Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-tf8b9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T15:49:58.494Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

COVID-19 and cognitive function: Evidence for increased processing speed variability in COVID-19 survivors and multifaceted impairment with long-COVID symptoms

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 May 2023

Krupa Vakani*
Affiliation:
Division of Psychology, Department of Life Sciences, College of Health, Medicine and Life Sciences, Brunel University London, Uxbridge, UK Centre for Cognitive and Clinical Neuroscience, College of Health, Medicine and Life Sciences, Brunel University London, Uxbridge, UK
Martina Ratto
Affiliation:
Being Well Group, Sheffield, UK
Anna Sandford-James
Affiliation:
The Scale Up Collective, London, UK
Elena Antonova
Affiliation:
Division of Psychology, Department of Life Sciences, College of Health, Medicine and Life Sciences, Brunel University London, Uxbridge, UK Centre for Cognitive and Clinical Neuroscience, College of Health, Medicine and Life Sciences, Brunel University London, Uxbridge, UK
Veena Kumari*
Affiliation:
Division of Psychology, Department of Life Sciences, College of Health, Medicine and Life Sciences, Brunel University London, Uxbridge, UK Centre for Cognitive and Clinical Neuroscience, College of Health, Medicine and Life Sciences, Brunel University London, Uxbridge, UK
*
Corresponding authors: Krupa Vakani and Veena Kumari; Emails: [email protected]; [email protected]
Corresponding authors: Krupa Vakani and Veena Kumari; Emails: [email protected]; [email protected]

Abstract

Background

There is increasing evidence for cognitive function to be negatively impacted by COVID-19. There is, however, limited research evaluating cognitive function pre- and post-COVID-19 using objective measures.

Methods

We examined processing speed, attention, working memory, executive function and memory in adults (≤69 years) with a history of COVID-19 (n = 129, none acutely unwell), compared to those with no known history of COVID-19 (n = 93). We also examined cognitive changes in a sub-group of COVID (n = 30) and non-COVID (n = 33) participants, compared to their pre-COVID-19 pandemic level.

Results

Cross-sectionally, the COVID group showed significantly larger intra-individual variability in processing speed, compared to the non-COVID group. The COVID sub-group also showed significantly larger intra-individual variability in processing speed, compared to their pre-COVID level; no significant change occurred in non-COVID participants over the same time scale. Other cognitive indices were not significantly impacted in the cross-sectional or within-subjects investigations, but participants (n = 20) who had needed hospitalisation due to COVID-19 showed poor attention and executive function relative to those who had not required hospitalisation (n = 109). Poor health and long-COVID symptoms correlated with poor cognitive function across domains in the COVID group.

Conclusions

The findings indicate a limited cognitive impact of COVID-19 with only intra-individual variability in processing speed being significantly impacted in an adult UK sample. However, those who required hospitalisation due to COVID-19 severity and/or experience long-COVID symptoms display multifaceted cognitive impairment and may benefit from repeated cognitive assessments and remediation efforts.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of the European Psychiatric Association

Introduction

A growing body of evidence indicates widespread brain and cognitive changes in people with a history of coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19), including those who did not show severe symptoms and did not require hospitalisation [Reference Najt, Richards and Fortune1, Reference Hampshire, Trender, Chamberlain, Jolly, Grant and Patrick2]. According to a systematic review [Reference Vanderlind, Rabinovitz, Miao, Oberlin, Bueno-Castellano and Fridman3], approximately 15–40% of COVID-19 survivors, compared to people without a history of COVID-19, show abnormal performance in one or more cognitive domain(s). More recent cross-sectional studies also indicate attention concentration [Reference Delgado-Alonso, Valles-Salgado, Delgado-Álvarez, Yus, Gómez-Ruiz and Jorquera4, Reference Henneghan, Lewis, Gill and Kesler5], processing speed [Reference Henneghan, Lewis, Gill and Kesler5], memory [Reference Guo, Benito Ballesteros, Yeung, Liu, Saha and Curtis6], visuospatial processing [Reference Delgado-Alonso, Valles-Salgado, Delgado-Álvarez, Yus, Gómez-Ruiz and Jorquera4], executive function [Reference Delgado-Alonso, Valles-Salgado, Delgado-Álvarez, Yus, Gómez-Ruiz and Jorquera4Reference Guo, Benito Ballesteros, Yeung, Liu, Saha and Curtis6] and general cognitive ability [Reference Hampshire, Trender, Chamberlain, Jolly, Grant and Patrick2] to be negatively impacted by COVID-19. Crivelli et al. [Reference Crivelli, Palmer, Calandri, Guekht, Beghi and Carroll7] in their review of 27 studies observed impaired attention, executive functions and memory in adults who had been assessed at some point, ranging from the acute phase to 7 months after the COVID-19 infection. Most of the existing studies with an objective assessment of cognitive function, however, have utilised cross-sectional designs and focused on adults in late adulthood (mean age across 27 studies = 56.05 years, [Reference Crivelli, Palmer, Calandri, Guekht, Beghi and Carroll7]) who may be particularly vulnerable to negative impacts of COVID-19 [Reference Matsui, Mitsuma, Nagata, Matsushita and Asahi8]. Furthermore, poor cognitive function itself has been linked to greater COVID-19 infection severity and mortality [Reference Batty, Deary and Gale9], raising the possibility that some of the COVID-19-related cognitive effects may be explained by pre-COVID-19 differences between COVID-19 and non-COVID groups.

The only study published to date (n = 785, age range: 51–81 years, Biobank cohort data, United Kingdom (UK) [Reference Douaud, Lee, Alfaro-Almagro, Arthofer, Wang and McCarthy10]) to use objective measures of cognitive function both pre- and post-COVID-19 reported a slight impairment in processing speed and executive function (as assessed by the Trail Making Test Trails A and B completion time, respectively) at 141 days, on average, from the COVID-19 diagnosis. There was no significant impact of COVID-19 history on eight other cognitive indices derived from six cognitive tests. Furthermore, many COVID-19 survivors report anxiety, depression, sleep difficulties and post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) [Reference Vanderlind, Rabinovitz, Miao, Oberlin, Bueno-Castellano and Fridman3, Reference Dai, Wang, Jiang, Li, Wang and Wu11, Reference Guo, Zheng, Shi, Wang, Li and Li12] which could cause or exacerbate cognitive difficulties reported by COVID-19 survivors. For many people, COVID-19 also has lasting effects, commonly referred to as long-COVID [Reference Mahase13]. In the UK, an estimated 1.9 million people have self-reported long-COVID symptoms at 4 weeks post-infection [14]. A large study (N = 236,379) reported neuropsychiatric diagnosis in 33.62% of patients 6-months post-infection, and this prevalence rate rose to 46.2% for patients who had received intensive care [Reference Taquet, Geddes, Husain, Luciano and Harrison15]. Although some of these consequences may be due to pre-existing medical and/or psychiatric conditions [Reference Levine, Sacktor and Becker16], it seems likely that COVID-19 itself results in short- and long-term neuropsychological symptoms for some people [Reference Butler, Pollak, Rooney, Michael and Nicholson17], and cognitive disruption may be more salient in association with long-COVID symptoms.

The main aims of the present study, therefore, were to examine: (i) the effects of COVID-19 history on cognitive function in the UK residents of working age (18–69 years); and (ii) the associations of long-COVID symptoms as well as physical and psychological well-being with cognitive function post COVID-19 diagnosis. To achieve these aims, we conducted a cross-sectional investigation of cognitive function and health in individuals with a confirmed COVID-19 diagnosis compared to those with no COVID-19 history (COVID and non-COVID groups, respectively) followed by a longitudinal investigation of participants in the COVID and non-COVID groups for whom pre-COVID-19 pandemic cognitive function data were available through an existing database. Based on the findings of previous reviews [Reference Vanderlind, Rabinovitz, Miao, Oberlin, Bueno-Castellano and Fridman3, Reference Crivelli, Palmer, Calandri, Guekht, Beghi and Carroll7], we expected multifaceted cognitive impairment, with the same cognitive indices being impacted by COVID-19 history in both the cross-sectional and longitudinal investigations. We further expected reduced physical and mental well-being in the COVID compared to the non-COVID group, and explored whether cognitive profiles associated with COVID-19 are explained, at least in part, by poor health and well-being. Lastly, we expected long-COVID symptoms to be associated with reduced cognitive function and poor well-being.

Methods

Participants and design

The cross-sectional investigation involved 222 adults (mean age = 38.70, SD = 12.08, range: 18–69): 129 with a COVID-19 diagnosis (COVID group) and 93 with no known/confirmed COVID-19 diagnosis (non-COVID group) (see Supplementary Table S1 for the demographic characteristics). The longitudinal investigation involved 63 of these 222 adults, who had pre-COVID-19 pandemic cognitive function data available via MyCognition [18]. Participants were recruited via social media platforms and MyCognition. Recruitment via MyCognition was conducted in two stages. First, a large group within the MyCognition database who had been assessed since 2017 (N = 2894) were invited to participate if they self-reported a confirmed COVID-19 diagnosis. An invitation to participate was then extended to adults with pre-COVID-19 cognitive data who self-reported no COVID-19 history. Participant testing period was March 2021–February 2022 for the COVID group and March 2021–March 2022 for the non-COVID group (recruitment of non-COVID participants stopped after the pandemic-related restrictions in the UK were fully lifted).

The study was approved by the University Research Ethics Committee (26518-A-Sep/2021–34167-1). All participants provided written consent and received £10 (Amazon voucher) for their time.

Measures and procedure

Demographic, physical and psychological well-being data were collected using self-report measures administered via Qualtrics (an online survey tool), taking ~45 min in total. The demographic items included age, sex, ethnicity, education, socio-economic status, existing mental and physical illnesses, and medication use. In addition, COVID participants were asked about their COVID-19 diagnosis, acute symptoms, subjective cognitive impairment (via a single question “Do you believe your cognitive functioning has been impacted due to your diagnosis of COVID-19?”) and chronic long-COVID symptoms at the time of participation. Cognitive data were collected via the MyCognition PRO mobile application, taking ~15 min.

Physical and psychological well-being

Physical and psychological well-being were assessed using three self-rated scales:

Short Form Health Survey-36 (SF-36) [Reference Ware and Sherbourne19]: SF-36 is a 36-item scale measuring physical, social and emotional functioning, and quality of life through eight dimensions: physical functioning, physical health, emotional problems, energy, emotional well-being, social functioning, pain and general health.

The Depression, Anxiety and Stress Scale-21 (DASS-21) [Reference Lovibond and Lovibond20]: DASS-21 is a 21-item scale assessing levels of depression (dysphoria, hopelessness, devaluation of life, self-deprecation, lack of interest, anhedonia, inertia), anxiety (autonomic arousal, skeletal muscle effects, situational anxiety, anxious affect) and stress (levels of chronic non-specific arousal such as problems with relaxation and emotional overactions).

Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI) [Reference Buysse, Reynolds, Monk, Berman and Kupfer21]: PSQI is a 19-item, four-point Likert scale assessing daytime dysfunction, use of sleeping medication, sleep disturbances, habitual sleep efficiency, sleep duration, sleep latency and subjective sleep score.

Cognitive function

Cognitive function was assessed online using a self-administered online assessment tool (MyCognition [MyCQ], https://www.mycognition.com/). The MyCQ tool comprises of digital versions of commonly utilised neuropsychological tests validated against the Cambridge Neuropsychological Automated Test Battery [Reference Domen, van de Weijer, Jaspers, Denys and Nieman22, Reference Reeson, Greenshaw, Agyapong, Hnatko, Pazderka and Polzin23] and assesses processing speed, attention, working memory, executive function and memory domains [Reference Bellens, Roelant, Sabbe, Peeters and van Dam24].

Processing speed was assessed using a Simple Reaction Time (RT) task, requiring participants to tap the circle button as quickly as possible when a red circle is presented on the screen (presentation time = 1 s, inter-stimulus interval = 3 s, 30 stimuli in total). Response accuracy (RA; % correct), average RT (ms) and intra-individual variability in RT were examined.

Attention was assessed using a Choice Reaction Time task, requiring participants to tap either the circle or triangle button depending on what shape is presented on the screen. There are 30 trials in total, and each stimulus (circle or triangle) is presented for 1 s, with a 3 s inter-stimulus interval. RA (% correct) and average RT (ms) for correct answers were examined.

Working Memory was assessed using the 2-Back task. Participants are asked to tap “Yes” or “No” depending on whether the picture presented to them on the screen (household objects, food and drink items) matches the picture shown two screens back (50 trials in total). RA (% correct) was used to index task performance.

Executive Function was assessed using the Trail-Making B task, requiring tapping a number and a letter in an ascending and alphabetical order, respectively, to produce an alternating sequence (e.g., 1, A, 2, B). The task has 25 trials (13 numbers, 12 letters). RA (% correct moves) and total task completion time (ms) were examined.

Memory was assessed using a Visual Recognition Memory task. Participants are presented with a set of 24 pictures (each picture for 2 s, inter-stimulus interval = 1 s) and instructed to remember them. They are then presented with 96 pictures, including 24 pictures presented earlier, and asked to tap either “Yes” or “No” depending on whether they remember seeing the picture earlier. RA (% correct) was used to index task performance.

Statistical analysis

For the cross-sectional investigation, we first compared the COVID and non-COVID groups on age and body mass index (BMI) (separately) using a 2 (Group: COVID, non-COVID) × 2 (Sex: Males, Females) analysis of variance (ANOVA). Group differences in each of the health and cognitive variables were examined using a 2 (Group) × 2 (Sex) ANOVA, followed by 2 (Group) × 2 (Sex) analyses of co-variance (ANCOVA), covarying for age, given that the COVID group, on average, was found to be older than the non-COVID group (Table 1). For the two cognitive variables showing a significant Group effect (see Results), further (exploratory) ANOVAs were run with Ethnicity (White British vs. all other ethnicities) included as an additional between-subjects factor. Any significant interactions were followed up with post hoc comparisons using paired or independent sample t-tests as appropriate. Effect sizes, where reported, are partial eta squared (ηp 2; the proportion of variance associated with a factor). In the COVID group, the relationships of cognitive variables with the overall long COVID-19 symptom load (a sum total of individual symptom ratings) were examined using Pearson’s correlations, and with each of the long-COVID symptoms (rated 0–7) explored using Spearman rank order correlations. Pearson’s correlations were also used to explore the relationships between all cognitive variables and the physical and mental health measures in the entire sample, and in the COVID and non-COVID groups separately.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics and group differences (ANOVA and ANCOVA results) in the demographic, mental health and well-being measures for the cross-sectional investigation

SF-36 (Short Form Health Survey-36): The response ranges between two- and six-point ordered Likert scales. Raw scores are transformed to produce a score between 0 and 100 for each dimension. The higher the score the better the overall health (19.47). Internal reliability in this sample: overall scale, Cronbach’s a = 0.96; all subscales, Cronbach’s a > 0.8, except a = 0.74 for Social Functioning.

DASS-21 (The Depression, Anxiety and Stress Scale-21): Each item is rated by participants on a four-point scale according to how often in the past week it applied to them, ranging from “Did not apply to me at all” (0) to “Applied to me very much or most of the time” (3). Higher scores indicate higher levels (severity) of symptoms. Internal reliability in this sample: Depression, Cronbach’s a = 0.93; Anxiety, Cronbach’s a = 0.82; Stress, Cronbach’s a =0.88.

PSQI (Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index): Participants answer the PSQI questions by relating them to their past month (21). Each component is scored between “No difficulty” (0) to “Severe difficulty” (3) and tallied up to yield a total score (range 0–21). Higher scores indicate poor sleep quality. Internal reliability in this sample: Global score, Cronbach’s a = 0.76.

a Sample size reduced by 2 (non-COVID).

b Sample size reduced by 1 (COVID).

c Sample size reduced by 7 (5 COVID, 2 non-COVID).

d Sample size reduced by 2 (COVID).

For the longitudinal (within-subjects) investigation, the COVID and non-COVID groups were compared on age and BMI using independent sample t-tests (sex not analysed due to relatively small number of males). The effect of COVID-19 diagnosis on each of the cognitive variables was then examined using a 2 (Group: COVID, non-COVID) × 2 (Time: Pre-COVID, Post-COVID) ANOVA with Group as a between-subjects and Time as a within-subject factor. Given that poor cognitive function has been linked to more severe acute COVID-19 and hospitalisation [Reference Batty, Deary and Gale9], the relationship between the overall long-COVID symptom load and pre-pandemic cognitive data in the longitudinal COVID sub-sample (n = 29) was also examined using Pearson’s correlations.

All analyses were performed using the Statistical Package for Social Sciences (for Windows, version 28; IBM, New York, NY). The data distribution on all variables met the assumptions of parametric statistical procedures. Alpha level for testing the significance of effects was maintained at p ≤ 0.05.

Results

Cross-sectional investigation

Sample characteristics

The majority of participants in both the COVID (n = 129) and non-COVID (n = 93) groups were White British, held a Bachelor’s degree or above and were in some form of employment. The COVID group was, on average, significantly older (Table 1), and had more people with at least one physical health problem (n = 58; 44.96%; most commonly related to lungs), compared to the non-COVID group (n = 21; 22.58%). Of various mental health conditions, anxiety, depression and insomnia were most commonly reported by both groups (Supplementary Table S1).

Within the COVID group, 20 participants (15.5%) had been hospitalised (Supplementary Tables S2 and S3). The most prevalent acute symptom (recalled retrospectively) was a high temperature (76.7%). At study entry (mean number of days since diagnosis = 263, SD = 192.16, range:20–714), a large proportion of the sample reported subjective cognitive impairment (78.3%), reduced psychological well-being (77.5%), and one or more long-COVID symptoms, most commonly exhaustion/fatigue (88.4%). The overall long-COVID symptom load, however, was not significantly correlated with the number of days since diagnosis [r(125) = 0.057, p = 0.527] or age [r(126) = 0.092, p = 0.299]. Nineteen of 20 hospitalised participants (95%) reported subjective cognitive impairment, and 18 (90%) reported reduced psychological well-being.

Mental health and psychological well-being in COVID versus non-COVID participants

There were significant main effects of Group in ANOVA analyses (Table 1), with the COVID group having significantly poorer health (SF-36), higher anxiety (DASS-21), and lower sleep quality (PSQI), compared to the non-COVID group. The ANCOVA analyses, with age as a covariate, retained these effects and, in addition, indicated significantly higher depression and stress levels (DASS-21) in the COVID, compared to the non-COVID group (Table 1).

There were significant sex differences in physical functioning, emotional problems, energy, emotional well-being, pain and general health (SF-36), stress (DASS-21), as well as sleep disturbance and daytime dysfunction (PSQI), indicating poorer health and psychological well-being in females compared to males. There were, however, no significant Group × Sex interactions (Table 1), except for sleep medication [in females, greater use of sleep medications by COVID compared to non-COVID group, t(163) = 3.65, p < 0.001].

Lastly, age was a significant covariate (in ANCOVAs) for BMI, emotional well-being (SF-36), depression, anxiety and stress (DASS-21), as well as sleep efficiency and daytime dysfunction (PSQI), indicating poorer health and psychological well-being with older age.

Cognitive function in COVID versus non-COVID participants

There were significant main effects of Group indicating significantly greater intra-individual variability in processing speed (p = 0.015) and lower attention RA (p = 0.022) in the COVID compared to non-COVID group (Table 2). The Group effect remained significant for processing speed variability (p = 0.034) but lost formal significance for attention RA (p = 0.052) when covarying for age, with ANCOVAs additionally revealing longer RTs being associated with older age (Table 2). Ethnicity did not show any main or interactive effects (Table 2).

Table 2. Descriptive statistics and group differences (ANOVA and ANCOVA results) in cognitive measures for the cross-sectional investigation

Note: Further (exploratory) analyses of processing speed RT variability and attention response accuracy (%) with Ethnicity (White British vs. All Other Ethnicities) included as an additional between-subjects factor retained the main effects of Group (processing speed RT variability, p = 0.006; attention RA, p = 0.042) but yielded no significant main or interactive effects involving Ethnicity (all p values > 0.341).

a Sample size reduced by 12 (10 COVID, 2 non-COVID).

b Sample size reduced by 17 (13 COVID, 4 non-COVID).

c Sample size reduced by 5 (2 COVID, 3 non-COVID).

d Sample size reduced by 3 (non-COVID).

e Sample size reduced by 2 (COVID).

Participants who had been hospitalised had longer attention RTs (p = 0.005) and lower executive function RA (%) (p = 0.012) than those who did not require hospitalisation (Table 3). These effects remained significant when covarying for age, despite older age being associated with poorer performance on both measures.

Table 3. Descriptive statistics and group differences between COVID hospitalised versus non-hospitalised sample (ANOVA and ANCOVA results) in cognitive measures for the cross-sectional investigation

a Sample size reduced by 10 (1 hospitalised, 9 non-hospitalised).

b Sample size reduced by 13 (1 hospitalised, 12 non-hospitalised).

c Sample size reduced by 2 (non-hospitalised).

Association between cognitive functions and long-COVID symptoms

Executive function task completion time, the RTs during processing speed and attention tasks, and attention RA variables were most commonly correlated, with small-to-medium effect sizes, with individual long-COVID symptoms, especially arrythmia, chest pain and headaches (Supplementary Table S4). The overall long-COVID symptom load was significantly associated with poor performance on all tasks, with small-to-medium-sized correlations (Table 4).

Table 4. Associations (Pearson correlation coefficients) between the cognitive variables and the total long COVID-19 symptom load in the COVID participants

Association between cognitive function, mental health and well-being

SF-36 dimensions correlated with many cognitive variables, especially executive function, with poorer health being associated with poorer performance (Table 5). Depression, anxiety and stress correlated negatively with executive function. Sleep disturbance was associated with poor performance in processing speed, attention and executive function. These associations were generally stronger in the COVID, relative to non-COVID, group (Table 5).

Table 5. Associations (Pearson correlation coefficients) of the cognitive variables with health and well-being measures for the cross-sectional investigation

Abbreviations: COV+, COVID group; COV−, Non-COVID group; DASS-21, The Depression, Anxiety and Stress Scale-21; PSQI, Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index; SF-36, Short Form Health Survey-36.

a Sample size reduced by 1.

b Sample size reduced by 7.

c Sample size reduced by 2.

d Sample size reduced by 5.

Longitudinal (within-subjects) investigation

Sample characteristics

The sub-sample for whom pre-COVID cognitive data were available had similar sample characteristics as the whole sample (Supplementary Table S1).

Pre- versus post-COVID-19 cognitive function

In line with the cross-sectional findings, there was a significant Group × Time interaction in RT variability in processing speed (p = 0.043; Table 6), explained by greater intra-individual variability in the COVID group post-COVID-19 diagnosis (but not acutely unwell) compared to their pre-pandemic scores [t(28)=2.01, p = 0.05]; there was no such change in the non-COVID group [t(30)=0.75, p = 0.461]. Additionally, there were main effects of Time on attention task RTs (p = 0.001) (shorter RTs the second time) and working memory RA (p = 0.033) (slightly higher the second time), most likely explained by practice-related effects.

Table 6. Descriptive statistics and changes from pre-pandemic assessment (ANOVA results) in cognitive measures for the longitudinal investigation (sub-sample with pre-pandemic cognitive data)

a Sample size reduced by 4 (1 COVID, 3 non-COVID).

b Sample size reduced by 6 (2 COVID, 4 non-COVID).

c Sample size reduced by 5 (1 COVID, 4 non-COVID).

d Sample size reduced by 1 (non-COVID).

Association between cognitive function and long-COVID symptoms

The higher overall long-COVID symptom load correlated with poorer executive function performance at study entry (post-Covid), as well as poorer executive function and memory in pre-pandemic cognitive data (see Table 4).

Discussion

The main aims of this study were to examine the impact of COVID-19 on cognitive function in a UK adult sample (≤69 years), and explore the roles of physical and mental health and long-COVID symptoms in cognitive function in these individuals. The findings showed: (i) significantly larger intra-individual variability in processing speed but no significant impact of COVID-19 on other cognitive measures in our cross-sectional investigation, and a further confirmation of a negative impact of COVID-19 on processing speed variability (but no other cognitive variables) in our within-subjects investigation (pre-pandemic vs. post-COVID-19 diagnosis); (ii) poorer attention and executive function in the COVID-19 group participants who had needed hospitalisation due to COVID-19 relative to those who had not; and (iii) medium-sized negative associations of cognitive performance on all tasks with physical health and long-COVID symptoms (and relatively fewer associations of cognitive variables with anxiety and depression) in the COVID-19 group in the cross-sectional investigation, and (iv) medium-sized negative association between pre-pandemic cognitive function (executive function, memory) and long-COVID symptoms in the longitudinal sample. Further noteworthy findings were: poorer physical and mental health in the COVID relative to non-COVID group; generally reduced psychological well-being in females, relative to males; and longer RTs with increasing age.

Concerning the cognitive impact of COVID-19, our findings indicated only a limited negative impact of COVID-19 history on cognitive function in UK adults <70 years, with only processing speed variability being impacted (with a small effect size) out of nine cognitive function indices examined in both the cross-sectional and longitudinal investigations. This is consistent with the findings of the UK Biobank data-based study [Reference Douaud, Lee, Alfaro-Almagro, Arthofer, Wang and McCarthy10] which found significant effects of COVID-19 on only 2 (Trails A and B completion time) out of 10 cognitive function indices examined. We did not detect the impact of COVID-19 as “slower speed” (including Trails B completion time) but rather a “more variable speed” on a task where speed was emphasised. Given that Douaud et al. [Reference Douaud, Lee, Alfaro-Almagro, Arthofer, Wang and McCarthy10] did not examine/report intra-individual variability in processing speed, the findings of their and our study combined suggest that intra-individual variability in speed might be relatively more sensitive to COVID-19 in people aged ≤69 years, since our sample had a wider age range of younger adults (18–69) compared to Douaud et al. [Reference Douaud, Lee, Alfaro-Almagro, Arthofer, Wang and McCarthy10] study’s age range (51–81 years). Elevated intra-individual variability in RTs, reflecting momentary lapses in attention and/or task-irrelevant cognitions and a neural dysfunction involving multiple networks [Reference Lin and McDonough25, Reference Weissman, Roberts, Visscher and Woldorff26], has also been shown to be a particularly sensitive measure in the context of ageing [Reference Dykiert, Der, Starr and Deary27, Reference Bielak, Cherbuin, Bunce and Anstey28], prediction of future cognitive outcomes [Reference Haynes, Bauermeister and Bunce29], and various neurodegenerative diseases [Reference Costa, Dogan, Schulz and Reetz30, Reference de Frias, Dixon and Camicioli31]. A positive consideration here is that there may be scope for improving/reducing variability in processing speed using continuous cognitive training [Reference Simpson, Camfield, Pipingas, Macpherson and Stough32] or mindfulness-based approaches [Reference Antonova, Schlosser, Pandey and Kumari33], given findings of a more stable performance in long-term meditators compared to meditation-naïve individuals [Reference Kumari, Antonova, Wright, Hamid, Hernandez and Schmechtig34], as well as other reports of improved processing speed following mindfulness practice [Reference Moore and Malinowski35, Reference Slagter, Lutz, Greischar, Francis, Nieuwenhuis and Davis36].

Against the backdrop of a limited general cognitive impact of COVID-19, our findings suggest reduced cognitive function across multiple domains in people who needed hospitalisation due to COVID-19. This is consistent with recent literature suggesting that brain and cognitive impairment may be more salient in people with a severe infection [Reference He, Yuan, Dang, Bai, Yang and Wang37] or hospitalisation [Reference Tassignon, Radwan, Blommaert, Stas, Allard and De Ridder38], highlighting the need for longitudinal cognitive monitoring and improvement efforts in such cohorts [Reference Davis, McCorkell, Vogel and Topol39], for example using non-invasive brain stimulation [Reference Linnhoff, Koehler, Haghikia and Zaehle40, Reference Santana, França, Sato, Silva, Queiroz and de Farias41]. Furthermore, we found sizable associations between overall long-COVID symptom load and cognitive function across all domains, suggesting that affected individuals may also benefit from longitudinal cognitive monitoring and rehabilitation efforts. Interestingly, further to previous literature linking poorer cognitive function to greater acute COVID-19 severity and hospitalisation [Reference Batty, Deary and Gale9], our finding suggests that poorer cognitive function (executive function, memory) may also be a precursor of long-COVID symptoms. Taken together, our findings indicate that at least a part of COVID-19-related cognitive impairment in cross-sectional studies may reflect reduced pre-infection cognitive level (for various other reasons); and that the most robust short-to-medium impact of COVID-19 may be limited to a more variable processing speed. Follow-up assessments of our and other samples are crucially needed to fully chart the cognitive trajectory of COVID-19 and long-COVID.

Our further findings confirmed poorer mental health and well-being in COVID-19 survivors [Reference Vanderlind, Rabinovitz, Miao, Oberlin, Bueno-Castellano and Fridman3, Reference Vindegaard and Benros42] and suggest that this may continue for some time post-infection. In addition, sex differences (across groups) were observed with women reporting poorer mental health and well-being, which is also in line with previous literature on sex differences in affective disorders [Reference Altemus, Sarvaiya and Neill Epperson43]. Lastly, our finding of longer RTs with increasing age is consistent with previous literature [Reference Madden, Birren and Schaie44].

The limitations of the present study include: (i) most participants being White-British, limiting generalisability of the findings; (ii) significantly older participants, on average, in the COVID than non-COVID groups (due to the open recruitment strategy), though all COVID-related effects were sustained when covarying for age; and (iii) a reliance on self-report for COVID-19 diagnosis, which cannot rule out that at least some non-COVID group participants may have been pre-symptomatic when assessed. Future research should include more ethnically-diverse samples with consideration to the impact of socio-economic factors and, importantly, assess cognitive function at numerous times post-infection to understand the potential long-term cognitive recovery, especially in association with varying levels of long-COVID symptoms [Reference Liu, Chen, Wang, Wang, Jiang and Yang45, Reference Hall, Ayaz, Meng, Hudson, Sakib and Quah46].

Conclusions

We observed a limited cognitive impact of COVID-19 with only intra-individual variability in processing speed being significantly affected (becoming less stable) in an adult UK sample. However, those who required COVID-19-related hospitalisation, did display multifaceted cognitive impairment. Furthermore, long-COVID symptoms were associated with reduced cognitive function (assessed post-COVID-19 diagnosis) but also with poorer executive function and memory prior to the COVID-19 pandemic, suggesting that poorer cognitive function may be a precursor of long-COVID symptoms. Further research is required to understand whether COVID-19 and long-COVID continue to impede cognitive function over a longer period of time.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1192/j.eurpsy.2023.25.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank the participants for their contribution to this research and dedicate this work to the memory of (late) Dr Keiron Sparrowhawk.

Financial support

This research was funded by the British Academy (SRG21\211061).

Author contribution

Conceptualization: K.V., V.K.; Data curation: K.V., A.S-J.; Formal analysis: K.V., E.A., V.K.; Funding acquisition: E.A., V.K.; Investigation: K.V., V.K.; Methodology: K.V., M.R., E.A., V.K.; Project administration: K.V., V.K.; Resources: M.R., A.S-J., V.K.; Supervision: E.A., V.K.; Writing—original draft: K.V.; Writing—review & editing: M.R., A.S-J., E.A., V.K.

Competing interest

M.R. works for Beingwell Group, Sheffield, United Kingdom. No conflicts of interest are reported by other authors.

References

Najt, P, Richards, HL, Fortune, DG. Brain imaging in patients with COVID-19: a systematic review. Brain Behav Immun Health. 2021;16:100290. doi:10.1016/j.bbih.2021.100290.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hampshire, A, Trender, W, Chamberlain, SR, Jolly, AE, Grant, JE, Patrick, F, et al. Cognitive deficits in people who have recovered from COVID-19. EClinical Med. 2021;39:101044. doi:10.1016/j.eclinm.2021.101044.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vanderlind, WM, Rabinovitz, BB, Miao, IY, Oberlin, LE, Bueno-Castellano, C, Fridman, C, et al. A systematic review of neuropsychological and psychiatric sequalae of COVID-19: implications for treatment. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2021;34(4):420–33. doi:10.1097/YCO.0000000000000713.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delgado-Alonso, C, Valles-Salgado, M, Delgado-Álvarez, A, Yus, M, Gómez-Ruiz, N, Jorquera, M, et al. Cognitive dysfunction associated with COVID-19: a comprehensive neuropsychological study. J Psychiatr Res. 2022;150:40–6. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2022.03.033.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henneghan, AM, Lewis, KA, Gill, E, Kesler, . Cognitive impairment in non-critical, mild-to-moderate COVID-19 survivors. Front Psychol. 2022;13:770459. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2022.770459.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guo, P, Benito Ballesteros, A, Yeung, SP, Liu, R, Saha, A, Curtis, L, et al. COVCOG 2: cognitive and memory deficits in long COVID: a second publication from the COVID and cognition study. Front Aging Neurosci. 2022;14:804937. doi:10.3389/fnagi.2022.804937.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crivelli, L, Palmer, K, Calandri, I, Guekht, A, Beghi, E, Carroll, W, et al. Changes in cognitive functioning after COVID‐19: a systematic review and meta‐analysis. Alzheimers Dement. 2022;18(5):1047–66. doi:10.1002/alz.12644.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matsui, T, Mitsuma, S, Nagata, A, Matsushita, S, Asahi, T. Accelerated cognitive decline after the COVID-19 pandemic in a community population of older persons with cognitive impairment: A 4-year time series analysis in the Tokyo Metropolis area. Geriatr Gerontol Int. 2023;23(3):200–4. doi:10.1111/ggi.14543.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Batty, GD, Deary, IJ, Gale, CR. Pre-pandemic cognitive function and COVID-19 mortality: prospective cohort study. Eur J Epidemiol. 2021;36(5):559–64. doi:10.1007/s10654-021-00743-7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Douaud, G, Lee, S, Alfaro-Almagro, F, Arthofer, C, Wang, C, McCarthy, P, et al. SARS-CoV-2 is associated with changes in brain structure in UK biobank. Nature. 2022;604(7907):697707. doi:10.1038/s41586-022-04569-5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dai, LL, Wang, X, Jiang, TC, Li, PF, Wang, Y, Wu, SJ, et al. Anxiety and depressive symptoms among COVID-19 patients in Jianghan Fangcang shelter Hospital in Wuhan, China. PLoS One. 2020;15(8):e0238416. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0238416.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guo, Q, Zheng, Y, Shi, J, Wang, J, Li, G, Li, C, et al. Immediate psychological distress in quarantined patients with COVID-19 and its association with peripheral inflammation: a mixed-method study. Brain Behav Immun. 2020;88:1727. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2020.05.038.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mahase, E. Covid-19: what do we know about “long covid”? BMJ. 2020;370:m2815. doi:10.1136/bmj.m2815.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Office for National Statistics. Prevalence of ongoing symptoms following coronavirus (COVID-19) infection in the UK: 30 March 2023 – Office for National Statistics [Internet] [cited 2022 Apr 11], https://www.ons.gov.uk/peoplepopulationandcommunity/healthandsocialcare/conditionsanddiseases/bulletins/prevalenceofongoingsymptomsfollowingcoronaviruscovid19infectionintheuk/3november2022; 2023 [accessed 11 April 2022].Google Scholar
Taquet, M, Geddes, JR, Husain, M, Luciano, S, Harrison, PJ. 6-month neurological and psychiatric outcomes in 236 379 survivors of COVID-19: a retrospective cohort study using electronic health records. Lancet Psychiatry. 2021;8(5):416–27. doi:10.1016/S2215-0366(21)00084-5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levine, A, Sacktor, N, Becker, JT. Studying the neuropsychological sequelae of SARS-CoV-2: lessons learned from 35 years of neuroHIV research. J Neurovirol. 2020;26(6):809–23. doi:10.1007/s13365-020-00897-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butler, M, Pollak, TA, Rooney, AG, Michael, BD, Nicholson, TR. Neuropsychiatric complications of COVID-19. BMJ. 2020;371:m3871. doi:10.1136/bmj.m3871.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MyCognition. Wellbeing | MyCognition | England [Internet]. What is MyCognition? [cited 2023 Feb 16], https://www.mycognition.com; 2023 [accessed 16 February 2023].Google Scholar
Ware, JE, Sherbourne, CD. The MOS 36-item short-form health survey (SF-36). I. Conceptual framework and item selection. Med Care. 1992;30(6):473–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lovibond, SH, Lovibond, PF. Manual for the depression anxiety stress scales. 2nd ed. Sydney: Psychology Foundation; 1995, p. 42 (Psychology Foundation Monograph).Google Scholar
Buysse, DJ, Reynolds, CF, Monk, TH, Berman, SR, Kupfer, DJ. The Pittsburgh sleep quality index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. 1989;28(2):193213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Domen, AC, van de Weijer, SCF, Jaspers, MW, Denys, D, Nieman, DH. The validation of a new online cognitive assessment tool: the MyCognition quotient. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. 2019;28(3):e1775. doi:10.1002/mpr.1775.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reeson, M, Greenshaw, A, Agyapong, V, Hnatko, G, Pazderka, H, Polzin, W, et al. Cognitive improvements in child sexual abuse victims occur following multimodal treatment program: as measured by MyCognition quotient. J Child Adolesc Behav. 2019;7(2):386.Google Scholar
Bellens, A, Roelant, E, Sabbe, B, Peeters, M, van Dam, PA. Evaluation of a new online cognitive assessment tool in breast cancer survivors with cognitive impairment: a prospective cohort study. Support Care Cancer. 2022;30(1):2131. doi:10.1007/s00520-021-06397-1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lin, SSH, McDonough, IM. Intra-individual cognitive variability in neuropsychological assessment: a sign of neural network dysfunction. Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 2022;29(3):375–99. doi:10.1080/13825585.2021.2021134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weissman, DH, Roberts, KC, Visscher, KM, Woldorff, MG. The neural bases of momentary lapses in attention. Nat Neurosci. 2006;9(7):971–8. doi:10.1038/nn1727.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dykiert, D, Der, G, Starr, JM, Deary, IJ. Age differences in intra-individual variability in simple and choice reaction time: systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS One. 2012;7(10):e45759. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0045759.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bielak, AAM, Cherbuin, N, Bunce, D, Anstey, KJ. Intraindividual variability is a fundamental phenomenon of aging: Evidence from an 8-year longitudinal study across young, middle, and older adulthood. Dev Psychol. 2014;50(1):143–51. doi:10.1037/a0032650.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haynes, BI, Bauermeister, S, Bunce, D. A systematic review of longitudinal associations between reaction time intraindividual variability and age-related cognitive decline or impairment, dementia, and mortality. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2017;23(5):431–45. doi:10.1017/S1355617717000236.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costa, AS, Dogan, I, Schulz, JB, Reetz, K. Going beyond the mean: intraindividual variability of cognitive performance in prodromal and early neurodegenerative disorders. Clin Neuropsychol. 2019;33(2):369–89. doi:10.1080/13854046.2018.1533587.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Frias, CM, Dixon, RA, Camicioli, R. Neurocognitive speed and inconsistency in Parkinson’s disease with and without incipient dementia: an 18-month prospective cohort study. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2012;18(4):764–72. doi:10.1017/S1355617712000422.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simpson, T, Camfield, D, Pipingas, A, Macpherson, H, Stough, C. Improved processing speed: online computer-based cognitive training in older adults. Educ Gerontol. 2012;38(7):445–58. doi:10.1080/03601277.2011.559858.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Antonova, E, Schlosser, K, Pandey, R, Kumari, V. Coping with COVID-19: mindfulness-based approaches for mitigating mental health crisis. Front Psych. 2021;12:563417. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2021.563417.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kumari, V, Antonova, E, Wright, B, Hamid, A, Hernandez, EM, Schmechtig, A, et al. The mindful eye: smooth pursuit and saccadic eye movements in meditators and non-meditators. Conscious Cogn. 2017;48:6675. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2016.10.008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moore, A, Malinowski, P. Meditation, mindfulness and cognitive flexibility. Conscious Cogn. 2009;18(1):176–86. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2008.12.008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slagter, HA, Lutz, A, Greischar, LL, Francis, AD, Nieuwenhuis, S, Davis, JM, et al. Mental training affects distribution of limited brain resources. PLoS Biol. 2007;5(6):e138. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0050138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
He, D, Yuan, M, Dang, W, Bai, L, Yang, R, Wang, J, et al. Long term neuropsychiatric consequences in COVID-19 survivors: cognitive impairment and inflammatory underpinnings fifteen months after discharge. Asian J Psychiatry. 2023;80:103409. doi:10.1016/j.ajp.2022.103409.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tassignon, B, Radwan, A, Blommaert, J, Stas, L, Allard, SD, De Ridder, F, et al. Longitudinal changes in global structural brain connectivity and cognitive performance in former hospitalized COVID-19 survivors: an exploratory study. Exp Brain Res. 2023;241(3):727–41. doi:10.1007/s00221-023-06545-5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, HE, McCorkell, L, Vogel, JM, Topol, EJ. Long COVID: major findings, mechanisms and recommendations. Nat Rev Microbiol. 2023;21:133–46. doi:10.1038/s41579-022-00846-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Linnhoff, S, Koehler, L, Haghikia, A, Zaehle, T. The therapeutic potential of non-invasive brain stimulation for the treatment of long-COVID-related cognitive fatigue. Front Immunol. 2023;13:935614. doi:10.3389/fimmu.2022.935614.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Santana, K, França, E, Sato, J, Silva, A, Queiroz, M, de Farias, J, et al. Non-invasive brain stimulation for fatigue in post-acute sequelae of SARS-CoV-2 (PASC). Brain Stimulat. 2023;16(1):100–7. doi:10.1016/j.brs.2023.01.1672.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vindegaard, N, Benros, ME. COVID-19 pandemic and mental health consequences: systematic review of the current evidence. Brain Behav Immun. 2020;89:531–42. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2020.05.048.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Altemus, M, Sarvaiya, N, Neill Epperson, C. Sex differences in anxiety and depression clinical perspectives. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2014;35(3):320–30. doi:10.1016/j.yfrne.2014.05.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Madden, DJ. Speed and timing of behavioural processes. In: Birren, JE, Schaie, KW, editors. Handbook of the psychology of aging. 5th ed. San Diego: Academic Press; 2001, p. 288312.Google Scholar
Liu, YH, Chen, Y, Wang, QH, Wang, LR, Jiang, L, Yang, Y, et al. One-year trajectory of cognitive changes in older survivors of COVID-19 in Wuhan, China: a longitudinal cohort study. JAMA Neurol. 2022;79(5):509. doi:10.1001/jamaneurol.2022.0461.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hall, PA, Ayaz, H, Meng, G, Hudson, A, Sakib, MN, Quah, ACK, et al. Neurocognitive and psychiatric symptoms following infection with COVID-19: evidence from laboratory and population studies. Brain Behav Immun – Health. 2023;28:100595. doi:10.1016/j.bbih.2023.100595.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptive statistics and group differences (ANOVA and ANCOVA results) in the demographic, mental health and well-being measures for the cross-sectional investigation

Figure 1

Table 2. Descriptive statistics and group differences (ANOVA and ANCOVA results) in cognitive measures for the cross-sectional investigation

Figure 2

Table 3. Descriptive statistics and group differences between COVID hospitalised versus non-hospitalised sample (ANOVA and ANCOVA results) in cognitive measures for the cross-sectional investigation

Figure 3

Table 4. Associations (Pearson correlation coefficients) between the cognitive variables and the total long COVID-19 symptom load in the COVID participants

Figure 4

Table 5. Associations (Pearson correlation coefficients) of the cognitive variables with health and well-being measures for the cross-sectional investigation

Figure 5

Table 6. Descriptive statistics and changes from pre-pandemic assessment (ANOVA results) in cognitive measures for the longitudinal investigation (sub-sample with pre-pandemic cognitive data)

Supplementary material: File

Vakani et al. supplementary material

Tables S1-S4

Download Vakani et al. supplementary material(File)
File 35.6 KB
Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.