Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-24T02:35:30.707Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Use of second-person pronouns and schizophrenia

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Andrew R. Watson*
Affiliation:
Division of Psychiatry, University of Edinburgh, Royal Edinburgh Hospital
Çaǧla Defteralı
Affiliation:
School of Philosophy Psychology and Language Sciences, University of Edinburgh
Thomas H. Bak
Affiliation:
School of Philosophy Psychology and Language Sciences, University of Edinburgh
Antonella Sorace
Affiliation:
School of Philosophy Psychology and Language Sciences, University of Edinburgh
Andrew M. McIntosh
Affiliation:
Division of Psychiatry, University of Edinburgh, Royal Edinburgh Hospital, Edinburgh, UK
David G. C. Owens
Affiliation:
Division of Psychiatry, University of Edinburgh, Royal Edinburgh Hospital, Edinburgh, UK
Eve C. Johnstone
Affiliation:
Division of Psychiatry, University of Edinburgh, Royal Edinburgh Hospital, Edinburgh, UK
Stephen M. Lawrie
Affiliation:
Division of Psychiatry, University of Edinburgh, Royal Edinburgh Hospital, Edinburgh, UK
*
Dr Andrew Watson, Division of Psychiatry, University of Edinburgh, Kennedy Tower, Royal Edinburgh Hospital, Morningside Place, Edinburgh EH10 5HF, UK. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Summary

A masked analysis of videotaped assessments of people at high genetic risk of schizophrenia revealed that those who subsequently went on to develop schizophrenia used significantly more second-person pronouns. This was evident before diagnosis, at two separate assessments approximately 18 months apart. This supports the view that people who go on to develop schizophrenia may have an abnormality in the deictic frame of interpersonal communication – that is, the distinction between concepts being self-generated or from elsewhere may be blurred prior to the onset of a diagnosis of schizophrenia.

Type
Short report
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2012 

Most speech contains a significant amount of errors, although these are typically corrected by the speaker almost immediately and do not result in the listener failing to understand the message. Reference Ferreira and Patson1 The gross abnormalities in psychotic speech, epitomised by formal thought disorder, are characterised by the speaker not having insight into the incomprehensibility of the errors they are making; indeed, it often seems that the person is convinced that the language they are using is completely understandable to the listener. Reference Covington, He, Brown, Naci, McClain and Fjordbak2 Studies of language abnormalities in schizophrenia have varied from transcribed analysis of single words Reference Thomas, Kearney, Napier, Ellis, Leudar and Johnson3 through to observer-rated scales looking at the proportion of time the speech is incomprehensible or disfluent in an interview. Reference Andreasen4 Abnormal use of referents in speech has also been found in schizophrenia Reference Champagne-Lavau, Fossard, Martel, Chapdelaine, Blouin and Rodriguez5 but importantly all these analyses have taken place at different points during the illness, and there has been very little opportunity to analyse the speech of people before they develop schizophrenia. The effects of an acute psychotic episode, of medication, the organisation of services and the response of the person to a diagnosis all may have an effect on the communication styles of people with an established diagnosis. The Edinburgh High Risk Study (EHRS) - and in particular videotaped interviews of some participants - offers the opportunity to prospectively and blindly examine the use of language of a group of people who are at high genetic risk of schizophrenia before they meet diagnostic criteria for psychosis. Reference Owens, Miller, Lawrie and Johnstone6

Method

The EHRS has been described in full elsewhere. Reference Owens, Miller, Lawrie and Johnstone6 Briefly, people at high genetic risk of schizophrenia, from multiply affected families, were identified and a control group from a similar social background was also recruited. Repeated assessments at approximately 18-month intervals were conducted. At each assessment a number of tests were done including magnetic resonance imaging brain scans, psychometric tests and a Present State Examination (PSE), Reference Wing, Cooper and Sartorius7 which was videotaped with the participant's consent. All the interviews were conducted by two researchers with extensive experience using the PSE (E.C.J. and D.C.G.O.). Over the course of the study, a high proportion of participants returned for follow-up assessments: 82% of the high-risk group and 75% of the control group. The EHRS split its participants into four groups, based on the PSE: those at high risk who subsequently developed schizophrenia; those who had psychotic symptoms but who did not develop schizophrenia; those at high risk without any psychotic experiences; and controls. In this study, we included a subgroup of the total EHRS population who all had two video-taped assessments in the first 5 years of the study. Table 1 shows the demographic data of the four groups. Owens et al Reference Owens, Miller, Lawrie and Johnstone6 details the demographics and symptoms profile of the whole EHRS study. There were no significant differences between the findings in that paper and the sub-sample used in the present study.

From the PSE, a 200-word transcript was taken. Once 200 words were reached, the analysis continued until the end of the answer of that question (mean number of words analysed 202.9). The sample of speech was taken from the first part of the interview, which concerns general physical and mental well-being, and was never further than the question regarding obsessional thoughts in the PSE. The median section of the PSE reached was number 7: ‘appetite, sleep, retardation, libido’. Reference Wing, Cooper and Sartorius7 There was no significant difference in the section reached between the groups or between the two interviewers. The transcriber and rater were masked as to whether the person had psychotic symptoms at the time of the interview or not, just as they were masked to group status and whether or not the person subsequently developed schizophrenia (six individuals did, an average of 943 days (s.e. = 160) after the first assessment). The transcripts were scanned a number of times by the rater to identify the parts of speech used. The percentage of the total number of words used for each speech variable was then obtained. The variables obtained were: singular and plural nouns; continuous, past tense and infinitive verbs; pronouns; articles; conjunctives; and errors. Errors were grouped in the analysis and consisted of self-repair, staggers, repeats and pause fillers.

Multivariate analysis of variance was performed to examine the differences between the groups at the participants' first assessment. Repeated measures analysis of variance was performed to examine differences between the groups over time, from first assessment to second assessment. Participant IQ and social class were entered as covariates. Significant results were then examined using one-way ANOVA, as reported below. The significance level was adjusted for multiple tests by the Bonferroni method.

Results

There were no significant differences between the high-risk group as a whole and the controls on any measure, nor were there significant differences between those within the high-risk group who had experienced psychotic symptoms and those who had not.

TABLE 1 Demographic details

High-risk group
Control group (n = 8) No psychotic experience (n = 12) Psychotic symptoms, no schizophrenia (n = 12) Schizophrenia (n = 6)
Gender, n
    Male 4 4 5 4
    Female 4 8 7 2
Age, years: mean (s.d.) 22.3 (2.0) 21.5 (3.0) 22.0 (3.2) 19.0 (1.5)
Social class, median 4 3 4 4
National Adult Reading Test, Reference Nelson and Willison11 mean (s.d.) 102.3 (12.0) 102.5 (9.5) 98.7 (9.22) 103.6 (12.7)

Within the high-risk group, however, those who developed schizophrenia used significantly more second-person pronouns than those who did not (P = 0.005, d.f. = 1, F = 9.4). As a percentage of the total words used, those who developed schizophrenia used second-person pronouns 0.5% of the time and those who did not used them 0.25% of the time (range 0-1.46). An example indicative of the abnormality is: patient: ‘You mentioned…’; interviewer: ‘No, you were telling me about…’.

The same pattern was found when the groups were compared across two assessments. Only the use of second-person pronouns within the high-risk group was significant, differing between those who developed schizophrenia and those who did not (P = 0.003, d.f. = 1, F = 11.7). There were no significant differences between the high-risk group and controls, and no significant differences between those who had had psychotic symptoms and those who had not.

Discussion

Pronouns are deictic words that, while having fixed semantic meaning, require consideration of place or time to have specific, denotational meaning. Reference Buhler and Goodwin8 Repetition of what another person says is not an option when using a second-person pronoun. They place the representative functions of speech, such as describing a table, in the appropriate interpersonal (and time/place) context for accurate communication between speaker and listener. Reference Buhler and Goodwin8 Difficulty in establishing the difference between ‘you’ and ‘I’ leads to this structuring frame of the representative functions of speech being disturbed and can make communication unintelligible. Reference Buhler and Goodwin8,Reference Crow9 Broadly intact representational memory structure has been described in schizophrenia, with abnormalities found when using words to construct relational interpretations. Reference Titone, Libben, Niman, Ranbom and Levy10 Abnormality in the deictic frame has been proposed as a fundamental disturbance in schizophrenia, underlying the blurred boundary between self and other seen across psychotic symptoms. Reference Crow9 The abnormal use of pronouns was found at two time points in the study and was consistent despite changes in the symptom profile in both the people who did and those who did not go on to develop schizophrenia. This suggests it reflects a more fundamental abnormality in people with schizophrenia's experience of the world rather than being an association with a specific symptom cluster.

Funding

This work was funded by the UK Medical Research Council.

Footnotes

Declaration of interest

None.

References

1 Ferreira, F, Patson, N. The ‘good enough’ approach to language comprehension. Lang Linguist Compass 2007; 1: 7183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
2 Covington, MA, He, C, Brown, C, Naci, L, McClain, JT, Fjordbak, BS, et al. Schizophrenia and the structure of language: the linguist's view. Schizophr Res 2005; 77: 8598.Google Scholar
3 Thomas, P, Kearney, G, Napier, E, Ellis, E, Leudar, I, Johnson, M. The reliability and characteristics of the brief syntactic analysis. Br J Psychiatry 1996; 168: 334–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
4 Andreasen, NC. Scale for the assessment of thought, language, and communication (TLC). Schizophr Bull 1986; 12: 473–82.Google Scholar
5 Champagne-Lavau, M, Fossard, M, Martel, G, Chapdelaine, C, Blouin, G, Rodriguez, JP, et al. Do patients with schizophrenia attribute mental states in a referential communication task? Cogn Neuropsychiatry 2009; 14: 217–39.Google Scholar
6 Owens, DGC, Miller, P, Lawrie, SM, Johnstone, EC. Pathogenesis of schizophrenia: a psychopathological perspective. Br J Psychiatry 2005; 186: 386–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
7 Wing, JK, Cooper, JE, Sartorius, N. The Description and Classification of Psychiatric Symptoms. An Instruction Manual for the PSE and CATEGO Systems. Cambridge University Press, 1974.Google Scholar
8 Buhler, K. Sprachtheorie. 1934. Reprinted as Theory of Language: The Representational Function of Language (trans. Goodwin, DF). John Benjamins Publishing Company, 1984.Google Scholar
9 Crow, TJ. The nuclear symptoms of schizophrenia reveal the four quadrant structure of language and its deictic frame. J Neurolinguistics 2010; 23: 19.Google Scholar
10 Titone, D, Libben, M, Niman, M, Ranbom, L, Levy, D. Conceptual combination in schizophrenia: Contrasting property and relational interpretations. J Neurolinguistics 2007; 20: 92110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
11 Nelson, H, Willison, J. National Adult Reading Test. nferNelson, 1991.Google Scholar
Figure 0

TABLE 1 Demographic details

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.