Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dzt6s Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-24T19:42:22.818Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The role of cognitive and social leisure activities in dementia risk: assessing longitudinal associations of modifiable and non-modifiable risk factors

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 January 2022

L. A. Duffner
Affiliation:
Alzheimer Centrum Limburg, School for Mental Health and Neuroscience (MHeNs), Department of Psychiatry and Neuropsychology, Faculty of Health, Medicine and Life Sciences, Maastricht University, The Netherlands
K. Deckers
Affiliation:
Alzheimer Centrum Limburg, School for Mental Health and Neuroscience (MHeNs), Department of Psychiatry and Neuropsychology, Faculty of Health, Medicine and Life Sciences, Maastricht University, The Netherlands
D. Cadar
Affiliation:
Department of Behavioural Science and Health, Institute of Epidemiology and Health, University College London, London, UK
A. Steptoe
Affiliation:
Department of Behavioural Science and Health, Institute of Epidemiology and Health, University College London, London, UK
M. de Vugt
Affiliation:
Alzheimer Centrum Limburg, School for Mental Health and Neuroscience (MHeNs), Department of Psychiatry and Neuropsychology, Faculty of Health, Medicine and Life Sciences, Maastricht University, The Netherlands
S. Köhler*
Affiliation:
Alzheimer Centrum Limburg, School for Mental Health and Neuroscience (MHeNs), Department of Psychiatry and Neuropsychology, Faculty of Health, Medicine and Life Sciences, Maastricht University, The Netherlands
*
Author for correspondence: S. Köhler, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Aims

With the projected surge in global dementia cases and no curative treatment available, research is increasingly focusing on lifestyle factors as preventive measures. Social and cognitive leisure activities are promising targets, but it is unclear which types of activities are more beneficial. This study investigated the individual and joint contribution of cognitive and social leisure activities to dementia risk and whether they modify the risks associated with other potentially modifiable and non-modifiable risk factors.

Methods

We used data from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ELSA) from 7917 participants, followed up from 2008/2009 (Wave 4) until 2018/2019 (Wave 9) for incident dementia. Self-reported baseline cognitive activities (e.g. ‘reading the newspaper’), the number of social memberships (e.g. being a member of a social club) and social participation (e.g. ‘going to the cinema’) were clustered into high and low based on a median split. Subsequently, their individual and joint contribution to dementia risk, as well as their interaction with other dementia risk factors, were assessed with Cox regression models, adjusting for age, sex, level of education, wealth and a composite score of 11 lifestyle-related dementia risk factors.

Results

After a median follow-up period of 9.8 years, the dementia incidence rate was 54.5 cases per 10.000 person-years (95% CI 49.0–60.8). Adjusting for demographic and other lifestyle-related risk factors, higher engagement in cognitive activities (HR = 0.58; 95% CI 0.40–0.84), a greater number of social memberships (HR = 0.65; 95% CI 0.51–0.84) and more social participation (HR = 0.71; 95% CI 0.54–0.95) were associated with lower dementia risk. In a joint model, only engagement in cognitive activities (HR = 0.60; 95% CI 0.40–0.91) and social memberships (HR = 0.75; 95% CI 0.56–0.99) independently explained dementia risk. We did not find any interaction with other modifiable and non-modifiable risk factors.

Conclusions

Engagement in cognitive and social leisure activities may be beneficial for overall dementia risk, independent of each other and other risk factors. Both types of activities may be potential targets for dementia prevention measures and health advice initiatives.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Dementia is a public health priority due to rising numbers in a continuously ageing world population (World Health Organization, 2018). Next to the emotional and care burden placed upon people with dementia and their families, dementia is also associated with major costs for society (Patterson, Reference Patterson2018). While certain risk factors such as age, sex, genetic predisposition and low parental socioeconomic status (SES) are non-modifiable, overall dementia risk is not set in stone (Livingston et al., Reference Livingston, Huntley, Sommerlad, Ames, Ballard, Banerjee, Brayne, Burns, Cohen-Mansfield, Cooper, Costafreda, Dias, Fox, Gitlin, Howard, Kales, Kivimäki, Larson, Ogunniyi, Orgeta, Ritchie, Rockwood, Sampson, Samus, Schneider, Selbaek, Teri and Mukadam2020; Steyaert et al., Reference Steyaert, Deckers, Smits, Fox, Thyrian, Jeon, Vernooij-Dassen and Köhler2020). According to the 2020 report of the Lancet Commission on Dementia Prevention, Intervention and Care, 40% of the risk is changeable by means of tackling modifiable risk factors (Livingston et al., Reference Livingston, Huntley, Sommerlad, Ames, Ballard, Banerjee, Brayne, Burns, Cohen-Mansfield, Cooper, Costafreda, Dias, Fox, Gitlin, Howard, Kales, Kivimäki, Larson, Ogunniyi, Orgeta, Ritchie, Rockwood, Sampson, Samus, Schneider, Selbaek, Teri and Mukadam2020). These, amongst others, include lifestyle factors such as adherence to a healthy (Mediterranean) diet, physical activity, as well as the timely management of depression, hypertension, hypercholesterolemia and diabetes (Petersson and Philippou, Reference Petersson and Philippou2016; Wium-Andersen et al., Reference Wium-Andersen, Rungby, Jørgensen, Sandbæk, Osler and Wium-Andersen2019; Thomassen et al., Reference Thomassen, Schurmann Tolstrup, Benn and Frikke-Schmidt2020; Litke et al., Reference Litke, Cancino Garcharna, Jiwani and Neugroschl2021).

A growing body of research also points to the engagement in cognitively stimulating leisure activities as protective against cognitive decline and dementia. For instance, a systematic review found that engagement in cognitively stimulating leisure activities in middle adulthood as well as in late life was associated with a lower risk for developing Alzheimer's disease and other dementias (Stern and Munn, Reference Stern and Munn2010). Research also supports evidence for the relationship between social activities and dementia risk. A meta-analysis showed that less frequent social contacts, higher levels of loneliness and lower levels of social interaction were related to higher dementia incidence (Kuiper et al., Reference Kuiper, Zuidersma, Oude Voshaar, Zuidema, van den Heuvel, Stolk and Smidt2015).

While mentally stimulating and social leisure activities appear to be promising factors for curbing cognitive decline and decreasing dementia risk, some studies with long follow-up suggest that reversed causality might be at play, where impending dementia leads to disengagement from cognitive and social activities (Foubert-Samier et al., Reference Foubert-Samier, Le Goff, Helmer, Pérès, Orgogozo, Barberger-Gateau, Amieva and Dartigues2014; Floud et al., Reference Floud, Balkwill, Sweetland, Brown, Mauricio Reus, Hofman, Blacker, Kivimaki, Green, Peto, Reeves and Beral2021). Furthermore, activities are often correlated, with cognitive activities having a substantive social component and vice versa. As both types of activities are usually studied in isolation, independent effects have thus far not been well established. In addition, only a few studies investigated their potential to moderate, or even compensate for, the influence of other modifiable and non-modifiable (age, sex, education, socioeconomic position) risk factors. A recent study assessed whether sex and marital status modified the association between engagement in leisure activities and dementia risk (Almeida-Meza et al., Reference Almeida-Meza, Steptoe and Cadar2021). Through independent analyses for various activities, the authors found that for specific subgroups of participants, some activities (reading the newspaper for females and mobile phone usage for males) independently predicted dementia risk.

Despite the fact that many studies included additional risk factors (including lifestyle) as covariates, they did not separately assess whether their associations could be moderated by the engagement in cognitive and social activities (Fancourt et al., Reference Fancourt, Steptoe and Cadar2018, Reference Fancourt, Steptoe and Cadar2020; Almeida-Meza et al., Reference Almeida-Meza, Steptoe and Cadar2021). However, in order to be able to tailor dementia prevention programmes to especially at-risk people, the potential of activity engagement for modifying the association between lifestyle risk and dementia has to be examined in more depth. While one study found that stimulating activities, as measured by a composite of education, occupation and leisure activities, may buffer the impact of diabetes on dementia risk (Marseglia et al., Reference Marseglia, Darin-Mattsson, Kalpouzos, Grande, Fratiglioni, Dekhtyar and Xu2020), there are, to date, no studies including a broader set of lifestyle factors.

The current study, therefore, aims at (a) investigating the independent as well as the joint contribution of cognitive and social leisure activities to dementia risk and (b) to study their potential to moderate the role of other modifiable and non-modifiable risk factors of dementia. We expect that higher numbers of cognitive and social leisure activities have independent associations with dementia and that they may moderate the influence of several other risk factors such as low education, low wealth and lifestyle factors.

Methods

Study design and participants

Data were extracted from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ELSA), a multi-centre panel study initiated in 1998 (Phelps et al., Reference Phelps, Marmot, Oskala, Clemens, Banks, Rogers, Steptoe, Blake, Nazroo and Oldfield2020), and representative of the general English population aged 50 and older. ELSA data collection takes place in 2-year intervals, and amongst others, entails information about health and social well-being, lifestyle, psychological factors and economic status. Details about the sampling and data collection procedures have already been specified elsewhere (Steptoe et al., Reference Steptoe, Breeze, Banks and Nazroo2013). This study used data of Wave 4 (2008/2009; n = 11 050) as the baseline, as this wave is the most complete with regard to information about lifestyle factors. In case of unavailability of variables of interest, data of Wave 3 (2006/2007) or Wave 5 (2010/2011) were used alternatively. The final assessment took place at Wave 9 (2018/2019), yielding a maximum observational period of 11 years. Prevalent dementia cases at Wave 4 (n = 213) and people who ceased their participation after Wave 4 (n = 890) were excluded from further analysis. Furthermore, those with information on fewer than 11 factors of the ‘LIfestyle for BRAin health’ (LIBRA)-score (Deckers et al., Reference Deckers, van Boxtel, Schiepers, de Vugt, Sánchez, Anstey, Brayne, Dartigues, Engedal, Kivipelto, Ritchie, Starr, Yaffe, Irving, Verhey and Köhler2015) (n = 2006) and missing information about education (n = 8) and invalid sampling weights (n = 16) were excluded. The final analytical sample included 7917 participants. Information about panel attrition between Wave 4 and Wave 9 can be found in online Supplementary Table 1.

Instruments and measures

Dementia ascertainment

Dementia diagnoses were ascertained through either physician diagnosis of dementia or Alzheimer's disease (self or informant-reported) or the total score on the 16-item Informant Score Questionnaire on Cognitive Decline in the Elderly (IQCODE; Jorm, Reference Jorm1994). The IQCODE is an informant-based dementia screening tool, in which a family member or caregiver is asked to rate changes in cognitive functioning of a person over a 2-year period (e.g. ‘Ability to remember the own address or telephone number as compared to the previous interview’). The IQCODE has shown satisfactory psychometric properties across various populations (see Jorm, Reference Jorm2004 for a review). Possible scores range from 1 (much improved) to 5 (much worse). The chosen cut-off of an average score of 3.38 is regarded as suggestive for pathological cognitive decline and has proven an appropriate trade-off between sensitivity (0.82) and specificity (0.84; Quinn et al., Reference Quinn, Fearon, Noel-Storr, Young, McShane and Stott2014).

Cognitive and social activity engagement

Information about engagement in cognitive activities was collected by questionnaires administered during the Wave 4 assessment. Questions about activities were asked in a closed manner. Cognitive activities included reading the newspaper, having a hobby, having taken a holiday or owning a mobile phone. Social activities were assessed in two ways. On the one hand, the membership of various clubs or societies, such as a political party, tenant groups, religious groups or charitable associations (social memberships), was assessed. On the other hand, social participation was inferred by questions about how frequently participants visited an art gallery, museum, theatre, concert or cinema, and how often the respondent ate out. Choices ranged from ‘twice a month’ to ‘never’. The number or frequency of engagement of the respective activities was summed up to obtain total scores for cognitive activities (theoretical range 0–7), social memberships (theoretical range 0–8) and social participation levels (theoretical range 0–20), respectively. For the present study, scores were clustered into high and low, based on a median split. Social memberships and social participation were used as separate variables for the further course of the analysis.

Other lifestyle factors

Additional modifiable lifestyle factors were assessed using the LIBRA score (Deckers et al., Reference Deckers, van Boxtel, Schiepers, de Vugt, Sánchez, Anstey, Brayne, Dartigues, Engedal, Kivipelto, Ritchie, Starr, Yaffe, Irving, Verhey and Köhler2015) based on data from Wave 4. The LIBRA score is a well-validated summary score, based on the relative contributions of 12 risk and protective factors for dementia: physical inactivity, smoking, alcohol use, diet, hypertension, cognitive activity, depression, obesity, diabetes, coronary heart disease, kidney disease (not available/measured in ELSA) and hypercholesterolemia (Vos et al., Reference Vos, van Boxtel, Schiepers, Deckers, de Vugt, Carrière, Dartigues, Peres, Artero, Ritchie, Galluzzo, Scafato, Frisoni, Huisman, Comijs, Sacuiu, Skoog, Irving, O'Donnell, Verhey, Visser and Köhler2017; Deckers et al., Reference Deckers, Köhler, van Boxtel, Verhey, Brayne and Fleming2018, Reference Deckers, Cadar, van Boxtel, Verhey, Steptoe and Köhler2019a, Reference Deckers, Nooyens, van Boxtel, Verhey, Verschuren and Köhler2019b, Reference Deckers, Barbera, Köhler, Ngandu, van Boxtel, Rusanen, Laatikainen, Verhey, Soininen, Kivipelto and Solomon2020; Schiepers et al., Reference Schiepers, Köhler, Deckers, Irving, O'Donnell, van den Akker, Verhey, Vos, de Vugt and van Boxtel2018). The complete LIBRA index ranges from −5.9 to +12.7, with higher scores representing higher dementia risk. For the purpose of this study, however, the measure of cognitive activity was excluded from the weighting, yielding an adapted theoretical range of −2.7 to +12.7 for the adjusted LIBRA (LIBRAadj). A detailed summary of the construction of the LIBRAadj scores can be found in online Supplementary Table 2 (adapted from Deckers et al., Reference Deckers, Cadar, van Boxtel, Verhey, Steptoe and Köhler2019a).

Socioeconomic status

The total net wealth of a household was considered as a proxy for resource-based SES at Wave 4. This was derived by summing up the value of possessions and assets and subtracting open mortgages and payments. The resulting relative amounts were then divided into tertiles, representing low, medium and high wealth.

Demographics

Information on age, sex and level of education was collected through questionnaires administered at Wave 4. The level of education was based on the highest academic degree obtained. Educational attainments were then categorised into low (no formal education), medium (ordinary level or secondary education) and high (college/university) education.

Ethical approval

ELSA received ethical approval from the National Health Service Multicentre Research and Ethics Committee and the University College London Research Ethics Committee. All participants provided written informed consent.

Statistical analysis

The difference in demographic information, cognitive activities, social memberships, social participation, wealth, education and LIBRAadj scores between people with and without dementia was assessed by independent samples t-tests or χ 2-tests. In case of violation of assumptions, a non-parametric alternative (Mann–Whitney U test) was chosen. For subsequent analyses, only cases without missing information on the specific activity variable were considered. Cox proportional hazards regression was used to study the individual contribution of cognitive activities, social memberships and social participation on dementia risk, controlling for age, sex, LIBRAadj score, net-wealth and education, resulting in hazard ratios (HRs) and their 95% confidence intervals (CIs). In the separate analyses per activity type, variables were added in a stepwise manner, starting with a minimally (age and sex) adjusted model (model 1), followed by a model that was additionally adjusted for education (model 2). A third model then further adjusted for wealth (model 3), followed by the full model also adjusting for LIBRAadj scores (model 4). Covariates were added in this specific order to assess changes to the association between the specific activity variable and dementia by the particular modifiable and non-modifiable risk factor. Model 4 is the main model used for testing the hypotheses of interest.

We then assessed the interactions between the three activity domains following the same approach as described above (models 1–4). Finally, the separate interactions between cognitive activities, social memberships or social participation with other modifiable (LIBRAadj) and non-modifiable (sex, education and net-wealth) risk factors were subsequently examined in the same manner. In all analyses, dementia was treated as the failure event. Survival time was defined as the period from birth until either the onset of dementia, the last interview or death (whichever came first). By defining survival time this way, age was considered in the time scale for all analyses. We used the Schoenfeld Residuals Test (Schoenfeld, Reference Schoenfeld1982) and clog-log plots to examine the proportional hazard assumption. Furthermore, a sampling weight (baseline cross-sectional weight) was used in order to back-weight estimates from the analytical sample to the total sample to minimise selection bias. In ELSA, participants can also be selected from the same household, and we therefore used a robust sandwich estimator to adjust standard errors for household clusters. All analyses were conducted in Stata (version 14.2; StataCorp, 2015), and the level of statistical significance was p < 0.05 in two-sided tests.

Results

Sample characteristics

Participants were followed up for a median duration of 9.8 years (IQR = 4.1 years). Until the end of the observation period, 360 individuals (4.5%) had developed dementia, yielding an incidence rate of 54.5 cases per 10 000 person-years (95% CI 49.0–60.8). The mean age was 65.9 years (s.d. = 9.5 years; range: 50–99), and 3349 participants (45.1%) were female. Baseline characteristics are summarised by dementia status in Table 1.

Table 1. Baseline characteristics of ELSA Wave 4 participants by incident dementia status

s.d., standard deviation; LIBRAadj score, LIfestyle for BRAin health score without the weight for cognitive activity.

1 Statistically significant at p≤0.05

2 Observed range −2.7 to 10.3 (theoretical range: −2.7 to 12.7).

Questionnaires about leisure activity engagement have been returned by approximately 80% of the total sample. There were 644 participants with missing information on cognitive activities. They more likely had lower LIBRAadj scores (z = 6.45; p < 0.001), and lower net-wealth (z = 7.40; p < 0.001) in comparison to people without missing information. Furthermore, 908 participants had missing information on social memberships. They were more likely to have dementia (χ 2 = 7.87; p = 0.005), were younger (z = −2.61; p = 0.01), had a lower education (χ 2 = 57.39; p < 0.001) and lower net-wealth (t = 7.02; p < 0.001) than those without missing information. People with missing information on social participation (n = 1183) more likely had dementia (χ 2 = 19.28; p < 0.001), were older (z = −8.62; p < 0.001), were more often female (χ 2 = 3.90; p = 0.048), had a lower education (χ 2 = 82.17; p < 0.001), had higher LIBRAadj scores (z = −2.49; p = 0.013) and a lower net-wealth (t = 6.46; p < 0.001).

Individual associations of cognitive activities, social memberships and social participation with dementia risk

In model 1, participation in a higher number of cognitive activities was associated with lower risk for dementia. This association remained significant in model 4. Table 2 presents a more detailed description of the stepwise analysis. Cumulative hazard curves by level of cognitive activities, adjusted for all covariates, can be found in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1. Cumulative hazard estimates of incident dementia by baseline level of cognitive activities (low v. high) adjusted for age, sex, level of education, wealth and LIBRAadj scores.

Table 2. Stepwise Cox proportional hazard regression for the individual contribution of cognitive activity, social memberships and social participation

HR, hazard ratio; s.e., standard error; LIBRAadj, LIfestyle for BRAin health score without the weight for cognitive activity.

Model 1 = age and sex; Model 2 = model 1 + education; Model 3 = model 2 + wealth; Model 4 = model 3 + LIBRAadj.

1 Statistically significant at p≤0.05.

A higher number of social memberships was significantly associated with lower dementia incidence in model 1. This association was still observed in model 4. Similarly, more social participation was associated with lower dementia risk across all models. Cumulative hazard curves for social memberships and social participation, adjusted for covariates, can be found in Figs 2 and 3, respectively. In general, associations became attenuated but remained significant with incremental adjustment for non-modifiable and modifiable risk factors. In particular, wealth and LIBRAadj scores seemed to partly account for the associations, with the most substantial reduction in effect size for the relation between social participation and incident dementia.

Fig. 2. Cumulative hazard estimates of incident dementia by baseline level of social memberships (low v. high), adjusted for age, sex, level of education, wealth and LIBRAadj scores.

Fig. 3. Cumulative hazard estimates of incident dementia by baseline level of social participation (low v. high), adjusted for age, sex, level of education, wealth and LIBRAadj scores.

The unique contribution of cognitive activities, social memberships and social participation

Table 3 presents multiple Cox proportional hazard models fitted to assess the joint contribution of the activity domains on dementia risk. The full model (model 4) thus included the three activities as predictors and age, sex, education, wealth and LIBRAadj scores as covariates. As compared with those reporting low engagement in cognitive activities, those high in cognitive activity showed a 40% lower risk for dementia (HR = 0.60; 95% CI 0.40–0.91; p = 0.017). Similarly, social memberships were still negatively associated with dementia risk though the association became modestly attenuated (HR = 0.74; 95% CI 0.56–0.99; p = 0.039), corresponding to a 26% lower risk in the high as compared to the low social membership group. Social participation, on the other hand, did not significantly predict dementia risk (HR = 0.81; 95% CI 0.60–1.10; p = 0.172).

Table 3. Interaction between levels of cognitive activity, social memberships, social participation and other dementia risk factors

HR, hazard ratio; s.e., standard error; LIBRAadj, LIfestyle for BRAin health score without the weight for cognitive activity.

1 Adjusted for age, education, wealth and LIBRAadj scores.

2 Adjusted for age, sex, wealth and LIBRAadj scores.

3 Adjusted for age, sex, education and LIBRAadj scores.

4 Adjusted for age, sex, education and wealth.

Interactions

We first tested the interaction between cognitive activity and the social activity variables. Neither the interaction between cognitive activities and social memberships (χ 2 = 0.19; df = 1; p = 0.661) nor the interaction between cognitive activities and social participation (χ 2 = 0.07; df = 1; p = 0.798) were statistically significant. Next, the interactions between the three activity variables and the other risk factors were assessed (Table 3). We did not observe significant interactions between cognitive activities, social memberships or social participation with sex, education, wealth and LIBRAadj scores.

Sensitivity analyses

Different sensitivity analyses were conducted to test the robustness of the associations outlined above. First, cognitive activities, social memberships and social participation were included as continuous variables. In the separate models, each increase in the respective activity domain was significantly associated with lower dementia risk, independent of the covariates. However, in the joint model, cognitive activities were significantly associated with dementia, while social memberships and social participation were not.

Next, multiple imputation (White et al., Reference White, Royston and Wood2011) was used to impute missing values of the activity variables, education, wealth and LIBRAadj factors. Multiple imputation by chained equations (MICE) was chosen, as this method allows for missing data on multiple variables (van Buuren and Groothuis-Oudshoorn, Reference van Buuren and Groothuis-Oudshoorn2000). Ten imputed datasets were created and combined using Rubin's rules (Rubin, Reference Rubin1996). The results of both the separate and joint models did not differ from the primary analyses.

In order to assess the possibility of reversed causality, we divided survival time into two parts, representing up to five years (incident dementia n = 147) and more than five years after baseline (incident dementia n = 213) and repeated the analyses with model 4. While in the first 5-year period, engagement in cognitive activities was strongly related to dementia risk in the separate model (HR = 0.42), it was not significant anymore in the second period (HR = 0.82). However, HRs for social memberships were comparable in both periods (HR first period = 0.69; HR second period = 0.70). For social participation, the association was stronger in the second period (HR = 0.77) as opposed to the first (HR = 0.85).

Discussion

In this prospective cohort study, the association between a cognitively and socially active lifestyle and incident dementia was assessed. Considering the multifacetedness of activity engagement, this study aimed at further disentangling the unique contributions of three activity domains on dementia risk. When studied separately, people who reported higher engagement in cognitive and social activities showed a lower risk for developing dementia as compared to people who reported lower engagement. These associations remained significant when adjusting for non-modifiable (age, sex, education, wealth) and modifiable (LIBRAadj scores) dementia risk factors. In addition, they did not interact with the latter, suggesting truly independent associations with dementia over and above other modifiable and non-modifiable risk factors. Furthermore, in the joint model including all three main predictors, associations for cognitive activities and social memberships and dementia risk were independent of each other. However, associations between social participation and dementia risk were attenuated and non-significant when considering the other activity domains.

These findings align with the growing body of literature underlining the relationship between engagement in cognitively stimulating and social leisure activities and dementia risk. This includes evidence of both cohort (Scarmeas et al., Reference Scarmeas, Levy, Tang, Manly and Stern2001; Wang et al., Reference Wang, Karp, Winblad and Fratiglioni2002; Fratiglioni et al., Reference Fratiglioni, Paillard-Borg and Winblad2004; Karp et al., Reference Karp, Paillard-Borg, Wang, Silverstein, Winblad and Fratiglioni2006; Foubert-Samier et al., Reference Foubert-Samier, Le Goff, Helmer, Pérès, Orgogozo, Barberger-Gateau, Amieva and Dartigues2014; Almeida-Meza et al., Reference Almeida-Meza, Steptoe and Cadar2021) and case-control studies (e.g. Fritsch et al., Reference Fritsch, Smyth, Debanne, Petot and Friedland2005; Lindstrom et al., Reference Lindstrom, Fritsch, Petot, Smyth, Chen, Debanne, Lerner and Friedland2005).

Cognitive leisure activities as well as social engagement have been suggested as socio-behavioural proxies for cognitive reserve, a heuristic concept proposed for explaining interindividual differences in cognitive functioning in the light of similar neuropathological burden (Stern, Reference Stern2002; Stern and Barulli, Reference Stern and Barulli2009; Stern et al., Reference Stern, Arenaza-Urquijo, Bartrés-Faz, Belleville, Cantilon, Chetelat, Ewers, Franzmeier, Kempermann, Kremen, Okonkwo, Scarmeas, Soldan, Udeh-Momoh, Valenzuela, Vemuri and Vuoksimaa2020). The underlying mechanisms behind cognitive reserve are largely unknown. However, changes in functional brain connectivity, especially in medial temporal and frontal regions, have been suggested (Anthony and Lin, Reference Anthony and Lin2018). Our findings fit with the idea of cognitive and social activities as being suitable proxies for cognitive reserve and show that they are independent of education and other dementia risk factors.

We also found that in the joint model, cognitive activities and social memberships were independently related to dementia risk, though the association between social memberships and dementia became modestly attenuated. Interestingly, this was also the case for cognitive activities, albeit to a lesser degree. Our findings align with those of Karp et al. (Reference Karp, Paillard-Borg, Wang, Silverstein, Winblad and Fratiglioni2006), who reported that mental and social components of activities were protective against dementia, independent of each other and after adjustment for covariates. On the other hand, social participation was not associated with dementia risk independent of cognitive activities and social memberships. The association between social participation and dementia was mainly attenuated by adding cognitive activities to the model and less so by adding social memberships. While the memberships of clubs and organisations and social activity engagement might be conceptually different entities, there may still be considerable overlap between the two. Our definition of social participation included activities such as going to museums and theatres, which often have both a social (going there with others) and cognitively stimulating component. Therefore, it does not come as a surprise that its association was explained by the other activities. Indeed, the three activity domains may lie on each other's causal pathway, in that memberships in more associations actually lead to more social participation, and they might partly relate to the same latent construct (cognitive reserve). Hence, the joint model might be over adjustment (Schisterman et al., Reference Schisterman, Cole and Platt2009).

While the current study consistently found positive prospective correlations between cognitive and social activity engagement and dementia risk over 11 years and across the different models, the risk for reversed causality cannot be entirely excluded. A large-scale longitudinal study of 851 307 women in the UK (Floud et al., Reference Floud, Balkwill, Sweetland, Brown, Mauricio Reus, Hofman, Blacker, Kivimaki, Green, Peto, Reeves and Beral2021) found that cognitive and social activities were associated with lower incident dementia within the first 4 years of follow-up, but the association diminished during the following 5–9 years of follow-up and was absent after more than 10 years of follow-up. The authors concluded that decreased activity engagement might actually be the result of impending dementia rather than a protective factor. Likewise, Eriksson Sörman et al. (Reference Eriksson Sörman, Sundström, Rönnlund, Adolfsson and Nilsson2014) reported that associations between social activity engagement and dementia were only significant in the first 5 years after baseline and disappeared thereafter. In the current study, engagement in cognitively stimulating activities was only strongly related to dementia within the first but not the second 5-year period, giving rise to the possibility of reversed causality, which is in line with the findings of Floud et al. (Reference Floud, Balkwill, Sweetland, Brown, Mauricio Reus, Hofman, Blacker, Kivimaki, Green, Peto, Reeves and Beral2021). However, contrary to both Floud et al. (Reference Floud, Balkwill, Sweetland, Brown, Mauricio Reus, Hofman, Blacker, Kivimaki, Green, Peto, Reeves and Beral2021) and Eriksson Sörman et al. (Reference Eriksson Sörman, Sundström, Rönnlund, Adolfsson and Nilsson2014), HRs between social memberships, social participation and dementia remained similar across both time periods, which may still support the possibility of a protective effect of social engagement for dementia. The differences in findings between the latter (Eriksson Sörman et al., Reference Eriksson Sörman, Sundström, Rönnlund, Adolfsson and Nilsson2014) and the current study could also have arisen through differences with regard to the operationalisation (e.g. frequency of engagement v. diversity of activities) and classification of activities as predominantly mental or social. Overall, future prospective research with sufficient follow-up should address reversed causality with regard to the activity engagement–dementia association, also in order to potentially exploit such directionality, for instance, for the purpose of an early dementia detection method.

Lastly, this study did not find significant interactions between cognitive activities, social memberships or social participation with other modifiable and non-modifiable dementia risk factors. Consequently, activity engagement might be equally beneficial for people of different ages, sex, socioeconomic strata or risk groups, which can be taken into account when designing and refining dementia prevention strategies. Contrary to our findings, a recent study using data from the Swedish National Study on Aging and Care – Kungsholmen found that engagement in an active lifestyle moderated the association between diabetes and dementia (Marseglia et al., Reference Marseglia, Darin-Mattsson, Kalpouzos, Grande, Fratiglioni, Dekhtyar and Xu2020). However, the authors used a composite measure of education, work complexity, leisure activities and social network to assess an ‘active life’, which makes a direct comparison with the current study on individual components less straightforward.

Strengths and limitations

This study has a number of strengths, including an extended period of follow-up in a large and representative sample of the British population and a high response rate, as well as a large number of modifiable dementia risk factors and plausible covariates included in the analyses. However, it also has some limitations. First, the sample was restricted to those without missing information on dementia incidence, LIBRAadj factors and education (28% missing data). This might have led to the selection of a generally healthier sample and thus to an underestimation of the true association between activity engagement and dementia. The loss of follow-up of some participants due to the longitudinal nature of this study could have further amplified that. It is noteworthy that missing values of LIBRAadj factors have been kept to a minimum by resorting to information of previous and subsequent waves. In addition, outcomes of the multiple imputation showed that results are stable compared to the primary analyses. Next, information on the activity variables was missing for some participants. Participants with missing information differed from the analytical sample on various key variables outlined above, which could have potentially induced bias. Moreover, the creation of composite scores for cognitive activity, social activity and social participation was done based on face validity and previous studies in ELSA and not on factor analysis. Due to the potential overlap, the individual contribution to explained dementia risk per activity domain might have been masked. In addition, dementia diagnoses, information on wealth, part of the LIBRAadj factors and engagement in activities were based on self or caregiver reports. This may be a source for response bias and exposure misclassification. Lastly, there may be other factors not considered, which may drive or confound the associations observed in this study, such as occupational complexity, parental SES, parental education or parental lifestyle. Despite the limitations, this study adds to the growing body of research demonstrating the potentially beneficial effects of engagement in cognitive and social leisure activities on dementia risk.

Conclusion

It was shown that cognitive and social activities were associated with dementia risk independent of various modifiable and non-modifiable risk factors. While activities partly overlap, both could equally play a role in programmes designed to decrease dementia risk. People with differing risk profiles may potentially benefit from a cognitively and socially stimulating lifestyle; therefore, targeted interventions should approach everybody equally. Future studies should preferably also further extend follow-up periods to minimise the risk for reversed causality.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S204579602100069X

Data

Data of the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing can be accessed freely via the UK data service (https://beta.ukdataservice.ac.uk/datacatalogue/series/series?id=200011). Upon reasonable request, data analysis protocols are available from the corresponding author.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the Adriana van Rinsum-Ponsen Stichting.

Conflict of interest

None.

Footnotes

The online version of this article has been updated since original publication. A notice detailing the changes has also been published at https://doi.org/10.1017/S2045796022000038

References

Almeida-Meza, P, Steptoe, A and Cadar, D (2021) Is engagement in intellectual and social leisure activities protective against dementia risk? Evidence from the English longitudinal study of ageing. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease 80, 555–565.Google ScholarPubMed
Anthony, M and Lin, F (2018) A systematic review for functional neuroimaging studies of cognitive reserve across the cognitive aging spectrum. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 33, 937948.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deckers, K, van Boxtel, MPJ, Schiepers, OJG, de Vugt, M, Sánchez, JLM, Anstey, KJ, Brayne, C, Dartigues, JF, Engedal, K, Kivipelto, M, Ritchie, K, Starr, JM, Yaffe, K, Irving, K, Verhey, FRJ and Köhler, S (2015) Target risk factors for dementia prevention: a systematic review and Delphi consensus study on the evidence from observational studies. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 30, 234246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deckers, K, Köhler, S, van Boxtel, M, Verhey, F, Brayne, C and Fleming, J (2018) Lack of associations between modifiable risk factors and dementia in the very old: findings from the Cambridge city over-75s cohort study. Aging and Mental Health 22, 12721278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deckers, K, Cadar, D, van Boxtel, MPJ, Verhey, FRJ, Steptoe, A and Köhler, S (2019 a) Modifiable risk factors explain socioeconomic inequalities in dementia risk: evidence from a population-based prospective cohort study. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease 71, 549557.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deckers, K, Nooyens, A, van Boxtel, M, Verhey, F, Verschuren, M and Köhler, S (2019 b) Gender and educational differences in the association between lifestyle and cognitive decline over 10 years: the Doetinchem cohort study. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease 70(s1), S31S41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deckers, K, Barbera, M, Köhler, S, Ngandu, T, van Boxtel, M, Rusanen, M, Laatikainen, T, Verhey, F, Soininen, H, Kivipelto, M and Solomon, A (2020) Long-term dementia risk prediction by the LIBRA score: a 30-year follow-up of the CAIDE study. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 35, 195203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eriksson Sörman, D, Sundström, A, Rönnlund, M, Adolfsson, R and Nilsson, LG (2014) Leisure activity in old age and risk of dementia: a 15-year prospective study. Journals of Gerontology – Series B Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 69, 493501.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fancourt, D, Steptoe, A and Cadar, D (2018) Cultural engagement and cognitive reserve: museum attendance and dementia incidence over a 10-year period. British Journal of Psychiatry 213, 661663.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fancourt, D, Steptoe, A and Cadar, D (2020) Community engagement and dementia risk: time-to-event analyses from a national cohort study. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health 74, 7177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Floud, S, Balkwill, A, Sweetland, S, Brown, A, Mauricio Reus, E, Hofman, A, Blacker, D, Kivimaki, M, Green, J, Peto, R, Reeves, GK and Beral, V (2021) Cognitive and social activities and long-term dementia risk: the prospective UK million women study. The Lancet Public Health 6, e116e123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foubert-Samier, A, Le Goff, M, Helmer, C, Pérès, K, Orgogozo, JM, Barberger-Gateau, P, Amieva, H and Dartigues, JF (2014) Change in leisure and social activities and risk of dementia in elderly cohort. Journal of Nutrition, Health and Aging 18, 876882.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fratiglioni, L, Paillard-Borg, S and Winblad, B (2004) An active and socially integrated lifestyle in late life might protect against dementia. Lancet Neurology 3, 343353.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fritsch, T, Smyth, KA, Debanne, SM, Petot, GJ and Friedland, RP (2005) Participation in novelty-seeking leisure activities and Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology 18, 134141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jorm, AF (1994) A short form of the informant questionnaire on cognitive decline in the elderly (IQCODE): development and cross-validation. Psychological Medicine 24, 145153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jorm, AF (2004) The informant questionnaire on cognitive decline in the elderly (IQCODE): a review. International Psychogeriatrics 16, 275293.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karp, A, Paillard-Borg, S, Wang, HX, Silverstein, M, Winblad, B and Fratiglioni, L (2006) Mental, physical and social components in leisure activities equally contribute to decrease dementia risk. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders 21, 6573.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuiper, JS, Zuidersma, M, Oude Voshaar, RC, Zuidema, SU, van den Heuvel, ER, Stolk, RP and Smidt, N (2015) Social relationships and risk of dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal cohort studies. Ageing Research Reviews 22, 3957.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindstrom, HA, Fritsch, T, Petot, G, Smyth, KA, Chen, CH, Debanne, SM, Lerner, AJ and Friedland, RP (2005) The relationships between television viewing in midlife and the development of Alzheimer's disease in a case-control study. Brain and Cognition 58, 157165.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Litke, R, Cancino Garcharna, L, Jiwani, S and Neugroschl, J (2021) Modifiable risk factors in Alzheimer disease and related dementias: a review. Clinical Therapeutics 43, 953965.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Livingston, G, Huntley, J, Sommerlad, A, Ames, D, Ballard, C, Banerjee, S, Brayne, C, Burns, A, Cohen-Mansfield, J, Cooper, C, Costafreda, SG, Dias, A, Fox, N, Gitlin, LN, Howard, R, Kales, HC, Kivimäki, M, Larson, EB, Ogunniyi, A, Orgeta, V, Ritchie, K, Rockwood, K, Sampson, EL, Samus, Q, Schneider, LS, Selbaek, G, Teri, L and Mukadam, N (2020) The lancet commissions dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2020 report of the Lancet Commission. The Lancet 396, 413–446.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marseglia, A, Darin-Mattsson, A, Kalpouzos, G, Grande, G, Fratiglioni, L, Dekhtyar, S and Xu, W (2020) Can active life mitigate the impact of diabetes on dementia and brain aging? Alzheimer's & Dementia: The Journal of the Alzheimer's Association 16, 15341543.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patterson, C (2018) World Alzheimer Report 2018. London, United Kingdom: Alzheimer's Disease International.Google Scholar
Petersson, SD and Philippou, E (2016) Mediterranean diet, cognitive function, and dementia: a systematic review of the evidence. Advances in Nutrition (Bethesda, MD.) 7, 889904.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Phelps, A, Marmot, M, Oskala, A, Clemens, S, Banks, J, Rogers, N, Steptoe, A, Blake, M, Nazroo, J and Oldfield, Z (2020) English Longitudinal Study of Ageing: Waves 0–9, 1998–2019, 31st Edn. Colchester, United Kingdom: UK Data Service. SN:5050, http://doi.org/10.5255/UKDA-SN-5050-18.Google Scholar
Quinn, TJ, Fearon, P, Noel-Storr, AH, Young, C, McShane, R and Stott, DJ (2014) Informant questionnaire on cognitive decline in the elderly (IQCODE) for the diagnosis of dementia within community dwelling populations. The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 12 (4), CD010079.Google Scholar
Rubin, DB (1996) Multiple imputation after 18+ years. Journal of the American Statistical Association 91, 473489.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scarmeas, N, Levy, G, Tang, MX, Manly, J and Stern, Y (2001) Influence of leisure activity on the incidence of Alzheimer's disease. Neurology 57, 22362242.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schiepers, OJG, Köhler, S, Deckers, K, Irving, K, O'Donnell, CA, van den Akker, M, Verhey, FRJ, Vos, SJB, de Vugt, ME and van Boxtel, MPJ (2018) Lifestyle for brain health (LIBRA): a new model for dementia prevention. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 33, 167175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schisterman, EF, Cole, SR and Platt, RW (2009) Overadjustment bias and unnecessary adjustment in epidemiologic studies. Epidemiology 20, 488495.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schoenfeld, D (1982) Partial residuals for the proportional hazards regression model. Biometrika 69, 239241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
StataCorp (2015) Stata Statistical Software: Release 14. College Station, TX: StataCorp LP.Google Scholar
Steptoe, A, Breeze, E, Banks, J and Nazroo, J (2013) Cohort profile: the English longitudinal study of ageing. International Journal of Epidemiology 42, 16401648.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stern, Y (2002) What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society: JINS 8, 448460.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stern, Y and Barulli, D (2009) Cognitive reserve. Handbook of Clinical Neurology 167, 181190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stern, C and Munn, Z (2010) Cognitive leisure activities and their role in preventing dementia: a systematic review. International Journal of Evidence-Based Healthcare 8, 217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stern, Y, Arenaza-Urquijo, EM, Bartrés-Faz, D, Belleville, S, Cantilon, M, Chetelat, G, Ewers, M, Franzmeier, N, Kempermann, G, Kremen, WS, Okonkwo, O, Scarmeas, N, Soldan, A, Udeh-Momoh, C, Valenzuela, M, Vemuri, P, Vuoksimaa, E and the Reserve, Resilience and Protective Factors PIA Empirical Definitions and Conceptual Frameworks Workgroup (2020) Whitepaper: defining and investigating cognitive reserve, brain reserve, and brain maintenance. Alzheimer's Dementia 16, 13051311.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steyaert, J, Deckers, K, Smits, C, Fox, C, Thyrian, R, Jeon, YH, Vernooij-Dassen, M and Köhler, S (2020) Putting primary prevention of dementia on everybody's agenda. Aging & Mental Health 25, 13761380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomassen, JQ, Schurmann Tolstrup, J, Benn, M and Frikke-Schmidt, R (2020) Type-2 diabetes and risk of dementia: observational and Mendelian randomisation studies in 1 million individuals. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences 29, e118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Buuren, S and Groothuis-Oudshoorn, CGM (2000) Multivariate Imputation by Chained Equations: MICE V1.0 User's Manual. Leiden, The Netherlands: TNO Prevention and Health.Google Scholar
Vos, SJB, van Boxtel, MPJ, Schiepers, OJG, Deckers, K, de Vugt, M, Carrière, I, Dartigues, JF, Peres, K, Artero, S, Ritchie, K, Galluzzo, L, Scafato, E, Frisoni, GB, Huisman, M, Comijs, HC, Sacuiu, SF, Skoog, I, Irving, K, O'Donnell, CA, Verhey, FRJ, Visser, PJ and Köhler, S (2017) Modifiable risk factors for prevention of dementia in midlife, late life and the oldest-old: validation of the LIBRA index. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease 58, 537547.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, HX, Karp, A, Winblad, B and Fratiglioni, L (2002) Late-life engagement in social and leisure activities is associated with a decreased risk of dementia: a longitudinal study from the Kungsholmen Project. American Journal of Epidemiology 155, 10811087.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
White, IR, Royston, P and Wood, AM (2011) Multiple imputation using chained equations: issues and guidance for practice. Statistics in Medicine 30, 377399.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wium-Andersen, IK, Rungby, J, Jørgensen, MB, Sandbæk, A, Osler, M and Wium-Andersen, MK (2019) Risk of dementia and cognitive dysfunction in individuals with diabetes or elevated blood glucose. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences 29, e43Google ScholarPubMed
World Health Organization (2018) Towards a Dementia Plan: A WHO Guide. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Baseline characteristics of ELSA Wave 4 participants by incident dementia status

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Cumulative hazard estimates of incident dementia by baseline level of cognitive activities (low v. high) adjusted for age, sex, level of education, wealth and LIBRAadj scores.

Figure 2

Table 2. Stepwise Cox proportional hazard regression for the individual contribution of cognitive activity, social memberships and social participation

Figure 3

Fig. 2. Cumulative hazard estimates of incident dementia by baseline level of social memberships (low v. high), adjusted for age, sex, level of education, wealth and LIBRAadj scores.

Figure 4

Fig. 3. Cumulative hazard estimates of incident dementia by baseline level of social participation (low v. high), adjusted for age, sex, level of education, wealth and LIBRAadj scores.

Figure 5

Table 3. Interaction between levels of cognitive activity, social memberships, social participation and other dementia risk factors

Supplementary material: File

Duffner et al. supplementary material

Duffner et al. supplementary material

Download Duffner et al. supplementary material(File)
File 20.9 KB