Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-g8jcs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T19:51:17.013Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

External cues improve visual working memory encoding in the presence of salient distractors in schizophrenia

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 March 2024

Catherine V. Barnes-Scheufler*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Psychosomatic Medicine and Psychotherapy, Goethe University Frankfurt, University Hospital, Frankfurt, Germany
Lara Rösler
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Psychosomatic Medicine and Psychotherapy, Goethe University Frankfurt, University Hospital, Frankfurt, Germany Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
Michael Schaum
Affiliation:
Leibniz Institute for Resilience Research, Mainz, Germany
Carmen Schiweck
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Psychosomatic Medicine and Psychotherapy, Goethe University Frankfurt, University Hospital, Frankfurt, Germany
Benjamin Peters
Affiliation:
Institute of Medical Psychology, Goethe University Frankfurt, Frankfurt, Germany Zuckerman Mind Brain Behavior Institute, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
Jutta S. Mayer
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, Goethe University Frankfurt, University Hospital, Frankfurt, Germany
Andreas Reif
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Psychosomatic Medicine and Psychotherapy, Goethe University Frankfurt, University Hospital, Frankfurt, Germany
Michael Wibral
Affiliation:
Campus Institute for Dynamics of Biological Networks, Georg-August University, Göttingen, Germany
Robert A. Bittner
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Psychosomatic Medicine and Psychotherapy, Goethe University Frankfurt, University Hospital, Frankfurt, Germany Ernst Strüngmann Institute (ESI) for Neuroscience in Cooperation with Max Planck Society, Frankfurt, Germany
*
Corresponding author: Catherine V. Barnes-Scheufler; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

People with schizophrenia (PSZ) are impaired in attentional prioritization of non-salient but relevant stimuli over salient distractors during visual working memory (VWM) encoding. Conversely, guidance of top–down attention by external predictive cues is intact. Yet, it is unknown whether this preserved ability can help PSZ encode more information in the presence of salient distractors.

Methods

We employed a visuospatial change-detection task using four Gabor patches with differing orientations in 66 PSZ and 74 healthy controls (HCS). Two Gabor patches flickered which were designated either as targets or distractors and either a predictive or a non-predictive cue was displayed to manipulate top–down attention, resulting in four conditions.

Results

We observed significant effects of group, salience and cue as well as significant interactions of salience by cue, group by salience and group by cue. Across all conditions, PSZ stored significantly less information in VWM than HCS. PSZ stored significantly less non-flickering than flickering information with a non-predictive cue. However, PSZ stored significantly more flickering and non-flickering information with a predictive cue.

Conclusions

Our findings indicate that control of attentional selection is impaired in schizophrenia. We demonstrate that additional top–down information significantly improves performance in PSZ. The observed deficit in attentional control suggests a disturbance of GABAergic inhibition in early visual areas. Moreover, our findings are indicative of a mechanism for enhancing attentional control in PSZ, which could be utilized by pro-cognitive interventions. Thus, the current paradigm is suitable to reveal both preserved and compromised cognitive component processes in schizophrenia.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Visuospatial working memory (VWM) and attention are two intricately and bi-directionally related cognitive domains (Cowan, Reference Cowan2001; Gazzaley & Nobre, Reference Gazzaley and Nobre2012; Mayer et al., Reference Mayer, Bittner, Nikolic, Bledowski, Goebel and Linden2007) essential for elucidating the basis of perturbed information processing in schizophrenia (Carter & Barch, Reference Carter and Barch2007). The role of top–down attention for the encoding of VWM representations is particularly important due to clear behavioral (Hahn et al., Reference Hahn, Robinson, Kaiser, Harvey, Beck, Leonard and Gold2010; Hartman, Steketee, Silva, Lanning, & McCann, Reference Hartman, Steketee, Silva, Lanning and McCann2003; Mayer & Park, Reference Mayer and Park2012) and neurophysiological (Bittner et al., Reference Bittner, Linden, Roebroeck, Hartling, Rotarska-Jagiela, Maurer and Haenschel2015; Haenschel et al., Reference Haenschel, Bittner, Haertling, Rotarska-Jagiela, Maurer, Singer and Linden2007) evidence for a primary impairment of this VWM component process in people with schizophrenia (PSZ).

Specifically, top–down attention is crucial for selecting goal-relevant information to be stored in VWM, and the efficiency of this mechanism has a considerable influence on VWM capacity (Cowan & Morey, Reference Cowan and Morey2006; Vogel, McCollough, & Machizawa, Reference Vogel, McCollough and Machizawa2005). During VWM encoding, top–down attention – which is driven by stimulus relevance, competes with bottom–up attention – which is driven by stimulus salience (Liesefeld, Liesefeld, Sauseng, Jacob, & Müller, Reference Liesefeld, Liesefeld, Sauseng, Jacob and Müller2020; Lorenc, Mallett, & Lewis-Peacock, Reference Lorenc, Mallett and Lewis-Peacock2021). Both the salience and relevance of an object influence its processing in VWM (Constant & Liesefeld, Reference Constant and Liesefeld2021). If multiple items compete for limited attentional resources, selection of relevant and inhibition of irrelevant information depends on the successful execution of two top–down control processes (Desimone & Duncan, Reference Desimone and Duncan1995). First, the control of selection aids the identification of relevant information which should be selected (Luck & Gold, Reference Luck and Gold2008). Second, the implementation of selection enables the differential processing of relevant and irrelevant information (Luck & Gold, Reference Luck and Gold2008). Neural computations during attentional competition assign a distinct priority to each stimulus, which is the combined representation of its salience and behavioral relevance (Fecteau & Munoz, Reference Fecteau and Munoz2006). To this end, the control of attentional selection is guided by an attentional set, which induces a top–down attentional bias toward the most relevant information based on current goals (Bichot, Rossi, & Desimone, Reference Bichot, Rossi and Desimone2005; Corbetta & Shulman, Reference Corbetta and Shulman2002; Gaspelin & Luck, Reference Gaspelin and Luck2018a, Reference Gaspelin and Luck2018b; Wolfe, Reference Wolfe1994). According to the signal suppression hypothesis, this mechanism also facilitates the active suppression of automatic attentional capture by visually salient distractors through inhibitory mechanisms (Gaspelin & Luck, Reference Gaspelin and Luck2018a, Reference Gaspelin and Luck2018b; Sawaki & Luck, Reference Sawaki and Luck2010). Thus, generating a priority map involves the parallelized up- and down-weighting of stimuli, both of which are essential elements of the implementation of selection (Gaspelin & Luck, Reference Gaspelin and Luck2018a, Reference Gaspelin and Luck2018b).

Behavioral VWM experiments using flickering stimuli as part of an encoding array clearly indicate a strong attentional bias in PSZ toward highly salient stimuli, even if they are behaviorally irrelevant (Hahn et al., Reference Hahn, Robinson, Kaiser, Harvey, Beck, Leonard and Gold2010). This bottom–up attentional bias surpasses the top–down bias induced by the attentional set, leading to a failure to correctly prioritize non-salient but relevant stimuli over salient but irrelevant distractors. Consequently, distractors are encoded into VWM with inadequately high priority. The correlation between the magnitude of the bottom–up attentional bias and reduced VWM capacity (Hahn et al., Reference Hahn, Robinson, Kaiser, Harvey, Beck, Leonard and Gold2010) underscores its importance for VWM dysfunction. These findings indicate a specific impairment of the implementation of attentional selection in PSZ, when top–down control is required to overcome salient distractors. Importantly, control of attentional selection relying solely on an internally generated attentional set is also disturbed in PSZ (Fuller et al., Reference Fuller, Luck, Braun, Robinson, McMahon and Gold2006).

Conversely, patients' ability to utilize external spatial cues to guide top–down attention during VWM encoding and to prioritize information correctly without the presence of salient distractors is intact (Gold et al., Reference Gold, Fuller, Robinson, McMahon, Braun and Luck2006). This is indicative of an ‘island of preserved cognitive function’ (Gold, Hahn, Strauss, & Waltz, Reference Gold, Hahn, Strauss and Waltz2009), which can provide important clues about the cognitive and neurophysiological mechanisms underlying attentional and VWM encoding dysfunction. However, it remains unknown whether top–down attentional control of PSZ aided by external cues can prevail when directly challenged by highly salient distractors. Elucidating this issue is crucial for models of impaired attentional control and stimulus prioritization during VWM encoding. In this context, impaired inhibition due to disturbances in the GABAergic system – a central pathophysiological mechanism in schizophrenia (Howes & Shatalina, Reference Howes and Shatalina2022; Lewis, Hashimoto, & Volk, Reference Lewis, Hashimoto and Volk2005) – is highly pertinent.

There is converging evidence that the suppression of attentional capture by salient stimuli relies on top–down inhibitory mechanisms, which induce local inhibition of such stimuli in early visual areas (Gaspelin & Luck, Reference Gaspelin and Luck2018a, Reference Gaspelin and Luck2018b). This process is primarily mediated by inhibitory GABAergic interneurons (Buschman & Kastner, Reference Buschman and Kastner2015; Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Xu, Kamigaki, Do, Chang, Jenvay and Dan2014). Notably, post-mortem studies have consistently demonstrated widespread abnormalities in cortical inhibitory GABAergic interneurons in PSZ resulting in reduced inhibition (Lewis, Reference Lewis2014). Neuroimaging, behavioral and computational data also support a central role of reduced GABAergic inhibition for cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia (Shaw et al., Reference Shaw, Knight, Freeman, Williams, Moran, Friston and Singh2020). Therefore, impaired GABAergic inhibition in early visual areas might be a crucial mechanism underlying the insufficient suppression of attentional capture by salient stimuli.

Yet, the successful use of spatial cues by PSZ to overcome this deficit would indicate that enhancing inhibitory control of attentional selection exerted by frontal-parietal areas could sufficiently strengthen local inhibition within early visual areas. Moreover, it would imply that impaired control of selection is the central attentional deficit. The goal of the current study was to test these hypotheses in a behavioral VWM experiment directly contrasting top–down and bottom–up attentional processes during VWM encoding. To this end, we employed a VWM paradigm with an encoding array containing an equal number of salient (flickering) and non-salient (non-flickering) Gabor patches with different orientations. Depending on the specific task condition, either the salient or non-salient Gabor patches would be most relevant and probed preferentially (Gaspelin & Luck, Reference Gaspelin and Luck2018a, Reference Gaspelin and Luck2018b). The encoding array was preceded by either a predictive external cue indicating the location of task-relevant stimuli, or a non-predictive external cue providing no such information.

Methods and materials

Participants

We recruited 66 PSZ from psychiatric outpatient facilities in and around Frankfurt am Main, Germany. We established diagnoses of all patients according to DSM-5 criteria based on a clinical interview and careful chart review at a consensus conference. We used the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) to assess current psychopathology. All patients were on stable antipsychotic medication for at least one month at the time of the study.

Seventy-four healthy control subjects (HCS) were recruited by online and printed advertisements. They were screened using the German version of the Structural Clinical Interview SCID-I from the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual, Version IV (Saß, Wittchen, Zaudig, & Houben, Reference Saß, Wittchen, Zaudig and Houben2003). They reported no history of any psychiatric illness and no family history of psychiatric illness in first-degree relatives. All participants reported no history of neurological illness, no drug use (excluding nicotine) within the past six months, normal or corrected-to-normal vision, and no color-blindness.

We matched groups for age, sex, premorbid IQ, years of education, and parental years of education (Table 1). Premorbid verbal intelligence was assessed by the German Mehrfachwahl-Wortschatz-Intelligenz Test (Lehrl, Merz, Burkhard, & Fischer, Reference Lehrl, Merz, Burkhard and Fischer2005). The ethics committee of the University Hospital Frankfurt approved all study procedures. Subjects provided written informed consent after receiving a complete description of the study.

Table 1. Participant characteristics of both groups including information regarding demographics, psychopathology and current medication.

Values are mean (S.D.), or n. All statistics reported are 2-tailed. Abbreviations: HCS = healthy control subjects; PSZ = people with schizophrenia; PANSS = Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale; FGA = first-generation antipsychotics; SGA = second-generation antipsychotics; df = degrees of freedom.

Working memory task

A visuospatial change detection task using Gabor patches (Fig. 1) was implemented on a personal computer using Presentation software version 14.9 (www.neurobs.com). Stimuli were presented on a gray background (RGB values: 191, 191, 191) in a dimly lit room with a viewing distance of approximately 60 cm.

Figure 1. Visual change detection task with four conditions; flickering/predictive cue, flickering/non-predictive cue, non-flickering/predictive cue, non-flickering/non-predictive cue. Flickering is indicated by white dashes around stimuli. The set size of four items was kept constant. In 80% of trials, a designated target stimulus was probed during retrieval (target trials). In 20% of trials, a distractor was probed during retrieval (catch trials). Only target trials are depicted in the figure.

Gabor patches had a visual angle of 0.96° each and were placed on an imaginary circle (visual angle of the diameter: 3.82°). Throughout the experiment a black fixation cross (visual angle: 0.48°) was presented at the center of the screen. Subsequently, either a predictive or a non-predictive external cue was presented by briefly turning the fixation cross white for 300 ms. For predictive cues, the white arms of the fixation cross indicated the future locations of the two most relevant Gabor patches. For non-predictive cues, the entire fixation cross turned white, providing no location information. After a 300 ms preparation interval, the encoding array was displayed for 400 ms consisting of four Gabor patches shown at four fixed locations equally spaced on an imaginary circle around the fixation cross. Two of the four patches flickered at a frequency of 7.5 Hz. To manipulate stimulus relevance, participants were instructed before each block that either the flickering or non-flickering stimuli would be probed preferentially (flickering-bias or non-flickering-bias). Accordingly, in 80% of trials we probed one of the relevant Gabor patches (target trials). In 20% of trials, we probed one of the irrelevant Gabor patches (catch trials). Predictive cues always indicated the future locations of the target Gabor patches irrespective of trial type.

The delay phase lasted for 2000 ms on average, with a jitter of ±250 ms. The retrieval array was displayed for 3000 ms consisting of one Gabor patch surrounded by a white frame at the probed location and three blurred out Gabor patches. Probed locations were randomized but counterbalanced. ‘Change’ trials featured a minimum Gabor patch rotation deviation of 45°. Participants had to indicate by button press within 3000 ms, if the orientation of the framed Gabor patch was identical to or different from the Gabor patch shown at the same location in the encoding array. This resulted in four main conditions: flickering-bias/predictive cue, flickering-bias/non-predictive cue, non-flickering-bias/predictive cue, non-flickering-bias/non-predictive cue. Incorporating catch trials in each condition allowed us to assess the efficiency of attentional control operationalized as the difference between the amount of relevant and irrelevant information stored in VWM.

A total of 400 trials were presented, 100 for each condition (80 target and 20 catch trials), divided into eight blocks, counterbalanced in order across participants and groups. Each block began with four practice trials to ensure familiarization with the current stimulus relevance and cue type. To obtain an independent estimate of VWM capacity, we also employed a 60-trial canonical visual change detection task (Barnes-Scheufler et al., Reference Barnes-Scheufler, Passow, Rösler, Mayer, Oertel, Kittel-Schneider and Bittner2021) (see online Supplementary materials).

Analysis of behavioral data

The amount of information stored in VWM was quantified using Cowan's K, where K = (hit rate + correct rejection rate −1) × memory set size (Cowan, Reference Cowan2001; Rouder, Morey, Morey, & Cowan, Reference Rouder, Morey, Morey and Cowan2011). We used a set size of two in accordance with previous studies using comparable task designs (Gold et al., Reference Gold, Fuller, Robinson, McMahon, Braun and Luck2006; Hahn et al., Reference Hahn, Robinson, Kaiser, Harvey, Beck, Leonard and Gold2010). This set size was the most appropriated given that only two stimuli were explicitly designated as targets, leading to a theoretical maximum K of two. Statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS (IBM) Version 22, and R Version 4.3.0 (www.r-project.org). Using a binomial expansion on the 400 trials, we determined that an accuracy of 56% had a cumulative probability of p(X > x) 0.009, i.e., the probability of getting 224 correct responses or more by chance was less than 1%. Accordingly, we excluded participants with an accuracy below 56% (n = 3, all PSZ).

For our main analysis, we fitted a linear mixed model (LMM) estimated using ML and nloptwrap optimizer with R to predict Cowan's K with group, salience, cue, age, and premorbid IQ (Formula: score ~ Salience × Cue × Group + Age + IQ) and subject as a random effect (formula: ~1 Subject). We chose this approach because our dependent variable included repeated measures due to the mixed conditions of our study design.

We based this analysis solely on target trials to study the differential processing of the most relevant information. Overall significance estimates of the LMM were obtained post-hoc with an ANOVA function using Satterthwaite's method. Post-hoc contrasts of the LMM were obtained with the emmeans package (Lenth et al., Reference Lenth, Bolker, Buerkner, Giné-Vázquez, Herve, Jung and Singmann2023), and were computed using the Kenward-Roger degrees-of-freedom method with pairwise t tests adjusted with the Tukey method. The t tests were adjusted for age and premorbid IQ, where age was fixed at 37.1 years and IQ at 112- as determined by the function. Cohen's d was approximated for the effects in the LMM by transforming t-values.

Attentional prioritization and independent VWM capacity estimate

To assess the relationship between the ability to prioritize relevant information and VWM capacity, we correlated the efficiency of attentional prioritization (Cowan's K for target trials minus Cowan's K for catch trials) using Spearman correlations in each group across all four conditions with an independent estimate of VWM capacity (Pashler's K, Reference Pashler1988]) derived from our canonical visual change detection task (Barnes-Scheufler et al., Reference Barnes-Scheufler, Passow, Rösler, Mayer, Oertel, Kittel-Schneider and Bittner2021).

Correlation between target trials and independent WM capacity estimate

Spearman correlations were conducted with the WM estimate of capacity (Pashler' s K) and target trials (Cowan's K) in each condition, separately for each group. Fisher z transformation was used to investigate group differences in correlation strengths.

Results

Amount of information stored in VWM

Averaged across all target conditions, HCS encoded 1.27 items, corresponding to an accuracy of 82% (Fig. 2, online Supplementary Tables S1 and S2). PSZ encoded 0.95 items, corresponding to an accuracy of 74%. HCS encoded more information into VWM in all four target conditions: flickering-bias/predictive cue (mean = 1.32, s.d. = 0.39), flickering-bias/non-predictive cue (mean = 1.29, s.d. = 0.41), non-flickering-bias/predictive cue (mean = 1.28, s.d. = 0.37), non-flickering-bias/non-predictive cue (mean = 1.20, s.d. = 0.43) than PSZ: flickering-bias/predictive cue (mean = 1.07, s.d. = 0.36), flickering-bias/non-predictive cue (mean = 0.97, s.d. = 0.46), non-flickering-bias/predictive cue (mean = 0.99, s.d. = 0.44), non-flickering-bias/non-predictive cue (mean = 0.77, s.d. = 0.44).

Figure 2. Amount of information stored in VWM in target trials, estimated with Cowan's K in healthy control subjects = HCS and people with schizophrenia = PSZ. F/PC = flickering/predictive cue; NF/PC = non-flickering/predictive cue; F/NPC = flickering/non-predictive cue; NF/NPC = non-flickering/non-predictive cue. The main group effect of the LMM is expressed as the between group difference, and the within group comparisons are expressed via the post-hoc between group tests. Asterisks indicate significance p < 0.001 = ***, p < 0.01 = **, p < 0.05 = *. Error bars indicate standard deviation.

LMM effects

We observed significant main effects of group, salience, and cue (all p < 0.001; Table 2). Additionally, we observed the following significant two-way interactions: group by salience (p = 0.037), group by cue (p = 0.003), and salience by cue (p = 0.013), but no significant three-way interaction between group, salience, and cue (p = 0.338). Furthermore, we observed significant effects of the covariates age (p < 0.001) and premorbid IQ (p = 0.022), but no significant interactions of group by age (p = 0.077) or group by premorbid IQ (p = 0.354). The model's total explanatory power was substantial (conditional R 2 = 0.78), and the part related to the fixed effects alone (marginal R 2) was 0.25, indicating a better fit when random effects were included (Barton & Barton, Reference Barton and Barton2015).

Table 2. Results of the linear mixed model investigating the effects of cue (predictive cue/non-predictive cue) and salience (flickering/non-flickering) in target trials in both PSZ and HCS with the covariates age and premorbid IQ.

Overall significance estimates of the linear mixed model were obtained post-hoc with an ANOVA function using Satterthwaite's method. Asterisks indicate significance p < 0.001 = ***, p < 0.01 = **, p < 0.05 = *.

Post-hoc tests

To further explore the significant two-way interactions, we conducted post-hoc tests, which revealed a significant reduction of the amount of information stored in VWM in target trials in PSZ compared to HCS in each condition (all p < 0.001; Table 3). There were no significant differences between conditions in HCS, however there was a trend level improvement for the amount of stored information in the flickering-bias/non-predictive cue condition compared to the non-flickering-bias/non-predictive cue condition (p = 0.050, d = 0.2). PSZ stored significantly more information in the flickering-bias/predictive cue condition compared to the flickering-bias/non-predictive cue condition, albeit with a small effect size (p = 0.049, d = 0.2). Additionally, PSZ stored significantly more information in the non-flickering-bias/predictive cue condition compared to the non-flickering-bias/non-predictive cue condition (p < 0.001, d = 0.5). Finally, PSZ stored significantly more information in the flickering-bias/non-predictive cue condition compared to the non-flickering-bias/non-predictive cue condition (p < 0.001, d = 0.5).

Table 3. Results of post-hoc between and within group comparisons across all four task conditions

Abbreviations: HCS = healthy control subjects; PSZ = people with schizophrenia; df = degrees of freedom; ES = effect size. Comparisons were adjusted for age and premorbid IQ. Asterisks indicate significance p < 0.001 = ***, p < 0.01 = **, p < 0.05 = *

Attentional prioritization and independent WM capacity estimate

Our independent estimate of WM capacity did not correlate with the efficiency of attentional prioritization across all conditions in HCS (rs = 0.162, p = 0.167) or in PSZ (rs = 0.179, p = 0.150), or within any condition of either group (Table 4).

Table 4. Results of Spearman correlations (two-tailed) of independent WM capacity estimate (Pashler's K) and attentional prioritization efficiency (target Cowan's K – catch Cowan's K) across all four conditions, and within each condition

Abbreviations: HCS = healthy control subjects; PSZ = people with schizophrenia.

Correlation between target trials and independent WM capacity estimate

In HCS, we observed a significant correlation between Cowan's K in target trials of each of the four conditions and working memory capacity (Pashler's K) (Table 5). In PSZ, Cowan's K correlated significantly with working memory capacity (Pashler's K) only in the two non-predictive cue conditions. However, Fisher z transformation did not reveal any significant group differences in the strength of these correlations.

Table 5. Results of correlations between visual working memory capacity (Pashler's K) and mean Cowan's K of each target condition

Abbreviations: HCS = healthy control subjects; PSZ = people with schizophrenia. Asterisks indicate significance p < 0.001 = ***, p < 0.01 = **, p < 0.05 = *. The non-significant z scores indicate no significant differences between groups of the correlations of VWM capacity and mean target Cowan's K in each condition.

Discussion

We studied the competition between bottom–up and top–down attentional processes during VWM encoding in PSZ and HCS to elucidate how disturbed attentional control contributes to VWM dysfunction in schizophrenia. We observed a significant effect of group with a decreased amount of information stored in VWM in PSZ across all conditions. Additionally, we observed a significant effect of salience and a significant interaction of salience by group. With predictive cues, there were no differences in the amount of stored information for salient compared to non-salient targets in either group. However, with non-predictive cues, PSZ stored significantly more information for salient compared to non-salient targets. HCS showed a trend in the same direction. We also observed a significant effect of cue and a significant interaction of cue by group. Only in PSZ did predictive cues increase the amount of information stored in VWM for both salient and non-salient targets compared to non-predictive cues. Notably, this improvement was not sufficient to increase the amount of stored information up to the level of HCS.

Overall, we could replicate the bottom–up attentional bias toward salient distractors during VWM encoding in PSZ (Hahn et al., Reference Hahn, Robinson, Kaiser, Harvey, Beck, Leonard and Gold2010) and demonstrate that PSZ were able to utilize predictive external cues to overcome this deficit in attentional control. Notably, the use of stationary rotating stimuli as distractors during VWM encoding did not induce a bottom–up attentional bias in PSZ (Erickson et al., Reference Erickson, Hahn, Leonard, Robinson, Gray, Luck and Gold2015). Yet, here stimulus salience might not have been sufficient to elicit automatic attentional capture, especially in light of extensive evidence for motion processing impairments in PSZ (Chen, Reference Chen2011). Conversely, our flickering visual stimuli should be particularly salient (Merigan & Maunsell, Reference Merigan and Maunsell1993).

However, the central question of our study was concerned with the effects of a predictive cue on attentional prioritization in the presence of salient distractors in PSZ. For the non-flickering-bias conditions, where participants had to suppress highly salient stimuli, a predictive cue significantly increased the amount of stored information only in PSZ. This implies that patients were able to utilize external cues to up-weight the most relevant information. However, there were no group differences in the amount of stored information in catch trials (online Supplementary Fig. S2) and thus no evidence for impaired down-weighting of distractors in PSZ. Therefore, contrary to our main hypothesis we did not observe a specific deficit in attentional prioritization in PSZ. Given that PSZ performed significantly worse in the non-flickering/non-predictive cue compared to the flickering/non-predictive cue condition, attentional capture of salient distractors might have still interfered more strongly with the up-weighting of non-salient targets.

Importantly, our task differs from a previous study reporting impaired attentional prioritization in PSZ (Hahn et al., Reference Hahn, Robinson, Kaiser, Harvey, Beck, Leonard and Gold2010), which directly probed stimulus location. This design likely increased the impact of attentional capture on the encoding of distractor-related information. Conversely, in our study, automatic attentional capture of the locations of distractors might not have sufficiently facilitated the encoding of their orientation. Encoding this additional stimulus feature despite its low relevance would have likely required the voluntary allocation of additional attentional resources. This might have obscured possible deficits in the down-weighting of salient distractors in PSZ.

For PSZ, both high stimulus salience and external cues increased the amount of relevant information stored in VWM. In line with previous studies, these findings indicate that patients' ability to utilize these specific types of bottom–up and top–down cues to implement attentional selection is largely intact (Gold et al., Reference Gold, Fuller, Robinson, McMahon, Braun and Luck2006, Reference Gold, Robinson, Leonard, Hahn, Chen, McMahon and Luck2017). They also confirm that patients' implementation of selection is impaired, when guided only by the attentional set (Fuller et al., Reference Fuller, Luck, Braun, Robinson, McMahon and Gold2006), which appears to contribute to the overall reduction of VWM capacity.

Yet, even when aided by both a predictive cue and high target salience, patients still showed a substantial VWM deficit. Therefore, our findings are compatible with a reduced VWM capacity in schizophrenia independent from specific deficits in attentional control (Barnes-Scheufler et al., Reference Barnes-Scheufler, Passow, Rösler, Mayer, Oertel, Kittel-Schneider and Bittner2021; Hahn, Robinson, Leonard, Luck, & Gold, Reference Hahn, Robinson, Leonard, Luck and Gold2018; Leonard et al., Reference Leonard, Kaiser, Robinson, Kappenman, Hahn, Gold and Luck2013). The correlations between target-trial performance and our independent measure of VWM capacity also support this notion, indicating that PSZ might have fewer slots in VWM.

Importantly, impairment in WM capacity appears to be closely connected to global cognitive impairment in PSZ, but can be distinguished from deficits in executive function (Gold et al., Reference Gold, Robinson, Leonard, Hahn, Chen, McMahon and Luck2017; Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, McMahon, Robinson, Harvey, Hahn, Leonard and Gold2013). Reduced WM capacity in PSZ could be explained by more severely limited attentional weights available for allocation across items. This is compatible with the finding that patients are impaired in their ability to distribute attention broadly (Gray et al., Reference Gray, Hahn, Robinson, Harvey, Leonard, Luck and Gold2014) and show attentional hyperfocusing during visual information processing (Hahn et al., Reference Hahn, Robinson, Kiat, Geng, Bansal, Luck and Gold2022; Luck, Leonard, Hahn, & Gold, Reference Luck, Leonard, Hahn and Gold2019b). It has been argued that hyperfocusing on internal representations in PSZ might constrain both VWM capacity and attentional resources (Luck, Hahn, Leonard, & Gold, Reference Luck, Hahn, Leonard and Gold2019a). Therefore, future studies should investigate the relationship between the capability to distribute attention broadly and attentional filtering during VWM encoding.

Moreover, increased vulnerability to attentional capture by salient distractors in PSZ can also be interpreted as a manifestation of attentional hyperfocusing, i.e. hyperfocusing on irrelevant information. This could imply a shared neurophysiological mechanism involving abnormal GABA-mediated inhibition for both phenomena. However, the exact relationship between the impaired implementation of attentional selection in the presence of salient visual distractors and the degree of attentional hyperfocusing in PSZ remains to be investigated. As attentional hyperfocusing appears to constrain the amount of information PSZ can encode into VWM (Luck et al., Reference Luck, Hahn, Leonard and Gold2019a), it is conceivable that it contributed to the general VWM impairment we observed in PSZ across all task conditions. However, we were not able to make any inferences regarding this interpretation based on our current paradigm.

Our results are well in line with a differential impairment of the two attentional constructs (Luck & Gold, Reference Luck and Gold2008). The control of selection appears to be largely intact in PSZ as the amount of stored information increased significantly when predictive cues were used to guide top–down attention. Conversely, the implementation of selection appears to be impaired in PSZ as less information was stored across all conditions. Post-hoc within group analyses also revealed that our independent estimate of VWM capacity correlated significantly with the amount of stored information in target trials of each condition in HCS, but only in the non-predictive cue conditions in PSZ (Table 5). However, there were no significant group differences regarding the strength of these correlations for any condition as indicated by the Fisher r to z transformation.

Furthermore, disturbances during VWM consolidation, which can constrain performance independent of storage capacity (Xie & Zhang, Reference Xie and Zhang2017, Reference Xie and Zhang2018), may have also contributed to the overall VWM deficit in patients considering that VWM consolidation appears to be slowed in schizophrenia (Fuller et al., Reference Fuller, Luck, Braun, Robinson, McMahon and Gold2009; Fuller, Luck, McMahon, & Gold, Reference Fuller, Luck, McMahon and Gold2005; Stablein et al., Reference Stablein, Storchak, Ghinea, Kraft, Knochel, Prvulovic and Oertel-Knochel2018).

Notably, the analysis of catch trials revealed a significant effect of cue (p = 0.004; online Supplementary Table S5). Predictive cues enhanced the suppression of irrelevant information in both groups, suggesting that patients' ability to use an external spatial cue to down-weight both salient and non-salient distractors is preserved. Additionally, our results are compatible with the interpretation that enhanced inhibitory control during attentional selection increases local inhibition within early visual areas. In accordance with the signal suppression hypothesis, increased inhibition could result in a relative enhancement of the cued locations in the attentional priority map by suppressing irrelevant locations.

The balance of excitatory and inhibitory (E/I) activation mediated by interneurons is a central principle underlying cortical computations, and is crucial for the generation of gamma oscillations, which support cognitive processes including attention and working memory (Buzsáki & Wang, Reference Buzsáki and Wang2012; Fries, Reference Fries2009). Disturbances in the E/I balance in schizophrenia have been attributed to neurobiological and functional abnormalities of cortical GABAergic inhibitory interneurons (Anticevic & Lisman, Reference Anticevic and Lisman2017). Specifically, reduced inhibition of excitatory pyramidal cells can cause widespread disturbances in the E/I balance (Gonzalez-Burgos, Cho, & Lewis, Reference Gonzalez-Burgos, Cho and Lewis2015). The bottom–up attentional bias toward salient distractors observed in our paradigm is indicative of reduced inhibition in schizophrenia, implicating GABA-A receptors as potential targets for future pro-cognitive pharmacological interventions (Page & Coutellier, Reference Page and Coutellier2018).

Interestingly, there was one significant group difference regarding the efficiency of attentional prioritization (target Cowan's K minus catch Cowan's K) for the non-flickering/non-predictive cue condition, with more information being stored by HCS (online Supplementary Table S8). In this condition the efficiency of attentional prioritization was lowest in PSZ, most likely due to hyperfocusing on salient, irrelevant information in the absence of an additional top–down predictive cue.

Notably, we observed a significant correlation of age and the cue effect only in HCS (rs = 0.240, p = 0.040; online Supplementary Table S4), indicating that with increasing age, the difference in the amount of stored information between predictive and non-predictive cue conditions increases. This could provide clues regarding which specific cognitive processes deteriorate in the aging brain. Additionally, in PSZ we observed a significant negative correlation between flickering and non-flickering conditions and premorbid IQ (rs = −0.254, p = 0.039; online Supplementary Table S4). This implies that with increasing IQ, the difference in the amount of stored information between flickering and non-flickering conditions decreases. It appears therefore, that patients with higher IQ were better able to guide attention to the most relevant items irrespective of their salience. This is particularly relevant considering that premorbid IQ predicts functional outcome in PSZ (Leeson, Barnes, Hutton, Ron, & Joyce, Reference Leeson, Barnes, Hutton, Ron and Joyce2009).

One limitation of our study is the lack of eye-tracking data to control for group differences in possible deviations of fixation, especially saccades to salient distractors during non-flickering-bias conditions. Both PSZ and HCS performed near chance level during catch trials across all conditions. However, excessive saccades to salient distractors might have still interfered with the encoding of target stimuli.

To summarize, the current paradigm allowed us to evaluate the utilization of external predictive cues to improve WM performance with visually salient distractors in PSZ. We report an overall reduction in the amount of information encoded into VWM across all four conditions in PSZ compared to HCS, with a significant improvement using predictive cues. Our findings are consistent with a limited VWM slot model in PSZ (Barnes-Scheufler et al., Reference Barnes-Scheufler, Passow, Rösler, Mayer, Oertel, Kittel-Schneider and Bittner2021), yet also indicate the possibility for a significant improvement in performance with the correct aids. The neurophysiological underpinnings of our findings should be illuminated using functional neuroimaging. The relevance of such a study is also underscored by the inclusion of both attention and working memory as constructs in the cognition domain of the Research Domain Criteria initiative (Insel et al., Reference Insel, Cuthbert, Garvey, Heinssen, Pine, Quinn and Wang2010). Given the presence of deficits in attention and working memory in related neuropsychiatric disorders such as bipolar disorder, our paradigm might therefore be valuable for studying the pathophysiology of impaired information processing across diagnostic categories.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291724000059

Acknowledgements

The authors are very thankful to Christoph Fehr and Peter Hustedt for their support in recruiting patients, as well as to Deliah Macht, Lisa Goldschmidt, Tobias Lehmann and Caroline Mirkes for assisting with data acquisition, and Danko Nikolic for helpful feedback.

Authors’ contributions

All authors made substantial contributions to the conception or design of the work, or the acquisition, analysis or interpretation of data. Authors LR, MS, BP, JSM, MW, and RAB designed the experiment. Authors CVB-S and LR acquired the data. Authors CVB-S, CS, BP, JSM, and RAB analyzed the data. Authors CVB-S and RAB undertook the literature searches and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed to and revised the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding statement

We report no financial relationships with commercial interests. C.V Barnes-Scheufler was supported by a ‘main doctus’ scholarship from The Polytechnic Foundation of Frankfurt am Main.

Competing interests

None.

Ethical standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

Publication history

This paper has also been uploaded as a preprint: doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.05.19.442954

References

Anticevic, A., & Lisman, J. (2017). How can global alteration of excitation/inhibition balance lead to the local dysfunctions that underlie schizophrenia? Biological Psychiatry, 81(10), 818820.10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.12.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barnes-Scheufler, C. V., Passow, C., Rösler, L., Mayer, J. S., Oertel, V., Kittel-Schneider, S., … Bittner, R. A. (2021). Transdiagnostic comparison of visual working memory capacity in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. International Journal of Bipolar Disorders, 9(1), 112.10.1186/s40345-020-00217-xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barton, K., & Barton, M. K. (2015). Package ‘mumin’. Version, 1(18), 439.Google Scholar
Bichot, N. P., Rossi, A. F., & Desimone, R. (2005). Parallel and serial neural mechanisms for visual search in macaque area V4. Science (New York, N.Y.), 308(5721), 529534.10.1126/science.1109676CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bittner, R. A., Linden, D. E., Roebroeck, A., Hartling, F., Rotarska-Jagiela, A., Maurer, K., … Haenschel, C. (2015). The when and where of working memory dysfunction in early-onset schizophrenia-A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Cerebral Cortex, 25(9), 24942506. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu050CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buschman, T. J., & Kastner, S. (2015). From behavior to neural dynamics: An integrated theory of attention. Neuron, 88(1), 127144.10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buzsáki, G., & Wang, X.-J. (2012). Mechanisms of gamma oscillations. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 35, 203.10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150444CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carter, C. S., & Barch, D. M. (2007). Cognitive neuroscience-based approaches to measuring and improving treatment effects on cognition in schizophrenia: The CNTRICS initiative. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 33(5), 11311137. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbm081CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, Y. (2011). Abnormal visual motion processing in schizophrenia: A review of research progress. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 37(4), 709715.10.1093/schbul/sbr020CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Constant, M., & Liesefeld, H. R. (2021). Massive effects of saliency on information processing in visual working memory. Psychological Science, 32(5), 682691.10.1177/0956797620975785CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corbetta, M., & Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(3), 201215.10.1038/nrn755CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cowan, N. (2001). The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(1), 87114.10.1017/S0140525X01003922CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cowan, N., & Morey, C. C. (2006). Visual working memory depends on attentional filtering. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(4), 139141.10.1016/j.tics.2006.02.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Desimone, R., & Duncan, J. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18, 193222. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.18.030195.001205CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Erickson, M. A., Hahn, B., Leonard, C. J., Robinson, B., Gray, B., Luck, S. J., & Gold, J. (2015). Impaired working memory capacity is not caused by failures of selective attention in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 41(2), 366373. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbu101CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fecteau, J. H., & Munoz, D. P. (2006). Salience, relevance, and firing: A priority map for target selection. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(8), 382390.10.1016/j.tics.2006.06.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fries, P. (2009). Neuronal gamma-band synchronization as a fundamental process in cortical computation. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 32(1), 209224.10.1146/annurev.neuro.051508.135603CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fuller, R. L., Luck, S. J., Braun, E. L., Robinson, B. M., McMahon, R. P., & Gold, J. M. (2006). Impaired control of visual attention in schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115(2), 266275. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.115.2.266CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fuller, R. L., Luck, S. J., Braun, E. L., Robinson, B. M., McMahon, R. P., & Gold, J. M. (2009). Impaired visual working memory consolidation in schizophrenia. Neuropsychology, 23(1), 7180. doi: 10.1037/a0013854CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fuller, R. L., Luck, S. J., McMahon, R. P., & Gold, J. M. (2005). Working memory consolidation is abnormally slow in schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114(2), 279.10.1037/0021-843X.114.2.279CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gaspelin, N., & Luck, S. J. (2018a). Combined electrophysiological and behavioral evidence for the suppression of salient distractors. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 30(9), 12651280. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01279CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gaspelin, N., & Luck, S. J. (2018b). The role of inhibition in avoiding distraction by salient stimuli. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(1), 7992.10.1016/j.tics.2017.11.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gazzaley, A., & Nobre, A. C. (2012). Top–down modulation: Bridging selective attention and working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(2), 129135. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gold, J. M., Fuller, R. L., Robinson, B. M., McMahon, R. P., Braun, E. L., & Luck, S. J. (2006). Intact attentional control of working memory encoding in schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115(4), 658.10.1037/0021-843X.115.4.658CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gold, J. M., Hahn, B., Strauss, G. P., & Waltz, J. A. (2009). Turning it upside down: Areas of preserved cognitive function in schizophrenia. Neuropsychology Review, 19(3), 294311. doi: 10.1007/s11065-009-9098-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gold, J. M., Robinson, B., Leonard, C. J., Hahn, B., Chen, S., McMahon, R. P., … Luck, S. J. (2017). Selective attention, working memory, and executive function as potential independent sources of cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 44(6), 1227–1234. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbx155Google Scholar
Gonzalez-Burgos, G., Cho, R. Y., & Lewis, D. A. (2015). Alterations in cortical network oscillations and parvalbumin neurons in schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 77(12), 10311040.10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.03.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, B. E., Hahn, B., Robinson, B., Harvey, A., Leonard, C. J., Luck, S. J., & Gold, J. M. (2014). Relationships between divided attention and working memory impairment in people with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 40(6), 14621471. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbu015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haenschel, C., Bittner, R. A., Haertling, F., Rotarska-Jagiela, A., Maurer, K., Singer, W., & Linden, D. E. (2007). Contribution of impaired early-stage visual processing to working memory dysfunction in adolescents with schizophrenia: A study with event-related potentials and functional magnetic resonance imaging. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64(11), 12291240.10.1001/archpsyc.64.11.1229CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hahn, B., Robinson, B. M., Kaiser, S. T., Harvey, A. N., Beck, V. M., Leonard, C. J., … Gold, J. M. (2010). Failure of schizophrenia patients to overcome salient distractors during working memory encoding. Biological Psychiatry, 68(7), 603609. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.04.014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hahn, B., Robinson, B. M., Kiat, J. E., Geng, J., Bansal, S., Luck, S. J., & Gold, J. M. (2022). Impaired filtering and hyperfocusing: Neural evidence for distinct selective attention abnormalities in people with schizophrenia. Cerebral Cortex, 32(9), 19501964.10.1093/cercor/bhab327CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hahn, B., Robinson, B. M., Leonard, C. J., Luck, S. J., & Gold, J. M. (2018). Posterior parietal cortex dysfunction is central to working memory storage and broad cognitive deficits in schizophrenia. Journal of Neuroscience, 38(39), 83788387. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0913-18.2018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hartman, M., Steketee, M. C., Silva, S., Lanning, K., & McCann, H. (2003). Working memory and schizophrenia: Evidence for slowed encoding. Schizophrenia Research, 59(2-3), 99113.10.1016/S0920-9964(01)00366-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howes, O. D., & Shatalina, E. (2022). Integrating the neurodevelopmental and dopamine hypotheses of schizophrenia and the role of cortical excitation-inhibition balance. Biological Psychiatry, 92(6), 501513.10.1016/j.biopsych.2022.06.017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Insel, T., Cuthbert, B., Garvey, M., Heinssen, R., Pine, D. S., Quinn, K., … Wang, P. (2010). Research domain criteria (RDoC): Toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167(7), 748751.10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09091379CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, M. K., McMahon, R. P., Robinson, B. M., Harvey, A. N., Hahn, B., Leonard, C. J., … Gold, J. M. (2013). The relationship between working memory capacity and broad measures of cognitive ability in healthy adults and people with schizophrenia. Neuropsychology, 27(2), 220229. doi: 10.1037/a0032060CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leeson, V. C., Barnes, T. R., Hutton, S. B., Ron, M. A., & Joyce, E. M. (2009). IQ as a predictor of functional outcome in schizophrenia: A longitudinal, four-year study of first-episode psychosis. Schizophrenia Research, 107(1), 5560.10.1016/j.schres.2008.08.014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lehrl, S., Merz, J., Burkhard, G., & Fischer, B. (2005). Mehrfach-wortschatz-intelligenztest MWT-B. Balingen: Spitta Verlag, 75.Google Scholar
Lenth, R. V., Bolker, B., Buerkner, P., Giné-Vázquez, I., Herve, M., Jung, M., … Singmann, H. (2023). emmeans: Estimated Marginal Means, aka Least-Squares Means. 2023-05-11. from https://github.com/rvlenth/emmeansGoogle Scholar
Leonard, C. J., Kaiser, S. T., Robinson, B. M., Kappenman, E. S., Hahn, B., Gold, J. M., & Luck, S. J. (2013). Toward the neural mechanisms of reduced working memory capacity in schizophrenia. Cerebral Cortex, 23(7), 15821592. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs148CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, D. A. (2014). Inhibitory neurons in human cortical circuits: Substrate for cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 26, 2226.10.1016/j.conb.2013.11.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, D. A., Hashimoto, T., & Volk, D. W. (2005). Cortical inhibitory neurons and schizophrenia. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6(4), 312324.10.1038/nrn1648CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liesefeld, H. R., Liesefeld, A. M., Sauseng, P., Jacob, S. N., & Müller, H. J. (2020). How visual working memory handles distraction: Cognitive mechanisms and electrophysiological correlates. Visual Cognition, 28(5-8), 372387.10.1080/13506285.2020.1773594CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lorenc, E. S., Mallett, R., & Lewis-Peacock, J. A. (2021). Distraction in visual working memory: Resistance is not futile. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 25(3), 228239.10.1016/j.tics.2020.12.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luck, S. J., & Gold, J. M. (2008). The construct of attention in schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 64(1), 3439. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.02.014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luck, S. J., Hahn, B., Leonard, C. J., & Gold, J. M. (2019a). The hyperfocusing hypothesis: A new account of cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 45(5), 9911000.10.1093/schbul/sbz063CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luck, S. J., Leonard, C. J., Hahn, B., & Gold, J. M. (2019b). Is attentional filtering impaired in schizophrenia? Schizophrenia Bulletin, 45(5), 10011011.10.1093/schbul/sbz045CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayer, J. S., Bittner, R. A., Nikolic, D., Bledowski, C., Goebel, R., & Linden, D. E. (2007). Common neural substrates for visual working memory and attention. Neuroimage, 36(2), 441453. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayer, J. S., & Park, S. (2012). Working memory encoding and false memory in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder in a spatial delayed response task. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121(3), 784.10.1037/a0028836CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merigan, W. H., & Maunsell, J. H. (1993). How parallel are the primate visual pathways? Annual Review of Neuroscience, 16(1), 369402.10.1146/annurev.ne.16.030193.002101CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Page, C. E., & Coutellier, L. (2018). Reducing inhibition: A promising new strategy for the treatment of schizophrenia. EBioMedicine, 35, 2526.10.1016/j.ebiom.2018.07.043CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pashler, H. (1988). Familiarity and visual change detection. Perception & Psychophysics, 44(4), 369378.10.3758/BF03210419CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rouder, J. N., Morey, R. D., Morey, C. C., & Cowan, N. (2011). How to measure working memory capacity in the change detection paradigm. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(2), 324330.10.3758/s13423-011-0055-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saß, H., Wittchen, H.-U., Zaudig, M., & Houben, I. (2003). Dsm-iv-tr. Diagnostische Kriterien. Hogrefe, Göttingen.Google Scholar
Sawaki, R., & Luck, S. J. (2010). Capture versus suppression of attention by salient singletons: Electrophysiological evidence for an automatic attend-to-me signal. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 72(6), 14551470.10.3758/APP.72.6.1455CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaw, A. D., Knight, L., Freeman, T. C., Williams, G. M., Moran, R. J., Friston, K. J., … Singh, K. D. (2020). Oscillatory, computational, and behavioral evidence for impaired GABAergic inhibition in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 46(2), 345353.Google ScholarPubMed
Stablein, M., Storchak, H., Ghinea, D., Kraft, D., Knochel, C., Prvulovic, D., … Oertel-Knochel, V. (2018). Visual working memory encoding in schizophrenia and first-degree relatives: Neurofunctional abnormalities and impaired consolidation. Psychological Medicine, 49(1), 19. doi: 10.1017/S003329171800051X.Google ScholarPubMed
Vogel, E. K., McCollough, A. W., & Machizawa, M. G. (2005). Neural measures reveal individual differences in controlling access to working memory. Nature, 438(7067), 500.10.1038/nature04171CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolfe, J. M. (1994). Guided search 2.0 a revised model of visual search. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1(2), 202238.10.3758/BF03200774CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xie, W., & Zhang, W. (2017). Familiarity speeds up visual short-term memory consolidation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 43(6), 1207.Google ScholarPubMed
Xie, W., & Zhang, W. (2018). Familiarity speeds up visual short-term memory consolidation: Electrophysiological evidence from contralateral delay activities. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 30(1), 113.10.1162/jocn_a_01188CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, S., Xu, M., Kamigaki, T., Do, J. P. H., Chang, W.-C., Jenvay, S., … Dan, Y. (2014). Long-range and local circuits for top–down modulation of visual cortex processing. Science (New York, N.Y.), 345(6197), 660665.10.1126/science.1254126CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Participant characteristics of both groups including information regarding demographics, psychopathology and current medication.

Figure 1

Figure 1. Visual change detection task with four conditions; flickering/predictive cue, flickering/non-predictive cue, non-flickering/predictive cue, non-flickering/non-predictive cue. Flickering is indicated by white dashes around stimuli. The set size of four items was kept constant. In 80% of trials, a designated target stimulus was probed during retrieval (target trials). In 20% of trials, a distractor was probed during retrieval (catch trials). Only target trials are depicted in the figure.

Figure 2

Figure 2. Amount of information stored in VWM in target trials, estimated with Cowan's K in healthy control subjects = HCS and people with schizophrenia = PSZ. F/PC = flickering/predictive cue; NF/PC = non-flickering/predictive cue; F/NPC = flickering/non-predictive cue; NF/NPC = non-flickering/non-predictive cue. The main group effect of the LMM is expressed as the between group difference, and the within group comparisons are expressed via the post-hoc between group tests. Asterisks indicate significance p < 0.001 = ***, p < 0.01 = **, p < 0.05 = *. Error bars indicate standard deviation.

Figure 3

Table 2. Results of the linear mixed model investigating the effects of cue (predictive cue/non-predictive cue) and salience (flickering/non-flickering) in target trials in both PSZ and HCS with the covariates age and premorbid IQ.

Figure 4

Table 3. Results of post-hoc between and within group comparisons across all four task conditions

Figure 5

Table 4. Results of Spearman correlations (two-tailed) of independent WM capacity estimate (Pashler's K) and attentional prioritization efficiency (target Cowan's K – catch Cowan's K) across all four conditions, and within each condition

Figure 6

Table 5. Results of correlations between visual working memory capacity (Pashler's K) and mean Cowan's K of each target condition

Supplementary material: File

Barnes-Scheufler et al. supplementary material

Barnes-Scheufler et al. supplementary material
Download Barnes-Scheufler et al. supplementary material(File)
File 212.3 KB