Motor Competence in Children and Adolescents: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Sports Medicine (Auckland, N.Z.) 46, 16631688.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barnett, LM, Ridgers, ND, Zask, A and Salmon, J (2015) Face validity and reliability of a pictorial instrument for assessing fundamental movement skill perceived competence in young children. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport 18, 98102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barnett, LM, Stodden, D, Cohen, KE, Smith, JJ, Lubans, DR, Lenoir, M, Iivonen, S, Miller, AD, Laukkanen, A, Dudley, D, Lander, NJ, Brown, H and Morgan, PJ (2016) Fundamental Movement Skills: an Important Focus. Journal of Teaching in Physical Education 35, 219225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Becker, DR, Miao, A, Duncan, R and McClelland, MM (2014) Behavioral self-regulation and executive function both predict visuomotor skills and early academic achievement. Early Childhood Research Quarterly 29, 411424.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bellows, LL, Johnson, SL, Davies, PL, Anderson, J, Gavin, WJ and Boles, RE (2013) The Colorado LEAP study: rationale and design of a study to assess the short term longitudinal effectiveness of a preschool nutrition and physical activity program. BMC Public Health 13, 1146.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bolger, LE, Bolger, LA, O’Neill, C, Coughlan, E, O’Brien, W, Lacey, S, Burns, C and Bardid, F (2020) Global levels of fundamental motor skills in children: A systematic review. Journal of Sports Sciences 39, 717–753.Google ScholarPubMed
Bonvin, A, Barral, J, Kakebeeke, TH, Kriemler, S, Longchamp, A, Schindler, C, Marques-Vidal, P and Puder, JJ (2013) Effect of a governmentally-led physical activity program on motor skills in young children attending child care centers: A cluster randomized controlled trial. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity 10, 90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brian, A, Taunton, S, Lieberman, LJ, Haibach-Beach, P, Foley, J and Santarossa, S (2018) Psychometric Properties of the Test of Gross Motor Development-3 for Children With Visual Impairments. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly 35, 145158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bronfenbrenner, U (1979) The ecology of human development: Experiments by nature and design. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Burdette, HL, Whitaker, RC and Daniels, SR (2004) Parental Report of Outdoor Playtime as a Measure of Physical Activity in Preschool-aged Children. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine 158, 353357.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cadoret, G, Bigras, N, Duval, S, Lemay, L, Tremblay, T and Lemire, J (2018) The mediating role of cognitive ability on the relationship between motor proficiency and early academic achievement in children. Human Movement Science 57, 149157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carson, V, Hunter, S, Kuzik, N, Wiebe, SA, Spence, JC, Friedman, A, Tremblay, MS, Slater, L and Hinkley, T (2016) Systematic review of physical activity and cognitive development in early childhood. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport 19, 573578.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carson, V, Lee, E-Y, Hewitt, L, Jennings, C, Hunter, S, Kuzik, N, Stearns, J, Unrau, S, Poitras, V, Gray, C, Adamo, K, Janssen, I, Spence, J, Timmons, B, Sampson, M and Tremblay, M (2017) Systematic review of the relationships between physical activity and health indicators in the early years (0-4 years). BMC Public Health 17(Suppl 5), 3363.Google Scholar
Cattuzzo, MT, Henrique, R, , A, Oliveira, I, Melo, BM and Moura, M (2017) Motor competence and health related physical fitness in youth: A systematic review. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport 19, 123129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chan, A.-W, Tetzlaff, JM, Altman, DG, Laupacis, A, Gøtzsche, PC, Krleža-Jerić, K, Hróbjartsson, A, Mann, H, Dickersin, K, Berlin, JA, Doré, CJ, Parulekar, WR, Summerskill, WSM, Groves, T, Schulz, KF, Sox, HC, Rockhold, FW, Rennie, D and Moher, D (2013) SPIRIT 2013 statement: defining standard protocol items for clinical trials. Annals of Internal Medicine 158, 200207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chinese Ministry of Education. (2012) ⟪3—6 Years Age Early Learning and Development Guideline⟫. https://www.unicef.cn/reports/3-6-children-learn-and-development-guide Google Scholar
Clark, J and Metcalf, JS (2002) The Mountain of Motor Development: A Metaphor. Motor Development: Research and Review: Vol. 2. 62–95.Google Scholar
Cliff, DP, Okely, AD, Morgan, PJ, Steele, JR, Jones, RA, Colyvas, K and Baur, LA (2011) Movement skills and physical activity in obese children: randomized controlled trial. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise 43, 90100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cools, W, De Martelaer, K, Samaey, C and Andries, C (2011) Fundamental movement skill performance of preschool children in relation to family context. Journal of Sports Sciences 2, 649660.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de la Osa, N, Granero, R, Penelo, E, Domènech, JM and Ezpeleta, L (2014) The short and very short forms of the children’s behavior questionnaire in a community sample of preschoolers. Assessment 21, 463476.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Denham, SA, Blair, KA, DeMulder, E, Levitas, J, Sawyer, K, Auerbach-Major, S and Queenan, P (2003) Preschool Emotional Competence: pathway to Social Competence?. Child Development 74, 238256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diao, Y, Dong, C, Barnett, LM, Estevan, I, Li, J and Ji, L (2018) Validity and Reliability of a Pictorial Instrument for Assessing Fundamental Movement Skill Perceived Competence in Chinese Children. Journal of Motor Learning and Development 6 (s2), S223S238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fang, Y, Wang, J, Zhang, Y and Qin, J (2017) The Relationship of Motor Coordination, Visual Perception, and Executive Function to the Development of 4–6-Year-Old Chinese Preschoolers’ Visual Motor Integration Skills. BioMed Research International 1, 1–8.Google Scholar
Fisher, A, Reilly, JJ, Kelly, LA, Montgomery, C, Williamson, A, Paton, JY and Grant, S (2005) Fundamental movement skills and habitual physical activity in young children. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise 37, 684688.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallahue, DL and Donnelly, FC (2007) Developmental Physical Education for All Children. Human Kinetics.Google Scholar
Gao, Z, Zeng, N, Pope, ZC, Wang, R and Yu, F (2019) Effects of exergaming on motor skill competence, perceived competence, and physical activity in preschool children. Journal of Sport and Health Science 8, 106113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glanz, K, Rimer, BK and Viswanath, K (2008) Health Behavior and Health Education: Theory, Research, and Practice (4th-Edition ed.). Jossey-Bass Inc.Google Scholar
Goodway, JD, Ozmun, JC and Gallahue, DL (2011) Understanding Motor Development: Infants, Children, Adolescents, Adults. Jones & Bartlett Learning.Google Scholar
Hacke, C, Ketelhut, S, Wendt, U, Müller, G, Schlesner, C and Ketelhut, K (2019) Effectiveness of a physical activity intervention in preschoolers: A cluster-randomized controlled trial. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine & Science in Sports 29, 742752.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hagmann-von Arx, P, Manicolo, O, Lemola, S and Grob, A (2016) Walking in School-Aged Children in a Dual-Task Paradigm Is Related to Age But Not to Cognition, Motor Behavior, Injuries, or Psychosocial Functioning. Frontiers in Psychology 7, 352.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hardy, LL, King, L, Farrell, L, Macniven, R and Howlett, S (2010) Fundamental movement skills among Australian preschool children. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport 13, 503508.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hardy, LL, Reinten-Reynolds, T, Espinel, P, Zask, A and Okely, AD (2012) Prevalence and Correlates of Low Fundamental Movement Skill Competency in Children. Pediatrics 130, e390e398.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hesketh, KR, O’Malley, C, Paes, VM, Moore, H, Summerbell, C, Ong, KK, Lakshman, R and van Sluijs, EMF (2017) Determinants of Change in Physical Activity in Children 0–6 years of Age: A Systematic Review of Quantitative Literature. Sports Medicine (Auckland, N.z.) 47, 13491374.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoffmann, TC, Glasziou, PP, Boutron, I, Milne, R, Perera, R, Moher, D, Altman, DG, Barbour, V, Macdonald, H, Johnston, M, Lamb, SE, Dixon-Woods, M, McCulloch, P, Wyatt, JC, Chan, A.-W and Michie, S (2014) Better reporting of interventions: Template for intervention description and replication (TIDieR) checklist and guide. BMJ 348, g1687.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hu fengshen (2017). Zhejiang children basketball invitational tournament unprecedented. http://www.zj.chinanews.com/jzkzj/2018-06-24/detail-iuiakuft0943198.shtml Google Scholar
Isquith, PK, Crawford, JS, Espy, KA and Gioia, GA (2005) Assessment of Executive Function in Preschool-Aged Children. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews 11, 209215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kenny, L, Hill, E and Hamilton, AF de C (2016) The Relationship between Social and Motor Cognition in Primary School Age-Children. Frontiers in Psychology 7, 228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LaFreniere, PJ and Dumas, JE (1996) Social competence and behavior evaluation in children ages 3 to 6 years: the short form (SCBE-30). Psychological Assessment 8, 369377.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leonard, HC (2016) The Impact of Poor Motor Skills on Perceptual, Social and Cognitive Development: The Case of Developmental Coordination Disorder. Frontiers in Psychology 7, 311.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leppänen, MH, Nyström, CD, Henriksson, P, Pomeroy, J, Ruiz, JR, Ortega, FB, Cadenas-Sánchez, C and Löf, M (2016) Physical activity intensity, sedentary behavior, body composition and physical fitness in 4-year-old children: Results from the ministop trial. International Journal of Obesity (2005) 40, 11261133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, C, Wang, L, Zhang, X, Zhao, Z, Huang, Z, Zhou, M, Wu, J and Zhang, M. (2020) Leisure-time physical activity among chinese adults — china, 2015. China CDC Weekly 2, 671–677.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, H, Zong, X, Ji, C and Mi, J (2010) Body mass index cut-offs for overweight and obesity in Chinese children and adolescents aged 2-18 years, Chinese. Journal of Epidemiology 31, 616620.Google ScholarPubMed
Libertus, K and Hauf, P (2017) Editorial: motor Skills and Their Foundational Role for Perceptual, Social, and Cognitive Development. Frontiers in Psychology 8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00301.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, Y, Song, Y and Liang, Z (2012) Evaluation of social competence and behavior of urban children in China. J Southeast Univ (Med Sci Edi) 31, 268273.Google Scholar
Logan, SW, Robinson, LE, Wilson, AE and Lucas, WA (2012) Getting the fundamentals of movement: A meta-analysis of the effectiveness of motor skill interventions in children. Child: Care, Health and Development 38, 305315.Google ScholarPubMed
Lubans, DR, Morgan, PJ, Cliff, DP, Barnett, LM and Okely, AD (2010) Fundamental Movement Skills in Children and Adolescents. Sports Medicine 40, 10191035.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ma, R, Lin, M and Song, H (2019) Effects of Movement Skill Development on Behavioral Self-regulation Ability of Preschool Children. CHINA SPORT SCIENCE 39, 4047.Google Scholar
McLennan, N and Thompson, J (2015) Quality Physical Education (QPE): Guidelines for policy makers; 2015.Google Scholar
Meador, D, Turner, KA, Lipsey, MW and Farran, DC (2013) Administering measures from the pri learning-related cognitive self- regulation study. Nashville, TN: Vanderbilt University, Peabody Research Institute.Google Scholar
Michel, E, Molitor, S and Schneider, W (2019) Motor coordination and executive functions as early predictors of reading and spelling acquisition. Developmental Neuropsychology 44, 282295.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ministry of Education of the People’s Republic of China. (2019) Notice of carrying out the pilot work of football characterized kindergartens. http://www.moe.gov.cn/srcsite/A17/moe_938/s3273/201904/t20190403_376570.html Google Scholar
Miyake, A, Friedman, NP, Emerson, MJ, Witzki, AH, Howerter, A and Wager, TD (2000) The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology 41, 49100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mukherjee, S, Ting Jamie, LC and Fong, LH (2017) Fundamental Motor Skill Proficiency of 6- to 9-Year-Old Singaporean Children. Perceptual and Motor Skills 124, 584600.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mulvey, KL, Taunton, S, Pennell, A and Brian, A (2018) Head, Toes, Knees, SKIP! Improving Preschool Children’s Executive Function Through a Motor Competence Intervention. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology 40, 233239.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newell, KM (1986) Constraints on the development of coordination. In Wade MG and Whiting HTA, editors, Motor development in children: aspects of coordination and control. Amsterdam: Springer Netherlands, 341–360.Google Scholar
NHS GGC (2017) Active/Energetic Play, NHS GGC. Available at: https://www.nhsggc.org.uk/kids/life-skills/play-leisure/activeenergetic-play/ (Accessed: 18 May 2021).Google Scholar
Niederer, I, Gut, J, Hartmann, T, Schindler, C, Barral, J and Puder, J (2011) Relationship of aerobic fitness and motor skills with memory and attention in preschoolers (Ballabeina): A cross-sectional and longitudinal study. BMC Pediatrics 11, 34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Niemistö, D, Finni, T, Cantell, M, Korhonen, E and Sääkslahti, A (2020) Individual, family, and environmental correlates of motor competence in young children: regression model analysis of data obtained from two motor tests. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 17, 2548.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Okely, AD, Booth, ML and Patterson, JW (2001) Relationship of physical activity to fundamental movement skills among adolescents. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise 33, 18991904.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palmer, KK, Chinn, KM and Robinson, LE (2019) The effect of the CHAMP intervention on fundamental motor skills and outdoor physical activity in preschoolers. Journal of Sport and Health Science 8, 98105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palmer, KK, Miller, AL, Meehan, SK and Robinson, LE (2020) The Motor skills At Playtime intervention improves children’s locomotor skills: A feasibility study. Child: Care, Health and Development. Google ScholarPubMed
Palou, P, Muntaner-Mas, A, Cantallops, J, Borràs, PA, Labayen, I, Jiménez-Pavón, D, Dorado García, C, Moliner-Urdiales, D, Rodríguez Pérez, MA, Rojo-Tirado, MA, Cadenas-Sanchez, C, Ortega, FB and Vidal-Conti, J (2019) A single question of parent-reported physical activity levels estimates objectively measured physical fitness and body composition in preschool children: the PREFIT project. Frontiers in Psychology 10, 1585.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parke, RD (1994) Progress, Paradigms, and Unresolved Problems: A Commentary on Recent Advances in Our Understanding of Children’s Emotions. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 40, 157169. JSTOR.Google Scholar
Piek, JP, Bradbury, GS, Elsley, SC and Tate, L (2008) Motor Coordination and Social–Emotional Behaviour in Preschool-aged Children. International Journal of Disability, Development and Education 55, 143151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Planinsec, J (2002) Relations Between the Motor and Cognitive Dimensions of Preschool Girls and Boys. Perceptual and Motor Skills 94, 415423.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ponitz, CC, McClelland, MM, Matthews, JS and Morrison, FJ (2009) A structured observation of behavioral self-regulation and its contribution to kindergarten outcomes. Developmental Psychology 45, 605619.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ponitz, CEC, McClelland, MM, Jewkes, AM, Connor, CMD, Farris, CL and Morrison, FJ (2008) Touch your toes! Developing a direct measure of behavioral regulation in early childhood. Early Childhood Research Quarterly 23, 141158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Puder, JJ, Marques-Vidal, P, Schindler, C, Zahner, L, Niederer, I, Bürgi, F, Ebenegger, V, Nydegger, A and Kriemler, S (2011) Effect of multidimensional lifestyle intervention on fitness and adiposity in predominantly migrant preschool children (Ballabeina): Cluster randomised controlled trial. BMJ (Clinical Research Ed.) 343, d6195.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Putnam, SP and Rothbart, MK (2006) Development of short and very short forms of the children’s behavior questionnaire. Journal of Personality Assessment 87, 102112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, LE and Goodway, JD (2009) Instructional climates in preschool children who are at-risk. Part I: Object-control skill development. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport 80, 533542.Google ScholarPubMed
Robinson, LE, Palmer, KK and Bub, KL (2016) Effect of the Children’s Health Activity Motor Program on Motor Skills and Self-Regulation in Head Start Preschoolers: an Efficacy Trial. Frontiers in Public Health 4, 173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, LE, Stodden, DF, Barnett, LM, Lopes, VP, Logan, SW, Rodrigues, LP and D’Hondt, E (2015) Motor Competence and its Effect on Positive Developmental Trajectories of Health. Sports Medicine 45, 12731284.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robinson, LE, Veldman, SLC, Palmer, KK and Okely, AD (2017) A Ball Skills Intervention in Preschoolers: The CHAMP Randomized Controlled Trial. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise 49, 22342239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothbart, MK, Ahadi, SA, Hershey, KL and Fisher, P (2001) Investigations of temperament at three to seven years: the Children’s Behavior Questionnaire. Child Development 72, 13941408.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seefeldt, V, Nadeau, CH, Halliwell, WR, Newell, KM and Roberts, GC (1980) Developmental motor patterns. Implications for elementary school physical education. In Psychology of motor behavior and sport (p. S. 314-323). Human Kinetics.Google Scholar
Spessato, BC, Gabbard, C, Valentini, N and Rudisill, M (2013) Gender differences in Brazilian children’s fundamental movement skill performance. Early Child Development and Care 183, 916923.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
State Sport General Admistration. (2003) The National Physical Fitness Measurement Standards Manual (Preschool children Version). http://www.sport.gov.cn/n16/n41308/n41323/n41345/n41426/n42527/n42587/171330.html Google Scholar
Stodden, DF, Gao, Z, Goodway, JD and Langendorfer, SJ (2014) Dynamic relationships between motor skill competence and health-related fitness in youth. Pediatric Exercise Science 26, 231241.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stodden, DF, Goodway, JD, Langendorfer, SJ, Roberton, MA, Rudisill, ME, Garcia, C and Garcia, LE (2008) A Developmental Perspective on the Role of Motor Skill Competence in Physical Activity: an Emergent Relationship. Quest 60, 290306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stokols, D (1992) Establishing and maintaining healthy environments: toward a social ecology of health promotion. American Psychologist 47, 622.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taggart, A and Keegan, L (1997) Developing fundamental movement skills in outdoor settings: three case studies of children playing. ACHPER Healthy Lifestyles Journal 158, 1117.Google Scholar
Taunton, SA, Mulvey, KL and Brian, AS (2018) Who SKIPS? Using Temperament to Explain Differential Outcomes of a Motor Competence Intervention for Preschoolers. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport 89, 200209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thelen, E (1992) Development as a Dynamic System. Current Directions in Psychological Science 1, 189193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Timmons, BW, Leblanc, AG, Carson, V, Connor Gorber, S, Dillman, C, Janssen, I, Kho, ME, Spence, JC, Stearns, JA and Tremblay, MS (2012) Systematic review of physical activity and health in the early years (aged 0-4 years). Applied Physiology, Nutrition, and Metabolism 37, 773792.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
True, L, Pfeiffer, KA, Dowda, M, Williams, HG, Brown, WH, O’Neill, JR and Pate, RR (2017) Motor competence and characteristics within the preschool environment. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport 20, 751755.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ulrich, DA (2013) The Test of Gross Motor Development-3 (TGMD-3): Administration, Scoring, & International Norms. Spor Bilimleri Dergisi 24, 2733.Google Scholar
van Beurden, E, Zask, A, Barnett, L and Dietrich, U (2002) Fundamental movement skills — How do primary school children perform? The ‘Move it Groove it’ program in rural Australia. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport 5, 244252.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Capelle, A, Broderick, CR, van Doorn, N, Ward, RE and Parmenter, BJ (2017) Interventions to improve fundamental motor skills in pre-school aged children: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport 20, 658666.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van der Fels, IMJ, te Wierike, SCM, Hartman, E, Elferink-Gemser, MT, Smith, J and Visscher, C (2015) The relationship between motor skills and cognitive skills in 4–16year old typically developing children: A systematic review. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport 18, 697703.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vandendriessche, JB, Vandorpe, BFR, Vaeyens, R, Malina, RM, Lefevre, J, Lenoir, M and Philippaerts, RM (2012) Variation in sport participation, fitness and motor coordination with socioeconomic status among Flemish children. Pediatric Exercise Science 24, 113128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Veldman, SLC, Palmer, KK, Okely, AD and Robinson, LE (2017) Promoting ball skills in preschool-age girls. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport 20, 5054.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wadsworth, DD, Johnson, JL, Carroll, AV, Pangelinan, MM, Rudisill, ME and Sassi, J (2020) Intervention Strategies to Elicit MVPA in Preschoolers during Outdoor Play. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Westendorp, M, Houwen, S, Hartman, E, Mombarg, R, Smith, J and Visscher, C (2014) Effect of a Ball Skill Intervention on Children’s Ball Skills and Cognitive Functions. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise 46, 414422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whitehead, M (2013) Definition of physical literacy and clarification of related. Journal of Sport Science and Physical Education 65, 28–33.Google Scholar
WHO (2020) WHO guidelines on physical activity and sedentary behaviour, p. 93. Available at: https://apps.who.int/iris/bitstream/handle/10665/337001/9789240014916-ara.pdf (Accessed: 19 May 2021).Google Scholar
WHO Team (2019) Guidelines on physical activity, sedentary behaviour and sleep for children under 5 years of age. Geneva, p. 33. Available at: https://apps.who.int/iris/bitstream/handle/10665/311664/9789240001749-chi.pdf?sequence=34&isAllowed=y (Accessed: 1 May 2021).Google Scholar
Wick, K, Leeger-Aschmann, CS, Monn, ND, Radtke, T, Ott, LV, Rebholz, CE, Cruz, S, Gerber, N, Schmutz, EA, Puder, JJ, Munsch, S, Kakebeeke, TH, Jenni, OG, Granacher, U and Kriemler, S (2017) Interventions to Promote Fundamental Movement Skills in Childcare and Kindergarten: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Sports Medicine 47, 20452068.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wu, H, Ruan, H and Zhang, X (2017) Correlation study of fundamental movement skill development and related influencing factors in primary school. Chinese Journal of Child Health Care 25, 935938.Google Scholar
Zeng, N, Ayyub, M, Sun, H, Wen, X, Xiang, P and Gao, Z (2017) Effects of Physical Activity on Motor Skills and Cognitive Development in Early Childhood: A Systematic Review. BioMed Research International 2017, 2760716.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zeng, N, Johnson, SL, Boles, RE and Bellows, LL (2019) Social-ecological correlates of fundamental movement skills in young children. Journal of Sport and Health Science 8, 122129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Theoretical framework of this project

Figure 1

Figure 2. Study procedure. HS: Hello Sunshine PE curriculum; OPE: Ordinary PE curriculum; FP: Free play PE

Figure 2

Table 1. Domains and factors measured by the tests