Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jn8rn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-18T19:43:05.399Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A social science mixed-methods approach to stimulating and measuring creativity in the design classroom

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 December 2023

Nilanjan Raghunath*
Affiliation:
Assistant Professor, Singapore University of Technology and Design, Singapore
Georgios Koronis
Affiliation:
Assistant Professor, University of the Aegean, Syros, Greece
Rianne Karthikayen
Affiliation:
Senior Researcher, Civil Service College, Institute of Governance and Policy, Singapore
Arlindo Silva
Affiliation:
Associate Professor, Singapore University of Technology and Design, Singapore
Christine Yogiaman
Affiliation:
Assistant Professor, Singapore University of Technology and Design, Singapore
*
Corresponding author Nilanjan Raghunath [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

This study aimed to evaluate the effectiveness of using a social science mixed-methods approach to stimulate creativity and improve the attainment of creative outcomes in teaching design. In a focused study framed through a design collaborative experiment within a classroom context, sketches produced by a sample of 351 students were analyzed and the impact of stimuli was categorized by visual, physical, quantitative and contextual information on creative processes and outcomes in product design. Sixteen combinations of these stimuli were integrated as parameters of design briefs (DBs) given to the participating students. This research was augmented with a survey to understand participants’ perceptions and reactions and was rated by expert judges. The results demonstrate that certain combinations of quantitative and qualitative stimuli have a positive impact on creative processes and outcomes. These findings will inform new techniques for engaging and inspiring students in design studies.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - ND
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided that no alterations are made and the original article is properly cited. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained prior to any commercial use and/or adaptation of the article.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

1. Introduction

Defining and exploring “creativity” is a complex and daunting task. It manifests differently across disciplines: as a process, a personal trait or a product. Creativity is not confined to a single field or practice. It is complex in its multilayered manifestation affecting individual performance, groups and product outcomes. There is thus a great incentive to innovate and use mixed methods to stimulate or simulate and measure the creative process. In social science disciplines, mixed methods are defined by studies mixing various qualitative and quantitative research techniques (Johnson, Onwuegbuzie, & Turner Reference Johnson, Onwuegbuzie and Turner2007; Onwuegbuzie & Collins Reference Onwuegbuzie and Collins2007). To explore the range of potential methods to motivate creativity, we studied the effects of various stimuli on participatory design among students. This exploration was specifically anchored in observing the effects of selected control stimuli on creativity, assessed through a social science mixed-methods approach including quantitative and qualitative measures of creativity. These stimuli were delivered in the form of design briefs (DBs) within a participatory design session. We tested the impact on creative ideation by varying the stimuli in DBs, gathered participant perceptions and ascertained product outcomes in the form of creativity scores assigned by expert judges. The participants of this controlled study were all students undergoing an introductory course in design. The classroom context is a thematically fitting space for the study, as it embodies the study’s significance.

The aims of this study were to test stimuli that potentially impact creative processes and outcomes and assess the effectiveness of mixed approaches in measuring and stimulating creativity using the framework of the “design brief,” within the context of design education. The results of the study are discussed, and the implications for design education are detailed. From this research, knowledge of a cohesive approach tackling creativity as a tangible concept is shared involving social science methods.

2. Theoretical background

There is plenty of literature, which proposes the requirement of creativity to foster innovation in educational and professional settings (Amabile Reference Amabile1996; Csikszentmihalyi Reference Csikszentmihalyi1996; Craft, Jeffrey, & Leibling Reference Craft, Jeffrey, Leibling, Craft, Jeffrey and Leibling2001; McIntyre Reference McIntyre2011). Research shows that definitions of creativity have evolved in three distinct phases: notably the first as a personality construct, the second as a mental process and the latest as a sociocultural system (Sawyer Reference Sawyer2011). Runco & Jaeger (Reference Runco and Jaeger2012) trace the evolution of definitions of creativity, with some conceptualizations of creativity dating as far back as 1839. Within Ma’s (Reference Ma2009) investigation of the creative components of problem definition and solution generation, he defines numerous aspects of creativity: as the creative side of an individual (person), as a problem-solving tool (process) and as an outcome (product). According to Sternberg & Lubart (Reference Sternberg, Lubart and Sternberg1999), creativity is measured via the creation of work that is novel and appropriate.

A creative product is defined as “anything that produces ‘effective surprise’ in the observer, in addition to a ‘shock of recognition’ that the product of response, although novel, is entirely appropriate” (Amabile Reference Amabile1983, pp. 358–359; Bruner Reference Bruner, Gruber, Terrell and Wertheimer1962). Similarly, other studies discern that creativity can be devised in novelty and usability (Sarkar & Chakrabarti Reference Sarkar and Chakrabarti2007). Dean et al. (Reference Dean, Hender, Rodgers and Santanen2006) examined 90 empirical studies on creativity and narrowed down the definition of “creativity” to four main constructs or dimensions: novelty, workability, relevance and thoroughness. In their research, Runco & Jaeger (Reference Runco and Jaeger2012) state that there is a wide-ranging consensus that originality and usefulness are essential components of creativity. From an engineering perspective, creativity entails producing original and possibly practicable ideas to solve a problem (Bourgeois-Bougrine et al. Reference Bourgeois-Bougrine, Buisine, Vandendriessche, Glaveanu and Lubart2017). While there are many differing perspectives on defining the nebulous concept of creativity, it is evident that creativity is a process and a multifaceted phenomenon, which is not focused on just one product.

Ma’s research primarily focuses on creativity within the design domain (2019). His work identifies five stages involving an exploration of the creative process within the realm of problem-solving, offering a structured framework to effectively approach challenges and generate innovative solutions. The first is looking at problem definition; the second, seeking retrieval of problem-related knowledge; the third, looking at solution generation; the fourth, identifying criteria for solution evaluation; and the final, selecting a solution for problem resolution and implementing it (Figure 1). In the context of this analysis, the understanding of the creative process undergoes a significant transformation. It is reconceptualized as a “reorganization of knowledge,” a concept articulated by Ma (Reference Ma2009, p. 31).

Figure 1. Ma’s stages of the problem-solving process.

The interest in such creativity-driven processes in design, especially within educational institutions, is amplified due to the challenges designers face during idea generation. These challenges, highlighted by Genco, Hölttä‐Otto, & Seepersad (Reference Genco, Hölttä‐Otto and Seepersad2012), include dealing with constraints that intermittently restrict their creative process. To address these challenges, research has delved into the use of design heuristics in concept generation, particularly among mechanical engineers and industrial designers. The results, as demonstrated by Yilmaz et al. (Reference Yilmaz, Daly, Seifert and Gonzalez2015), highlight that design experts employ heuristics, showcasing their positive impact on fostering creativity within distinct design disciplines. Moreover, the pivotal role of collaborative interactions among designers in group design settings has gained prominence (Sauder & Jin Reference Sauder and Jin2016). These interactions have been shown to be instrumental in enhancing creativity-related thinking processes. By sharing design elements and posing questions, creativity is stimulated, underscoring the influence of collaboration on the creative endeavors of designers.

In seeking solutions, some categorize the inherently social and collaborative aspects of creativity research (Littleton, Rojas-Drummond, & Miell Reference Littleton, Rojas-Drummond and Miell2008). One such manifestation of this is the practice of brainstorming. While brainstorming usually demands that people openly talk about novel solutions to a problem, this method is ineffective for developing design solutions (White, Wood, & Jensen Reference White, Wood and Jensen2012). Classic brainstorming has been shown to lack the desired collaborative effect, as groups do not produce better solutions in the brainstorming environment than individuals working alone (Mullen, Johnson, & Salas Reference Mullen, Johnson and Salas1991). One resolution of this issue is the utilization of stimuli to remove mental blocks while facilitating the creation of new ideas (Howard, Dekoninck, & Culley Reference Howard, Dekoninck and Culley2010).

Much research has gone into understanding the dearth of research on external and intrapersonal factors on observable creativity in the field of social psychology (Amabile Reference Amabile1996; Amabile and Pillemer Reference Amabile and Pillemer2012; Hennessey Reference Hennessey2003). Means of quantifying and measuring creativity have been formulated as a result. One such “gold standard” of creativity assessment is the Consensual Assessment Technique (CAT) (Carson Reference Carson2006), though it is limited to research settings (Baer & McKool Reference Baer, McKool and Schreiner2009). Overall, all this has advanced the understanding of creativity, as well as causes and methods of stimulation or obstacles for creative outcomes such as competition, diversity of partners and technological collaboration (Nieto & Santamaría Reference Nieto and Santamaría2007).

During the initial stages of the design process, often considered the most creative phase, the utilization of diverse stimuli can be exceptionally advantageous (Liikkanen & Perttula Reference Liikkanen and Perttula2008). Indeed, the approach adopted by student designers when addressing design challenges and generating creative solutions may vary based on how tasks are exemplified, the structure of DBs and exposure to informational stimuli (Sarkar & Chakrabarti Reference Sarkar and Chakrabarti2008). Furthermore, it is worth noting that the formulation and delivery of DBs, as well as the combination of relevant information to address specific design tasks, wield significant influence over the creative outcomes of design solutions (Koronis, Casakin, & Silva Reference Koronis, Casakin and Silva2021; Koronis et al. Reference Koronis, Silva, Kang Kai Siang and Yogiaman2022). In this context, the incorporation of analogical sources within DBs, as a means of imparting inspiring information, emerges as a promising avenue for enhancing creative design outcomes (Casakin, Koronis, & Silva Reference Casakin, Koronis and Silva2022; Koronis, Casakin, & Silva Reference Koronis, Casakin and Silva2023). Furthermore, it is evident that collaborative elements of creativity are important and can yield innovative outcomes (see Koronis et al. Reference Koronis, Chia, Kang, Silva, Yogiaman and Raghunath2019).

Similarly, another example of one such significant factor is the environment. A classroom climate is considered by some as a favorable environment for creativity and stimulation, especially if it presents high competition with low friction (Ma Reference Ma2009). The meta-analysis found that creativity scores were higher when pupils were allowed to manipulate materials, engage in discussions, participate in self-evaluation and learn via self-initiative (Lopez, Esquivel, & Houtz Reference Lopez, Esquivel and Houtz1993; Ma Reference Ma2009). Existing conventions and formats applied in structuring information should be evaluated, such as “DBs” within design contexts.

In the context of education, educators who aim to nurture students’ creativity are particularly interested in crafting DBs that encourage creative project outcomes across various undergraduate levels, as highlighted by Sadowska & Laffy (Reference Sadowska and Laffy2017) and Raghunath, Koronis, & Silva (Reference Raghunath, Koronis and Silva2020). Therefore, the investigation of various conditions in which the DB and external stimuli, such as physical objects, may influence the creativity of students’ design outputs within a studio environment becomes a key area of focus.

When employing DBs to provide this essential information, several critical factors should be considered like the level of detail included in written instructions and the potential inclusion of visual prompts. Striking the right balance between clarity and ambiguity becomes paramount to achieving the desired outcomes. While some level of ambiguity can foster innovation and creativity, it is essential to avoid an excessive lack of clarity that might lead to confusion and frustration among students, as cautioned by Sawyer (Reference Sawyer2017). Ultimately, such frustration can hinder the generation of truly creative and impactful results.

2.1. The design brief

In a design project, DBs are typically presented at the beginning of the process (Camburn et al. Reference Camburn, Auernhammer, Sng, Mignone, Arlitt, Perez and Wood2017) to set expectations and the outcome, thereby setting the pitch for the project and guiding its process (Koronis et al. Reference Koronis, Chia, Kang, Silva, Yogiaman and Raghunath2019). The DB is an essential agreement between the creator and the receiver – often a business or client – in accordance with the scope and purpose of the project, as well as the parameters of the product that is to be created. A DB is also known as an innovation brief, a creative brief, a job ticket, a marketing brief or a project brief, in other disciplines (Phillips Reference Phillips2004). A well-written DB, says Phillips (Reference Phillips2004), is a road map of sorts, or a contractual agreement between interested parties, and may be seen as a business plan, as well as a project tracking tool, attached with various guidelines, specifications or parameters.

Across different fields, research has found that DBs help to focus conceptual thinking (Fleer Reference Fleer2000), spur on questioning (Fleer Reference Fleer2000) and can be a medium of public engagement (Payne et al. Reference Payne, Mackrill, Cain, Strelitz and Gate2015). This results in an emotional relationship being created between the designer and the end user (McDonagh-Philp & Lebbon Reference McDonagh-Philp and Lebbon2000). Much research has explored the use of DBs as important to the design process (Phillips Reference Phillips2004; Lawson Reference Lawson2006; Cross Reference Cross and Michel2007; Goldschmidt & Rodgers Reference Goldschmidt and Rodgers2013; Haug Reference Haug2015). Prior research from Dorst & Cross (Reference Dorst and Cross2001) found that stimulating the DB by providing varying information may spur creative concepts. Some studies revealed specific preferences, such as designers preferring visual representations as stimuli when pursuing inspiration (Vasconcelos et al. Reference Vasconcelos, Cardoso, Sääksjärvi, Chen and Crilly2017).

Visual stimuli attached to the DB, including artwork, industrial artifacts and natural elements, frequently serve as sources of inspiration by sparking the generation of ideas. Consequently, these visual representations can expand the range of known solutions (Gonçalves, Cardoso, & Badke-Schaub Reference Gonçalves, Cardoso and Badke-Schaub2016) and expedite the creative problem-solving process, leading to time and effort savings (Cai, Yi-Luen Do, & Zimring Reference Cai, Yi-Luen Do and Zimring2010).

3. Aims and contributions

In the pedagogy of design education, helping students ascertain and develop ideas that will generate creative solutions is an area of importance for tertiary institutions, particularly in design disciplines. Creativity in the form of designed outcomes can be stimulated when designers are exposed to stimuli attached to a DB.

We believe that a participatory and collaborative design process can stimulate new creative design solutions. This proposition stems from the notion that creativity can be cultivated and that effectively achieving design solutions can be accomplished by thoughtfully incorporating a diverse range of stimuli into DBs. We thus empirically manipulated the structure of the DB. This manipulation involved providing student designers with stimuli in the form of visual and textual displays, along with detailed written instructions for executing the design task. As such, the first hypothesis in our study was as follows:

H1: By introducing various stimuli in design briefs, we anticipate observing differences in creativity levels among students who receive different and distinct sets of instructions. Consequently, our experiment aims to establish statistically significant differences when comparing groups of students who received different design briefs.

The second area of focus was whether the DB would effectively stimulate students. Prior literature suggests that the briefing process is important to the design outcomes (Nutt Reference Nutt1993); however, the exact measures of what constitutes a creativity-stimulating DB are insufficiently covered in the existing literature. As such, in this study, we intended to gain a comprehensive understanding of how participants perceived DBs and triangulate this with how students performed with the briefs they received. Consequently, the second set of hypotheses was as follows:

H2a: Students receiving only the baseline design brief will find it unclear (too vague) to help them conceptualize product ideas.

H2b: Students receiving the design brief with one or more stimuli or parameters will find it more suitable to help them conceptualize product ideas.

A comprehensive approach to assessing creativity is necessary (Feldhusen & Goh Reference Feldhusen and Goh1995; Cropley Reference Cropley2000; Plucker & Makel Reference Plucker, Makel, Kaufman and Sternberg2010; Park, Chun, & Lee Reference Park, Chun and Lee2016). A study (Park et al. Reference Park, Chun and Lee2016) discussed the merits of using a triangulation of subjective and objective assessment methods for creativity research. They define subjective evaluation methods as qualitative, which involve analyzing data from direct fieldwork observations, surveys, interviews and written documents. Furthermore, they define objective evaluation methods as comprising quantitative analysis that concerns systematic empirical research through quantitative approaches. Triangulation has been generally defined as a permutation of methodologies to study the same or similar phenomenon (Denzin Reference Denzin1978; Jick Reference Jick1979). The proposition of the above studies inspired our study – as we aimed to employ a nuanced amalgamation of creativity assessment metrics – to achieve a triangulated understanding of creativity stimulation and measurement.

4. Methods

Social science mixed methods with qualitative and quantitative measures were used to stimulate and measure creativity, as exhibited in Figure 2. We adopted a concurrent triangulated approach to answering a research question (Creswell & Clark Reference Creswell and Clark2018). In a similar vein, we collected and analyzed various sets of data and then merged all the results into an overall interpretation. The purpose of this study was to try and ascertain the DB and the most efficient stimulus or parameters that interact most effectively to create the highest creativity scores.

Figure 2. An overview of the model developed for the stimulation and measurement of creativity.

Within this context, we purposefully integrated a modified 6-3-5/Collaborative Sketch (C-Sketch) methodology into our research. It is worth clarifying that the use of the modified collaborative method was not intended as a manipulable experimental variable. Instead, it functioned as the conduit through which we captured the design output generated by each team, thereby serving as our designated data collection approach.

In the first component of our study, students were shown the DB and stimuli and explained what was expected of them. The second component of our study was the combined 6-3-5/C-Sketch techniques that were aimed at encouraging concept generation (see Otto & Wood Reference Otto and Wood2001). Students participating in the study were introduced to the C-Sketch ideation method as it is effective in the idea generation process, especially in providing feedback on proposed ideas (Shah et al. Reference Shah, Vargas-Hernandez, Summers and Kulkarni2001). The 6-3-5 brainwriting method is frequently employed and has been praised for reducing individual dominance in idea generation (King & Sivaloganathan Reference King and Sivaloganathan1999). Developed by Bernd Rohrbach, the 6-3-5 method is a brainstorming method, using written words (Rohrbach Reference Rohrbach1969), while the C-Sketch (Shah Reference Shah1993; Shah et al. Reference Shah, Vargas-Hernandez, Summers and Kulkarni2001) uses sketching as the primary form of communication. In trying to stimulate creativity without verbal aids, C-Sketch was also a useful tool to teach our students to generate ideas using drawings as the main procedure of communication, with additions by peers serving as nonverbal modes of idea generation or refinement (see Shah et al. Reference Shah, Vargas-Hernandez, Summers and Kulkarni2001). This combined 6-3-5/C-Sketch methodology does not provide contextual information such as participant perception or familiarity. To address this limitation, we paired the 6-3-5/C-Sketch method with a post-sketch survey. This information helped us contextualize the sketches and gather insight into the DB perceptions.

The sketches were presented to the judges who marked them based on a rubric, which aimed to score creativity on the aforementioned measures: novelty, appropriateness and usability, which were decided to be critical factors for measuring creativity.

Furthermore, the assessment was conducted with the use of the aforementioned CAT, by Amabile (Reference Amabile1996), which measures creativity via constructs of novelty, appropriateness and usability. CAT was used as it is a tool used in multiple studies for assessing creativity based on the evaluations of professionals and academic experts (Amabile Reference Amabile1979; Stubbs & Amabile Reference Stubbs and Amabile1979; Berglas, Amabile, & Handel Reference Berglas, Amabile and Handel1981; Stubbs Reference Stubbs1981; Amabile Reference Amabile1982; Hennessey Reference Hennessey1982; Amabile, Goldfarb, & Brackfield Reference Amabile, Goldfarb and Brackfield1990) and is also validated by Cheng (Reference Cheng, Wegerif, Li and Kaufman2015). This technique is also validated in various other contexts and disciplines, and it was developed initially for researching artistic creativity in children (Amabile Reference Amabile1996), in pedagogical studies (Baer Reference Baer1993; Baer Reference Baer2003), in gender and ethnicity studies (Kaufman, Baer, & Gentile Reference Kaufman, Baer and Gentile2004) and in comparing domain-specific and domain-general models of creativity (Hennessey Reference Hennessey1994). CAT is not connected to any specific theory of creativity, and its validity does not change with any construct of creativity (Baer & McKool Reference Baer, McKool and Schreiner2009).

We also used the metrics of novelty and appropriateness as described by Kampylis & Valtanen (Reference Kampylis and Valtanen2010) with the addition of usability. We postulate that the incorporation of usability overlaps with workability (“An idea is workable/feasible if it can be easily implemented and does not violate known constraints”) and relevance in the way (Dean et al. Reference Dean, Hender, Rodgers and Santanen2006, p. 650) described it. An idea is relevant if it “applies to the stated problem and will be effective at solving the problem” (Dean et al. Reference Dean, Hender, Rodgers and Santanen2006, p. 663). Usability was thus included as a third metric, defined in the international standard, ISO 9241-11, as “the extent to which specified users can use a product to achieve specified goals with effectiveness, efficiency and satisfaction in a specified context of use” (ISO 2018).

4.1. Participants

Students in this experiment followed an “introduction to design” course, mandatory for all freshmen (first-year undergraduate students). The course introduces essential skills and mindset of innovation, entrepreneurship and design methodologies, including teamwork and workflow organization and various computational techniques. Participants ranged between 18 and 21 years old and were in classes representing a mix of genders, nationalities, educational backgrounds and academic scores. Each group of five to six people was formed freely, and most participants remained in groups they had formed earlier in the semester. A total of 351 students participated in this study, producing a total of 1,003 sketches. Overall, 74 workgroups were created. The participants were told that the completed sketches would not impact their academic standing or performance in any manner.

4.2. Experimental procedure

A full-factorial design was employed via the design of experiments (DOEs) described by Jiju (Reference Jiju2003) to lay out the varying conditions. DBs were presented to the students either in succinct form or along with one of four stimuli (quantitative, visual, contextual or physical) to re-design an orange squeezer. In our experimental context, “stimulus” pertains to the input parameters contained within the DB. All participants received design instructions, which covered the basic principles of the project.

The baseline (BL) DB requested students to “design a device to extract orange juice from fresh oranges at home” and detailed that it should be low cost, easy to manufacture, machine washable and have a small footprint. The only other information given in the BL brief was not to use a blender or blender-type machine as a base for the design. No quantifiable requirements were given in the BL, such as the exact cost or the size. Detailed examples of each DB can be found in Supplementary Appendix A for reference.

The first stimulus was quantitative requirements (Q) (quantitative here refers to the type of information in the DB), which provided participants with specifications for the product. These included a target cost of $20 (which the research team decided was reasonable), a limit of less than two manufacturing processes, machine washability, whereby no damage should be seen after 100 washes, and its volume quantified by 15 × 15 × 15 cm. The second stimulus was a visual example (V), which was delivered in the form of a video demonstrating the use of an existing orange squeezer. The third stimulus was a physical example (P), whereby participants received an actual orange squeezer to interact with during the ideation session. Orange squeezers were picked as they are commonly used in product design classes (see Koronis et al. Reference Koronis, Chia, Kang, Silva, Yogiaman and Raghunath2019). The last stimulus provided was contextual (C), which presented the participants with details regarding the pain points users had with existing orange squeezers and reasons for using an orange squeezer in the setting. Sixteen cohorts were created for the varied DBs, including one cohort that received only the BL brief. The remaining 15 cohorts were given DBs paired with one or several stimuli, as illustrated in Figure 3. Groups exposed to different stimuli were each conducted in separate classrooms, and the experiment was carried out in distinct sessions.

Figure 3. Cohort (Co.) breakdown by design brief and stimuli.

4.3. The 6-3-5/C-sketch method

As described earlier, our research paired the 6-3-5 with the C-Sketch method, such that the format of 6-3-5 remained. Our experiment is in line with the study of Gonçalves et al. (Reference Gonçalves, Cardoso and Badke-Schaub2016), but with the use of sketching as the primary form of communication, following previous research work by Linsey et al. (Reference Linsey, Green, Murphy, Wood and Markman2005) where ideas are exemplified in the form of sketches and text.

Accordingly, groups were tasked with creating three sketches. Students were asked to ponder about this problem for a few minutes, as per the retrieval of problem-related knowledge. Next, students embarked on the sketch exercise, which sought to generate solutions. Students then pass their C-Sketches around the table for their peers to respond and improve, thus evaluating the solution. The first round takes 15 minutes, and participants then pass their sketches to their adjacent team members for 10 minutes before passing them on again, all the way around the group until they return to the original creator in a total of five rounds.

Each group member uses an assigned color pen, and when the creator receives their sketch back at the end of the exercise, they can assess what was added to their sketch and in what order. The notion is that teammates may draw inspiration from one another and add creatively, in esthetics or functions, to their peers’ sketches (Goldschmidt Reference Goldschmidt1991; Gu et al. Reference Gu, Chen, Huang, Liu and Huang2018).

Our study applied this method so that students were first exposed to the DB allocated to their class and then asked to participate in the 6-3-5/C-Sketch method. They were tasked with creating three sketches before passing them clockwise through the rest of their group until they received their original sketch back. Group sizes averaged five people; the 6-3-5/C-Sketch method is illustrated in Figure 4.

Figure 4. Illustration of the 6-3-5/C-sketch.

4.4. Judges’ rating

The assessment was conducted by expert judges who were not told the study’s hypotheses and experimental conditions, inspecting creativity as a product based on the sketches submitted by students. Judges were selected based on their level of experience and background. The judging panel included faculty members and researchers specializing in either engineering product development or architecture and having at least 10 years of proficiency in architecture, design education and product development. Thus, they qualified to be part of an expert panel of judges (Kaufman & Kaufman Reference Kaufman and Kaufman2007).

The sketches were evaluated in accordance with Amabile’s (Reference Amabile1996) well-established CAT (see Supplementary Appendix B) to quantitatively assess and derive creativity scores for each sketch. Sketches were all scanned and anonymized before being circulated to respective judges. The sketches were grouped by cohort to ensure grading for each cohort aligned with the specific DB received. Judges were instructed to go through the sketches and assign a numerical criterion ranging from 1 to 5 for each of the three components (namely novelty, appropriateness and usability).

The novelty was measured in consideration of how the design was different (or unfamiliar) from the usual way of extracting the juice and quantified along a Likert scale where 1 indicated the sketch to be entirely like an existing method and 5 indicated a real surprise. Appropriateness was measured in consideration of the extent to which the design aligned to the brief’s requirements, where 1 indicated the sketch not to be aligned and 5 indicated complete alignment. Lastly, usability was measured as the ability of the design to extract efficiently and effectively the most amount of juice with nominal effort and discomfort, where 1 denoted a poor fit and 5 denoted an excellent fit.

Three judges were selected for the assessment of the sketches to ensure inter-rater reliability. These judges independently assessed the sketches and graded each sketch for novelty, appropriateness and usability. The scores were checked for inter-rater reliability. Any set of scores with a standard deviation that indicated the presence of an outlier was carefully examined for errors in data entry or re-evaluated by all judges to achieve consensus.

While assessing sketches, it is noteworthy that a few instances revealed a notable standard deviation among the scores provided by the judges. However, it is imperative to emphasize that the overarching reliability values remained within the “good-to-fair” spectrum. Despite affording judges an ample amount of time for the meticulous evaluation of the design outcomes, it is plausible that certain variations in scoring arose due to factors such as fatigue or the potential for human error. Indeed, the task of evaluating numerous drawings while considering multiple metrics and design requisites can, at times, introduce inherent challenges that may affect the precision of judgment.

4.5. Post-sketch survey

A post-sketch survey was administered (see Supplementary Appendix C) to ascertain participants’ genders, perceptions of their received DBs and familiarity with orange squeezers. The survey was administered to all participants after they completed the 6-3-5/C-Sketch method to gather feedback on the different briefs received across various cohorts. The questions provided had optional answers tied to scales ranging from 1 to 3 (Supplementary Appendix C). Only those who completed the post-survey were included in the analysis, resulting in a sample of 278. Only those who willingly completed the survey, allowing their scores to be matched with the design outcomes, were included in the analysis. Participants who chose not to participate or left survey items incomplete were not considered in the analysis.

To generate a consolidated score, we computed the average of the individual contributions made by each student. This resulted in each student’s dataset being consolidated to yield a mean score for their sketches, as opposed to the prior practice of having three separate scores per creativity metric, corresponding to each of the three sketches. As a robustness check, we compared the statistical analysis of the sketches (N = 278) to the statistical results of the individual contributions (N = 1003). We found that the results were very comparable. With this aggregation, though, the data are no longer normally distributed (p < 0.05 for the Shapiro–Wilk and Kolmogorov–Smirnov tests). However, the variances were homogeneous (p > 0.24 for Levene’s test); as such, the one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was considered still proper for the analysis.

5. Results

5.1. Statistical analysis of creative outcomes

IBM Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) version 25 was employed to calculate the intra-class correlation coefficient (ICC) estimates based on an average-measures, absolute-agreement, two-way mixed-effects model. The inter-rater reliability – a measurement of consistency between multiple raters – determines the agreement’s level across all judges (Heale & Twycross Reference Heale and Twycross2015). The ICC estimates fared in the good-to-fair range, with novelty scores of ICC = 0.66, appropriateness scores of ICC = 0.56 and usability scores of ICC = 0.55. The mean score of the three judges’ scores was taken for each metric to ensure that a single score would be attributed to these individual metrics of creativity.

According to Shapiro–Wilk’s and Levene’s tests, distributions of creativity scores were not normal or homogeneous. Thus, the Kruskal–Wallis test, the non-parametric equivalent to the standard ANOVA test, was employed for statistical analysis. Statistical analysis was conducted at the sketch level, treating each design solution as an individual and distinct observation. The test revealed statistically significant differences across conditions. The mean was calculated for each sketch from the three judges’ scores. The mean was extrapolated for each group within each DB type (see Table 1).

Table 1. Mean scores and standard deviation (SD) per brief

Note: “Groups” signifies the group of students per condition. “Students” counts the individual participants per condition. “Sketches” shows the number of design sketches produced per condition.

In examining the differences between various briefs using the Kruskal–Wallis test, we observed a significant impact on novelty scores related to the various stimuli, H (15, n = 1003) = 51.76, p < 0.05. The highest-performing group for novelty was the quantitative group (Brief Q, μ = 2.99, SD = 0.8). Conversely, the worst-performing group for novelty was exposed to the paired quantitative and physical stimuli (Brief QP, μ = 2.47, SD = 0.8).

The magnitude of the adjusted statistical significance of the average scores indicated that appropriateness scores were also significantly affected by providing different kinds of stimuli, H (15, n = 1003) = 59.38, p < 0.05. For appropriateness scores, the quantitative–visual group achieved the highest performance (Brief QV, μ = 3.17, SD = 0.66), whereas the worst-performing group was exposed to no stimuli (Brief BL, μ = 2.57, SD = 0.8).

Finally, the Kruskal–Wallis test revealed strong evidence of different conditions in usability scores, H (15, n = 1003) = 73.34, p < 0. According to mean scores, participants exposed to physical and contextual (PC) stimuli marked the highest scores in this metric (Brief PC, μ = 3.17 SD = 0.66). Lastly, the worst-performing group was tied between those exposed to the physical (Brief P, μ = 2.63, SD = 0.6) and those exposed to the quantitative (Brief Q, μ = 2.65, SD = 0.8).

These findings combined suggest the confirmation of hypothesis (H1). They indicate that different briefs do indeed stimulate differing dimensions of creativity encompassing novelty, usability and appropriateness in the context of the design outcomes.

5.2. Post-experimental survey statistics analysis

A total of 278 participants completed the post-sketch survey, and 36% of participants were female. The descriptive data can be found in Supplementary Appendix D. The results showed that 68% of participants who answered regarding the adequacy of the DB (DB1) felt that the DB was just right (compared with 12% who found it too specific, and 20% who found it too vague). Interestingly, in survey item DB2, which focused on the influence of the DB on their initial ideas, 33% of participants across all cohorts who received different DBs said that the DB did not influence their initial ideas. In contrast, 24% indicated that the DB helped them broaden their initial ideas, and 43% found it helpful to narrow down their initial ideas.

A one-way within-group ANOVA was conducted to assess the experimental condition’s impact on the perception of the DB. The ANOVA findings are displayed in Supplementary Appendix E. There was a statistically significant difference at the p < 0.05 level in DB perception (DB1) for the experimental conditions: F (15,262) = 1.93, p = 0.02. The effect size, calculated using eta squared, was 0.09. No significant correlations existed between DB influence and DB2 and the creativity metrics (novelty, appropriateness and usability). A summary of significant findings is displayed in Table 2.

Table 2. Key findings from mean scores and statistical analysis

Survey results revealed that those in the PC condition felt most that the DB was just right. In particular, among the 38 students who took the survey within this condition, 23 of them (61%) expressed this sentiment. However, it is worth noting that this perception did not necessarily lead to highly novel, appropriate or usable ideas. Those in the quantitative, visual and physical (QVP) condition scored the lowest mean, thus inferring that their brief was seen as too specific (by six students) but leaning slightly toward just right (10 students). The quantitative and physical (QP) condition had the most centered mean score, inferring that students with this condition were more prone to perceiving the DB as being just right.

Within the conditions, those in the PC (QPC) scored the highest mean in DB1 (seven participants), thus inferring that they found their DB the most vague out of all the groups. This result showed that having a higher level of detailed information assisted all brief variations expected from the BL brief.

We hypothesized that those in the BL group would be more likely to find the DB too vague (H2a). However, 60% of the BL group participants (13 out of 21) found it to be just right, 15% found it too specific and 25% found it too vague, which disproves H2a.

Our survey shows that briefs were generally perceived as just right by most participants, but some degree of ambiguity is desirable to foster innovation and creativity. Instructors should create the necessary environment that not only encourages this but also ensures that students do not become overly frustrated or confused in the process (Sawyer Reference Sawyer2012, Reference Sawyer2017). On an added note, perhaps, additional information may fixate the designer; thus, they tend to produce fewer novel solutions (Jansson & Smith Reference Jansson and Smith1991); however, this trend was not observed in our sample.

We further hypothesized that those in conditions with one or more stimuli would find it just right (H2b). Our survey supported this notion as 70% of participants across all conditions – exclusive of BL – found the DB just right, while 11% found it too specific and 19% found it too vague. Interestingly, the statistical analysis unveiled a noteworthy pattern. Participants in both the QVP condition and the PC condition not only regarded the brief as too specific but also failed to produce highly rated novel, appropriate or usable ideas. In contrast, those who scored high or low on creativity metrics had insignificantly different DB perceptions about the specificity of the parameters provided in the DB.

Therefore, the results affirm H2b. The inclusion of stimuli in the DB likely facilitated a better understanding of how the device functions and aided students in envisioning the practical implementation of their design solution. Nonetheless, this did not necessarily result in achieving top quartile scores, although it did demonstrate an enhancement over the mean score.

6. Discussion

6.1. Initial stimulation: design brief

This research aimed to highlight the different outcomes that emerged from the inclusion of DBs in teaching design, combined with a range of stimuli, including quantitative elements (the granular requirements specified in the DB), visual (the accompanying video of an orange squeezer), physical (a physical orange squeezer) and contextual (detailed script exhibiting users’ frustrations with existing orange squeezers and users’ need for orange squeezers) stimuli. The study investigated how these various elements affected outcomes, with a particular focus on how outcomes varied based on the inclusion of these components.

In trying to contribute to this research area focused on effective DB crafting for education, we found that pairing briefs with various inspirational stimuli increases groups’ performance across different metrics. Conversely, we found that what helps in one creativity metric can be a limiting parameter for another metric. Also, combining many stimuli all together was not effective in improving novelty scores. In our sample, novelty is enhanced when single stimuli are present (Brief BL or Q), but those in turn drastically decrease appropriateness and usability. Maybe, significantly improving these metrics requires more information and interaction with artifacts. It underscores the multifaceted nature of improving design outcomes and emphasizes the idea that a singular approach may not be universally applicable to all the diverse metrics under consideration.

While statistical analysis did not reveal significant correlations, our analysis aimed to uncover qualitative insights and potential trends that may inform future research or provide a richer context for understanding the complex relationship between experimental conditions and creativity scores. This approach thus aligns with the understanding that creativity is influenced by a multitude of factors that extend beyond simple correlations. As such, our approach in the subsequent section aimed to explore potential patterns or trends that might not be immediately apparent through traditional statistical correlations.

6.2. Analysis and discussion of visual data and the generation of creative design solutions

6.2.1. Novelty

Groups using the control brief recorded a higher mean score than those using other briefs. The control brief was nominated as “just right” by 75% of the participants (n = 15). Figure 5 shows an exemplar of a highly novel solution to the orange squeezer design. The solution was seen, to some extent, as surprising as it changed existing designs. It consists of a sophisticated machine utilizing a rotatable Archimedes screw system that grabs the orange and blends the inside of one orange. The gadget is holding the orange in place using a clamp, and a syringe spinner extracts the juice through an inlet. It is noteworthy that although no extra stimuli were provided to the design team, the generated solution scored 4.33 for novelty. One plausible but straightforward motive could be that the information in the BL brief was sufficient (just right) for most of this group – and gave impetus to their imagination. At the same time, it did not constrain or fixate them on the example solutions.

Figure 5. Sketch from brief BL (baseline) that scored high in novelty.

The condition where most solutions scored low on novelty was Brief QP, as this condition was the least favorable for novelty as realized from the mean scores. Five students in this group (50%) found the brief narrowing down their ideas, so that it could also play a role in lowering this group’s overall performance. In Figure 6, an example of a solution for this situation is illustrated. Remarkably, although no external visual examples were provided to the design team, the design outcome was almost a replica of the standard prototype for the manual orange squeezer on the market. The standard prototype is probably common knowledge to most designers and, combined with the constraining information, may have introduced to the participants of this group some level of fixation, thus affecting the novelty of their design solution. Only slight variations were observed, such as adding a filter to the base of the squeezer, which was not found in the visual example. On this line, additional studies also showed that exposure to visual examples constrains ideation (Koronis et al., Reference Koronis, Silva, Kang and Yogiaman2020; Vasconcelos et al. Reference Vasconcelos, Cardoso, Sääksjärvi, Chen and Crilly2017; Viswanathan and Linsey Reference Viswanathan and Linsey2012). As a result, the Figure 3 solution scored only 1.00 point for novelty, and the brief itself was perceived by 50% of the students, the one here included, as a restricting one.

Figure 6. Sketch of an orange squeezer from Brief QP with a low novelty score.

6.2.2. Appropriateness

This metric’s highest scores are found in groups provided with Brief QV, where students were shown the BL brief supplemented by a QV example. Eleven participants (61%) found this brief suitable and possibly helped them conceptualize appropriate ideas. Figure 7 shows an illustration of a characteristic solution for this condition considered a satisfactory response to the brief requirements (i.e., manufacturability, washability and low cost). The supplementary quantitative information likely served to gain an insightful understanding of the design requirement and consequently enabled students to pay particular attention to BL briefs’ needs. However, the video exemplar probably helped us understand how such requirements can be fulfilled in practice. The solution was rated as highly appropriate and in line with the brief requests; thus, it scored a mean score of 4.67 out of 5. If the primary goal is to increase performance and user-friendliness, it is arguable that providing a visual or video example is useful for appropriateness (see Koronis et al., Reference Koronis, Silva, Kang and Yogiaman2020). This assertion can be supported by Fu, Cagan, & Kotovsky (Reference Fu, Cagan and Kotovsky2010), who claimed that good examples can help design teams generate high-quality ideas that accomplish the brief’s requirements. Accordingly, students can build on solutions that are known to be effective.

Figure 7. Sketch from BL brief QV with a high appropriateness score.

In the state where most solutions scored low in appropriateness, the BL brief without any exemplar was provided to the students. The survey found that participants designated this brief as highly suitable (13 out of 21), while about half (11 in total) found it helpful in narrowing their ideas down. Figure 8 illustrates an example portrayed by a complex mechanism composed of many parts, making it difficult to manufacture, clean and largely exceed target costs, which were indicated. As a result, this solution did not address the design requirements. It is judicious to cogitate that the lack of outside help such as quantitative requirements with specific numerical values or a visual prototype has been the leading cause of these low scores (see Koronis et al., Reference Koronis, Silva, Kang and Yogiaman2020).

Figure 8. Sketch from brief BL that fared among the lowest scores in appropriateness.

6.2.3. Usability

When participants were provided with the BL brief paired with all stimuli, namely Brief QVPC, it was evidenced that most solutions scored highly for usability. Not surprisingly, this group found the brief not narrowing down their ideas while it was perceived by 13 students (72%) as a just-right brief. An example of a knowledge solution created in this condition is seen in Figure 9. The solution consisted of a practical device comprised of an orange crusher to squeeze out the juice. A juice collector is mounted at the base of the squeezer, making the extraction of juice feasible from more than one orange at once. The available quantitative and contextual information likely helped students generate efficient design outcomes that did not violate the brief constraints.

Figure 9. Sketch from brief QVPC that ranked high in usability.

Furthermore, the instructional video example possibly helped to gain insight into the devices’ function and how they can be effortlessly executed and created in practice. The solution was seen as very useful and scored 4.33. As the literature shows, good examples can help design teams generate high-quality ideas that fulfill the brief’s requirements (Fu et al. Reference Fu, Cagan and Kotovsky2010). On another note, contextual briefs help inspire designers by communicating the higher management’s vision for the design project, aiding designers to commiserate with user needs or difficulties while considering the broader societal benefit of their product or design (Guay, Vallerand, & Blanchard Reference Guay, Vallerand and Blanchard2000).

When students were exposed to a physical example along with the brief (P), most design outcomes were scored low in the usability metric. Half of the group participants (10 students) found it restricting their ideas, while four (20%) nominated it as too vague. Figure 10 depicts an ideal solution that did not satisfy functional needs. The solution comprised a detachable sharpened straw, which could be incorporated into the fruit. The lower part of the straw had a rough surface when turned around and broke through the orange skin. The idea was that the device would allow users to drink the juice directly from the orange. However, this was deliberated as impractical and scored 1 for this metric. The results show that when quantitative information is absent, usability seems to be lowered, and thus, the physical example alone was not effective in conveying how the device could be used in real life.

Figure 10. Sketch from brief P that scored low in usability.

6.3. Post-sketch survey assessment

The inclusion of a post-sketch survey served to validate our hypotheses regarding participants’ perceptions of the DB. In the future, this survey can be expanded to include personality data and deeper personal insights. This information, paired with the DBs, was a thoughtful way to assess the success of the DBs administered to varying groups.

Interestingly, those who felt the DB was too specific were referring to the quantitative, video and physical conditions, while those who thought it was too vague were referring to the PC conditions. Those who found the DB just right were referring to the quantitative and physical conditions, which is interesting as it infers providing potential designers with an interactive object, such as the physical orange squeezer, and the parameters within which to create are most liked by designers. Lastly, those referring to the quantitative, video, physical and contextual conditions found the DB to influence their ideas the least, while those with the quantitative, video and physical conditions were most likely to find the DB to either narrow down or broaden their ideas. We have noticed that contextual briefs, in general, assist in motivating designers to empathize with user needs or difficulties and see the broader societal benefits of their work (Guay et al. Reference Guay, Vallerand and Blanchard2000). However, this was not evidenced in our samples.

In conclusion, our findings suggest that the use of stimuli in conjunction with a BL DB can indeed impact creative outcomes and influence participants’ perceptions of the design process. Importantly, the strength and significance of these patterns varied across different experimental conditions, underscoring the need for further exploration in future research.

6.4. Judge’s rating assessment

Judges were selected carefully for this study to ensure that reliability in scoring was attained. Amabile (Reference Amabile1996) places great emphasis on the selection of appropriate judges for assessment. The need for expert judges was conclusively validated by Dollinger & Shafran (Reference Dollinger and Shafran2005) and later by Kaufman et al. (Reference Kaufman, Baer, Cole and Sexton2008), whose studies found that significant differences existed between expert and non-expert judges’ creativity assessment scores.

Hennessey, Amabile, & Mueller (Reference Hennessey, Amabile, Mueller, Runco and Pritzker2011) addressed the key limitations of the CAT tool, which include the time required to select appropriate judges for evaluation and employ the assessment tool. Additionally, the CAT is not tied to a particular stimulus in methodology, and its use of the paired 6-3-5/C-Sketch method is relatively novel and worthy of exploration. Kaufman (Reference Kaufman2016, p. 78) argues that since experts do not discuss the rating process with one another, this becomes counterintuitive but essential. Hekkert & Van Wieringen (Reference Hekkert and Van Wieringen1996) found that consensus usually only showed the original opinion of one (likely vocal) expert when experts discussed their opinions. We had our judges go through a training session to understand the evaluation rubric and process. All raters evaluated 18% of the data to provide sufficient inter-rater reliability until a good agreement in rating was obtained. We conducted the training of our judges in a cyclical fashion, which involved ongoing discussions and feedback sessions to calibrate their assessments. This iterative process aimed to enhance inter-rater reliability and ensure a consistent understanding of the evaluation rubric and criteria among all judges.

7. Limitations and significance of results

We created a mixed-methods model of creativity stimulation and measurement; it is apparent that there are limitations in the current experimental design, leading to a low level of correlation between variables. It is also evident that refining this model is crucial, particularly concerning the validation of creativity scores. The motivation to add usability as a metric was based on our expert judges’ feedback from our earlier pilot study. In that study, judges felt that the appropriateness metric – which was evaluated in relation to the DB’s requirements – did not sufficiently account for the idea’s effectiveness in satisfying the end user’s goals. We acknowledge the difficulty in assessing user–product interactions unless a usability test is run (Sonderegger & Sauer Reference Sonderegger and Sauer2009). Nevertheless, we initially expected that the sketches would provide sufficient insight into the product’s performance and that our expert judges would evaluate them accordingly. However, it is apparent that further refinement of our model and the introduction of a secondary measure of creativity are warranted to bolster the overall validity and comprehensiveness of our study.

We acknowledge the fruitfulness of using a peer evaluation of the sketches by student designers. Secondly, our research was limited in identifying the reasons behind attaining creativity scores via our observation-based assessment. This can be further expanded by having a more investigative post-sketch survey to flesh out more information on our student’s cognitive process when they receive briefs paired with various stimuli. Lastly, our research is limited to providing participant profiles to better understand our sample in terms of BL creativity scores, whether creative personalities emerge from the sample, and their perceptions before and after the sketch exercise.

An additional implication stemming from the collaborative nature of this design task is the potential dependence of findings on the specific idea generation method that was employed. In the context of the C-Sketch/365 process, it is important to note that sketches were not created independently but by a team of student designers. However, it is reasonable to regard the final collective outcome as a distinct and independent observation, initiated by one student and further developed by other team members. Consequently, we acknowledge that our research findings do not explain whether implicit factors inherent in the entire creative process may either enhance or reduce the impacts on individual creative outcomes.

The stimulation and measurement of creativity have been attempted within this study through the use of existing but disparate design tools and frameworks: from DBs paired with different stimuli to the 6-3-5/C-Sketch methodology, post-sketch survey and sketch assessments for the stimulation and measurement of creativity. This mixed application of methodologies was assessed via a post-sketch survey and judgment of creativity based on a rubric-based system. Despite the inherent limitations, this paper introduces a novel and controlled method for investigating and quantifying creativity within research and design contexts. This method is aimed at refining our understanding of creativity and its promotion across various fields and workplaces, with particular relevance to educational settings.

8. Conclusions

This study aimed to answer two research questions, which focused on the identification of stimuli that augmented the creativity metrics and tested how various DBs were seen by the participants. In evaluating the stimuli that enhance creativity, our study identified quantitative information and the BL brief (Q) as positively influencing novelty scores. However, representations that enhanced the appropriateness of design outcomes were the pairing of QV, while usability increased when all effects were combined (QVPC). The statistical analysis indicated that those who scored high or low on creativity indices had insignificantly different perceptions of the DB. No significant correlations existed between DB perceptions and creativity metrics.

In our study, we introduced four distinct stimuli to augment the DB, with the aim of stimulating creativity. We meticulously analyzed the advantages and limitations associated with each stimulus, contributing valuable insights to the ongoing discourse on the composition of effective DBs. However, our contributions extend beyond the confines of educational design settings, carrying implications that are particularly relevant to boosting creativity in other institutional settings.

We firmly advocate for a broader and more concerted endeavor in the realm of controlled research, specifically directed toward optimizing DBs to unleash creativity. We encourage educators, researchers and practitioners to recognize the significance of nurturing creativity within educational contexts. The insights garnered from this study can inform educational practices, fostering an environment where creativity is not just encouraged but actively cultivated.

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit http://doi.org/10.1017/dsj.2023.32.

References

Amabile, T. M. 1979 Effects of external evaluation on artistic creativity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 37 (2), 221233. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.37.2.221Google Scholar
Amabile, T. M. 1982 Children’s artistic creativity: Detrimental effects of competition in a field setting. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 8, 573578.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amabile, T. M. 1983 The social psychology of creativity: A componential conceptualization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 45, 357377.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amabile, T. M. 1996 Creativity in Context: Update to the Social Psychology of Creativity. Routledge.Google Scholar
Amabile, T. M., Goldfarb, P. & Brackfield, S. C. 1990 Social influences on creativity: Evaluation, coaction, and surveillance. Creativity Research Journal 3, 621.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amabile, T. M., & Pillemer, J. 2012 Perspectives on the Social Psychology of Creativity. Journal of Creative Behavior 46 (1), 315; doi:10.1002/jocb.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baer, J. 1993 Divergent Thinking and Creativity: A Task-Specific Approach. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc..Google Scholar
Baer, J. 2003 The impact of the core knowledge curriculum on creativity. Creativity Research Journal 15, 297300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baer, J. & McKool, S. 2009 Assessing creativity using the consensual assessment technique. In Handbook of Research on Assessment Technologies, Methods, and Applications in Higher Education (ed. Schreiner, C. S.), pp. 6577. IGI Global; doi:10.4018/978-1-60566-667-9.ch004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berglas, S., Amabile, T. M. & Handel, M. 1981 Effects of evaluation on children’s artistic creativity. Unpublished manuscript, Brandeis University.Google Scholar
Bourgeois-Bougrine, S., Buisine, S., Vandendriessche, C., Glaveanu, V. & Lubart, T. 2017 Engineering students’ use of creativity and development tools in conceptual product design: What, when and how? Thinking Skills and Creativity 24, 104117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J. S. 1962 The conditions of creativity. In Contemporary Approaches to Creative Thinking: A Symposium Held at the University of Colorado (ed. Gruber, H. E., Terrell, G. & Wertheimer, M.), pp. 130. Atherton Press.Google Scholar
Cai, H., Yi-Luen Do, E. & Zimring, C. M. 2010 Extended linkography and distance graph in design evaluation: An empirical study of the dual effects of inspiration sources in creative design. Design Studies 31, 146168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Camburn, B. A., Auernhammer, J. M., Sng, K. H. E., Mignone, P. J., Arlitt, R. M., Perez, K. B., & Wood, K. L. 2017 Design innovation: A study of integrated practice. In Proceedings of the ASME 2017 International Design Engineering Technical Conferences and Computers and Information in Engineering Conference. V007T06A031; doi:10.1115/DETC2017-68382.Google Scholar
Carson, S. 2006 Creativity and Mental Illness. Invitational Panel Discussion Hosted by Yale’s Mind Matters Consortium. New Haven, CT.Google Scholar
Casakin, H., Koronis, G. & Silva, A. 2022 The effect of potential analogies and negative issues on creative designs. Journal of Mechanical Design 145 (5), 051401; doi:10.1115/1.4056233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheng, M. Y. V. 2015 Assessment for creative teaching and learning in disciplined improvisation. In The Routledge International Handbook of Research on Teaching Thinking (ed. Wegerif, R., Li, L. & Kaufman, J.), pp. 330343. Routledge.Google Scholar
Craft, A., Jeffrey, B. & Leibling, M. 2001 Creativity in Education (ed. Craft, A., Jeffrey, B. & Leibling, M.). Continuum.Google Scholar
Creswell, J. W. & Clark, V. L. P. 2018 Designing and Conducting Mixed Methods Research, 3rd edn. Sage.Google Scholar
Cropley, A. J. 2000 Defining and measuring creativity: Are creativity tests worth using? Roeper Review 23 (2), 7279.Google Scholar
Cross, N. 2007 From a design science to a design discipline: Understanding designerly ways of knowing and thinking. In Design Research Now: Essays and Selected Projects (ed. Michel, R.), pp. 4154: Birkhäuser; doi:10.1007/978-3-7643-8472-2_3.Google Scholar
Csikszentmihalyi, M. 1996 The creative personality. Psychology Today 29, 3640.Google Scholar
Dean, D. L., Hender, J. M., Rodgers, T. L. & Santanen, E. L. 2006 Identifying quality, novel, and creative ideas: Constructs and scales for idea evaluation. Journal of the Association for Information Systems 7, 646698.Google Scholar
Denzin, N. K. 1978 The Research Act: A Theoretical Orientation to Sociological Methods. McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Dollinger, S. J. & Shafran, M. 2005 Note on consensual assessment technique in creativity research. Perceptual and Motor Skills 100, 592598.Google Scholar
Dorst, K. & Cross, N. 2001 Creativity in the design process: Co-evolution of problem–solution. Design Studies 22 (5), 425437.Google Scholar
Feldhusen, J. F. & Goh, B. E. 1995 Assessing and accessing creativity: An integrative review of theory, research, and development. Creativity Research Journal 8 (3), 231247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fleer, M. 2000 Interactive technology: Can children construct their own technological design briefs? Research in Science Education 30 (2), 241253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fu, K., Cagan, J. & Kotovsky, K. 2010 Design team convergence: The influence of example solution quality. Journal of Mechanical Design 132, 111005.Google Scholar
Genco, N., Hölttä‐Otto, K. & Seepersad, C. C. 2012 An experimental investigation of the innovation capabilities of undergraduate engineering students. Journal of Engineering Education 101, 6081.Google Scholar
Goldschmidt, G. 1991 The dialectics of sketching. Creativity Research Journal 4, 123143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldschmidt, G. & Rodgers, P. A. 2013 The design thinking approaches of three different groups of designers based on self-reports. Design Studies 34, 454471.Google Scholar
Gonçalves, M., Cardoso, C. & Badke-Schaub, P. 2016 Inspiration choices that matter: The selection of external stimuli during ideation. Design Science 2, e10.Google Scholar
Gu, J., Chen, Z., Huang, Q., Liu, H. & Huang, S. 2018 A multilevel analysis of the relationship between shared leadership and creativity in inter‐organizational teams. Journal of Creative Behavior 52, 109126.Google Scholar
Guay, F., Vallerand, R. J. & Blanchard, C. 2000 On the assessment of situational intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: The situational motivation scale (SIMS). Motivation and Emotion 24, 175213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haug, A. 2015 Emergence patterns for client design requirements. Design Studies 39, 4869.Google Scholar
Heale, R. & Twycross, A. 2015 Validity and reliability in quantitative studies. Evidence-Based Nursing 18, 6667.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hekkert, P. & Van Wieringen, P. C. W. 1996 Beauty in the eye of expert and nonexpert beholders: A study in the appraisal of art. American Journal of Psychology 109, 389407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hennessey, B. 1982 Effects of reward and task label on children’s creativity in three domains. Unpublished manuscript, Brandeis University, Waltham.Google Scholar
Hennessey, B. A. 1994 The consensual assessment technique: An examination of the relationship between ratings of product and process creativity. Creativity Research Journal 7, 193208.Google Scholar
Hennessey, B. A. 2003 The Social Psychology of Creativity, Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research 47 (3), 253271; doi: 10.1080/00313830308601.Google Scholar
Hennessey, B. A., Amabile, T. M. & Mueller, J. S. 2011 Consensual assessment. In Encyclopaedia of Creativity (ed. Runco, M. A. & Pritzker, S. R.), Vol. 1, pp. 253260. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Howard, T. J., Dekoninck, E. A. & Culley, S. J. 2010 The use of creative stimuli at early stages of industrial product innovation. Research in Engineering Design 21, 263274.Google Scholar
International Organization for Standardization. 2018 Ergonomics of human-system interaction–Part 11: Usability: Definitions and Concepts (ISO standard No. 9241-11).Google Scholar
Jansson, D. G. & Smith, S. M. 1991 Design fixation. Design Studies 12, 311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jick, T. D. 1979 Mixing qualitative and quantitative methods: Triangulation in action. Administrative Science Quarterly 24, 602611.Google Scholar
Jiju, A. 2003 Design of Experiments for Engineers and Scientists. Elsevier Ltd.Google Scholar
Johnson, R. B., Onwuegbuzie, A. J. & Turner, L. A. 2007 Toward a definition of mixed methods research. Journal of Mixed Methods Research 1, 112133.Google Scholar
Kampylis, P. G. & Valtanen, J. 2010 Redefining creativity—Analyzing definitions, collocations, and consequences. Journal of Creative Behavior 44, 191214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaufman, J. C. 2016 Creativity 101. Springer Publishing Company.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaufman, J. C., Baer, J., Cole, J. C. & Sexton, J. D. 2008 A comparison of expert and nonexpert raters using the consensual assessment technique. Creativity Research Journal 20, 171178.Google Scholar
Kaufman, J. C., Baer, J. & Gentile, C. A. 2004 Differences in gender and ethnicity as measured by ratings of three writing tasks. Journal of Creative Behavior 38, 5669.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaufman, S. B., Kaufman, J. C. 2007 Ten years to expertise, many more to greatness: An investigation of modern writers. Journal of Creative Behavior 41, 114124.Google Scholar
King, A. M. & Sivaloganathan, S. 1999 Development of a methodology for concept selection in flexible design strategies. Journal of Engineering Design 10, 329349.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koronis, G., Silva, A., Kang, J. K. S., & Yogiaman, C. 2020 How to Best Frame a Design Brief to Maximize Novelty and Usefulness in Idea Generation. In Proceedings of the Design Society: DESIGN Conference. Cambridge University Press, 1, pp. 17451754; doi: 10.1017/dsd.2020.77Google Scholar
Koronis, G., Casakin, H. & Silva, A. 2021 Crafting briefs to stimulate creativity in the design studio. Thinking Skills and Creativity 40, 100810.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koronis, G., Casakin, H. & Silva, A. 2023 An experimental comparison of analogy representation effects on creative outcomes. Journal of Creative Behavior; doi:10.1002/jocb.611.Google Scholar
Koronis, G., Chia, P. Z., Kang, K. S. J., Silva, A., Yogiaman, C. & Raghunath, N. 2019 An empirical study on the impact of design brief information on the creativity of design outcomes with consideration of gender, and gender diversity. Journal of Mechanical Design 141, 071102071114.Google Scholar
Koronis, G., Silva, A., Kang Kai Siang, J. & Yogiaman, C. 2022 A study on the link between design brief structure and stimulus fidelity to optimize novelty and usefulness. Artificial Intelligence for Engineering Design, Analysis and Manufacturing 36, e5.Google Scholar
Lawson, B. 2006 How Designers Think. Routledge; doi:10.4324/9780080454979.Google Scholar
Liikkanen, L. A. & Perttula, M. 2008 Inspiring design idea generation: Insights from a memory-search perspective. Journal of Engineering Design 21, 545560.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Linsey, J. S., Green, M. G., Murphy, J. T., Wood, K. L. & Markman, A. B. 2005 Collaborating to success: An experimental study of group idea generation techniques. In Proceedings of the ASME 2005 International Design Engineering Technical Conferences and Computers and Information. Volume 5a: 17th International Conference on Design Theory and Methodology, pp. 277290. ASME; doi:10.1115/DETC2005-85351.Google Scholar
Littleton, K., Rojas-Drummond, S. & Miell, D. 2008 Introduction to special issue: Collaborative creativity: Sociocultural perspectives. Thinking Skills and Creativity 3, 175176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lopez, E. C., Esquivel, G. B. & Houtz, J. C. 1993 The creative skills of culturally and linguistically diverse gifted students. Creativity Research Journal 6, 401412.Google Scholar
Ma, H. H. 2009 The effect size of variables associated with creativity: A meta-analysis. Creativity Research Journal 21, 3042.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDonagh-Philp, D. & Lebbon, C. 2000 The emotional domain in product design. Design Journal 3, 3143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McIntyre, P. 2011 Rethinking the creative process: The systems model of creativity applied to popular songwriting. Journal of Music Technology and Education 4, 7790.Google Scholar
Mullen, B., Johnson, C. & Salas, E. 1991 Productivity loss in brainstorming groups: A meta-analytic integration. Basic and Applied Social Psychology 12, 323.Google Scholar
Nieto, M. J. & Santamaría, L. 2007 The importance of diverse collaborative networks for the novelty of product innovation. Technovation 27, 367377.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nutt, B. 1993 The strategic brief. Facilities 11, 2832.Google Scholar
Onwuegbuzie, A. J. & Collins, K. M. 2007 A typology of mixed methods sampling designs in social science research. Qualitative Report 12, 281316.Google Scholar
Otto, K. N. & Wood, K. L. 2001 Product Design: Techniques in Reverse Engineering and New Product Development. Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Park, N. K., Chun, M. Y. & Lee, J. 2016 Revisiting individual creativity assessment: Triangulation in subjective and objective assessment methods. Creativity Research Journal 28, 110.Google Scholar
Payne, S. R., Mackrill, J., Cain, R., Strelitz, J. & Gate, L. 2015 Developing interior design briefs for health-care and well-being centers through public participation. Architectural Engineering and Design Management 11 (4), 264279.Google Scholar
Phillips, P. L. 2004 Creating the Perfect Design Brief: How to Manage Design for Strategic Advantage. Skyhorse Publishing Inc.Google Scholar
Plucker, J. A. & Makel, M. C. 2010 Assessment of creativity. In The Cambridge Handbook of Creativity (ed. Kaufman, J. C. & Sternberg, R. J.), pp. 4873. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Raghunath, N., Koronis, G. & Silva, A. 2020 Creativity, entrepreneurship, innovation, leadership, and professional motivations: Comparing design students in different levels of undergraduate education. In Proceedings of the 2020 IEEE International Conference on Teaching, Assessment, and Learning for Engineering, pp. 7782. TALE; doi:10.1109/TALE48869.2020.9368467.Google Scholar
Rohrbach, B. 1969 Kreativ nach regeln. Absatzwirtschaft 12, 7375.Google Scholar
Runco, M. A. & Jaeger, G. J. 2012 The standard definition of creativity. Creativity Research Journal 24 (1), 9296.Google Scholar
Sadowska, N. & Laffy, D. 2017 The design brief: Inquiry into the starting point in a learning journey. Design Journal 20 (Supplement 1), S1380S1389.Google Scholar
Sarkar, P. & Chakrabarti, A. 2007 Development of a method assessing design creativity. In Proceedings the 16th International Conference on Engineering Design, pp. 349350. ICEDGoogle Scholar
Sarkar, P. & Chakrabarti, A. 2008 The effect of representation of triggers on design outcomes. Artificial Intelligence for Engineering Design, Analysis, and Manufacturing 22 (2), 101116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sauder, J. & Jin, Y. 2016 A qualitative study of collaborative stimulation in group design thinking. Design Science 2, E4; doi:10.1017/dsj.2016.1.Google Scholar
Sawyer, K. 2012 Extending sociocultural theory to group creativity. Vocations and Learning 5, 5975.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sawyer, K. 2017 Group Genius: The Creative Power of Collaboration. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Sawyer, R. K. 2011 Explaining Creativity: The Science of Human Innovation. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Shah, J. J. 1993 Method 5-1-4 GA variation on Method 635. MAE540 Class Notes, 635.Google Scholar
Shah, J. J., Vargas-Hernandez, N. O. E., Summers, J. D. & Kulkarni, S. 2001 Collaborative sketching (C-sketch)—An idea generation technique for engineering design. Journal of Creative Behavior 35, 168198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sonderegger, A. & Sauer, J. 2009 The influence of laboratory set-up in usability tests: Effects on user performance, subjective ratings, and physiological measures. Ergonomics 52, 13501361.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. & Lubart, T. I. 1999 The concept of creativity: Prospects and paradigms. In Handbook of Creativity (ed. Sternberg, R. J.), pp. 315. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Stubbs, M. L. 1981 Methodological improvements in exploring the relationship between fantasy and creativity. Unpublished manuscript, Brandeis University.Google Scholar
Stubbs, M. L. & Amabile, T. M. 1979 Explaining the relationship between fantasy and creativity. In 3rd Annual Conference on the Fantasy and Imaging Process, New York. 1979.Google Scholar
Vasconcelos, L. A., Cardoso, C. C., Sääksjärvi, M., Chen, C. C. & Crilly, N. 2017 Inspiration and fixation: The influences of example designs and system properties in idea generation. Journal of Mechanical Design 139, 113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Viswanathan, V. K., & Linsey, J. S. 2012 Physical Models and Design Thinking: A Study of Functionality, Novelty and Variety of Ideas. Journal of Mechanical Design 134 (9), 091004-091004-091013. doi:10.1115/1.400714Google Scholar
White, C. K., Wood, K. L. & Jensen, D. 2012 From brainstorming to C-sketch to principles of historical innovators: Ideation techniques to enhance student creativity. Journal of STEM Education 13, 1225.Google Scholar
Yilmaz, S., Daly, S., Seifert, C. & Gonzalez, R. 2015 How do designers generate new ideas? Design heuristics across two disciplines. Design Science 1, E4.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Ma’s stages of the problem-solving process.

Figure 1

Figure 2. An overview of the model developed for the stimulation and measurement of creativity.

Figure 2

Figure 3. Cohort (Co.) breakdown by design brief and stimuli.

Figure 3

Figure 4. Illustration of the 6-3-5/C-sketch.

Figure 4

Table 1. Mean scores and standard deviation (SD) per brief

Figure 5

Table 2. Key findings from mean scores and statistical analysis

Figure 6

Figure 5. Sketch from brief BL (baseline) that scored high in novelty.

Figure 7

Figure 6. Sketch of an orange squeezer from Brief QP with a low novelty score.

Figure 8

Figure 7. Sketch from BL brief QV with a high appropriateness score.

Figure 9

Figure 8. Sketch from brief BL that fared among the lowest scores in appropriateness.

Figure 10

Figure 9. Sketch from brief QVPC that ranked high in usability.

Figure 11

Figure 10. Sketch from brief P that scored low in usability.

Supplementary material: File

Raghunath et al. supplementary material
Download undefined(File)
File 664.8 KB