Hostname: page-component-5cf477f64f-qls9x Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-04-06T00:30:57.878Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Longitudinal associations of naturalistic psychedelic use with psychotic and manic symptoms

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  31 March 2025

Otto Simonsson*
Affiliation:
Department of Neurobiology, Care Sciences and Society, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden Center for Healthy Minds, University of Wisconsin – Madison, Madison, WI, USA
Simon B. Goldberg
Affiliation:
Center for Healthy Minds, University of Wisconsin – Madison, Madison, WI, USA Department of Counseling Psychology, University of Wisconsin – Madison, Madison, WI, USA
Walter Osika
Affiliation:
Department of Neurobiology, Care Sciences and Society, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
Cecilia U. D. Stenfors
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Stockholm University, Stockholm, Sweden
Sankalp Chaturvedi
Affiliation:
Department of Management and Entrepreneurship, Imperial College London, London, UK
Caroline M. Swords
Affiliation:
Center for Healthy Minds, University of Wisconsin – Madison, Madison, WI, USA Department of Counseling Psychology, University of Wisconsin – Madison, Madison, WI, USA
Jayanth Narayanan
Affiliation:
Department of Management & Organizational Development, Northeastern University, Boston, MA, USA
Peter S. Hendricks
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neurobiology, University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham, AL, USA
*
Corresponding Author: Otto Simonsson; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Findings from contemporary clinical trials suggest that psychedelics are generally safe and may be effective in the treatment of various psychiatric disorders. However, less is known about the risks associated with psychedelic use outside of medically supervised contexts, particularly in populations that are typically excluded from participation in clinical trials.

Methods

Using a preregistered longitudinal observational research design with a purposive sample of US residents between 18 and 50 years old (N=21,990), we investigated associations between self-reported naturalistic psychedelic use and psychotic and manic symptoms, with emphasis on those with psychiatric histories of schizophrenia or bipolar I disorder.

Results

The follow-up survey was completed by 12,345 participants (56% retention), with 505 participants reporting psychedelic use during the 2-month study period. In covariate-adjusted regression models, psychedelic use during the study period was associated with increases in the severity of psychotic and manic symptoms. However, such increases were only observed for those who reported psychedelic use in an illegal context. While increases in the severity of psychotic symptoms appeared to depend on the frequency of use and the intensity of challenging psychedelic experiences, increases in the severity of manic symptoms appeared to be moderated by a personal history of schizophrenia or bipolar I disorder and the subjective experience of insight during a psychedelic experience.

Conclusions

The findings suggest that naturalistic psychedelic use specifically in illegal contexts may lead to increases in the severity of psychotic and manic symptoms. Such increases may depend on the frequency of use, the acute subjective psychedelic experience, and psychiatric history.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

There has been a resurgence of scientific interest in psychedelics as an adjunct to psychotherapy (i.e. psychedelic-assisted therapy) for treating various mental health conditions (Nutt & Carhart-Harris, Reference Nutt and Carhart-Harris2021). For example, recent studies indicate that psychedelic-assisted therapy is associated with reductions in depressive symptoms in patients with major depressive disorder or treatment-resistant depression (Goodwin et al., Reference Goodwin, Aaronson, Alvarez, Arden, Baker, Bennett and Malievskaia2022; Raison et al., Reference Raison, Sanacora, Woolley, Heinzerling, Dunlop, Brown and Griffiths2023) and reductions in anxiety symptoms in patients with life-threatening cancer (Griffiths et al., Reference Griffiths, Johnson, Carducci, Umbricht, Richards, Richards and Klinedinst2016; Ross et al., Reference Ross, Bossis, Guss, Agin-Liebes, Malone, Cohen and Schmidt2016). The evidence from modern-day trials suggests that psychedelics are relatively safe in controlled settings and in some specific psychiatric populations (Roscoe & Lozy, Reference Roscoe and Lozy2022; Simonsson et al., Reference Simonsson, Carlbring, Carhart-Harris, Davis, Nutt, Griffiths and Goldberg2023a), but less is known about risks associated with psychedelic use outside of tightly controlled clinical trials (e.g. Evans et al., Reference Evans, Robinson, Argyri, Suseelan, Murphy-Beiner, McAlpine and Prideaux2023; Simonsson, Johnson, & Hendricks, Reference Simonsson, Johnson and Hendricks2024a), especially in potentially vulnerable populations.

Modern clinical trials usually implement exclusion criteria that disqualify individuals with either a personal or family history (e.g. first- or second-degree) history of psychotic or bipolar disorders (Johnson, Richards, & Griffiths, Reference Johnson, Richards and Griffiths2008). These criteria partially exist based on the concern that psychedelic use may trigger or worsen psychotic or manic symptoms in these populations (Strassman, Reference Strassman1984). Notably, however, in a recent clinical trial using psychedelic-assisted therapy in patients with bipolar II disorder, no increases in psychotic or manic symptoms were reported (Aaronson et al., Reference Aaronson, van der Vaart, Miller, LaPratt, Swartz, Shoultz and Suppes2024). Yet little is known about the risk of psychotic or manic episodes following naturalistic psychedelic use that lacks the extensive safeguards of a clinical trial, especially among individuals with other psychiatric histories of concern such as schizophrenia or bipolar I disorder (Barber, Nemeroff, & Siegel, Reference Barber, Nemeroff and Siegel2022; Bosch, Halm, & Seifritz, Reference Bosch, Halm and Seifritz2022; Dos Santos, Bouso, & Hallak, Reference Dos Santos, Bouso and Hallak2017; Gard et al., Reference Gard, Pleet, Bradley, Penn, Gallenstein, Riley and Woolley2021; Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Richards and Griffiths2008; Sapienza et al., Reference Sapienza, Bosia, Spangaro, Martini, Agostoni, Cuoco and Cavallaro2023, Reference Sapienza, Martini, Comai, Cavallaro, Spangaro, De Gregorio and Bosia2025; Wolf et al., Reference Wolf, Singh, Blakolmer, Lerer, Lifschytz, Heresco-Levy and Lerer2023). Such epidemiological research has become increasingly urgent in countries such as the United States, where there have been rapid increases in naturalistic psychedelic use (Walsh, Gorfinkel, Shmulewitz, Stohl, & Hasin, Reference Walsh, Gorfinkel, Shmulewitz, Stohl and Hasin2024), as well as recent legislative reforms to legalize or decriminalize the use of certain psychedelics in some jurisdictions (Siegel, Daily, Perry, & Nicol, Reference Siegel, Daily, Perry and Nicol2023).

Recent epidemiological research has sought to quantify the risk of psychotic or manic episodes following naturalistic psychedelic use among individuals with a history of psychotic or bipolar disorders (Honk et al., Reference Honk, Stenfors, Goldberg, Hendricks, Osika, Dourron and Simonsson2024; Morton et al., Reference Morton, Sakai, Ashtari, Pleet, Michalak and Woolley2023; Simonsson et al., Reference Simonsson, Goldberg, Chambers, Osika, Simonsson and Hendricks2023b, Reference Simonsson, Mosing, Osika, Ullén, Larsson, Lu and Wesseldijk2024b). For example, in a recent cross-sectional study, among adolescents with a greater genetic vulnerability to schizophrenia or bipolar I disorder, naturalistic psychedelic use was associated with more manic symptoms than in individuals with a lower genetic vulnerability (Simonsson, Mosing, et al., Reference Simonsson, Mosing, Osika, Ullén, Larsson, Lu and Wesseldijk2024b). Another recent study using a longitudinal research design found that naturalistic psychedelic use during the study period was associated with a decrease in the number of psychotic symptoms among participants with a personal history of psychotic disorders and an increase in the number of psychotic symptoms among those with a personal history of bipolar disorders. However, the study did not ask participants to report manic symptoms or specify the type of psychotic or bipolar disorder in their personal history (Honk et al., Reference Honk, Stenfors, Goldberg, Hendricks, Osika, Dourron and Simonsson2024), which precluded exploration of disorder-specific interactions with naturalistic psychedelic use on psychotic or manic symptoms. Prior work has also not investigated the quality of the acute psychedelic experience (e.g. psychologically challenging experiences, insightful experiences) and its association with psychotic or manic symptoms.

Since individuals with a personal or family history of psychotic or bipolar disorders are typically excluded from participation in clinical trials involving psychedelics, exploring naturalistic psychedelic use offers an opportunity to investigate psychiatric risks in these populations. Using a longitudinal research design with a large US sample (N=21,990), we conducted a study to better understand associations between naturalistic psychedelic use and psychotic or manic symptoms, with a specific focus on individuals with histories of certain psychotic or bipolar disorders. We aimed to longitudinally investigate (1) associations between naturalistic psychedelic use and changes in the severity of psychotic or manic symptoms; and (2) interactions between naturalistic psychedelic use and specific psychiatric histories (i.e. schizophrenia, bipolar I disorder) on changes in the severity of psychotic or manic symptoms.

Methods

Participants

The participants in this study were recruited through Prolific Academic (https://prolific.com), which is an online platform that facilitates study participant recruitment for researchers. Using purposive sampling to maximize the number of participants who were likely to report psychedelic use during the study period, we recruited US residents between 18 and 50 years old, based on results from a recent longitudinal study showing that 83% of participants who reported psychedelic use during the study period were 47 years or younger (Simonsson et al., Reference Simonsson, Osika, Stenfors, Goldberg, Honk and Hendricks2023d). We initially aimed to recruit at least 13,000 participants between 18 and 50 years of age, which we estimated would result in at least 395 participants who would report psychedelic use during the study period. However, only 336 participants reported psychedelic use during the first study period in 2023. We therefore continued recruitment during 2024 and aimed to recruit at least 7000 additional participants. The study (design plan, sampling plan, variables, and analysis plan) was preregistered and updated on the Open Science Framework (OSF) at https://osf.io/249db

Materials and procedure

Participants were recruited from June 2023 until September 2023 and in June 2024. Participants completed a baseline survey and were invited to complete a follow-up survey approximately two months later. This study was part of a larger survey and participants received a small monetary reimbursement upon completing both the baseline and follow-up surveys. Study procedures were determined to be exempt from review by the Institutional Review Board at the University of Wisconsin–Madison.

Measures

Psychedelic use

All participants were asked at baseline to report which, if any, of the following psychedelics they had used in the past two months: psilocybin, ayahuasca, N, N-Dimethyltryptamine (DMT), lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD), mescaline, peyote, or San Pedro. At follow-up, participants were asked to report which, if any, of the same psychedelics they had used during the two-month study period. Those who reported psychedelic use at follow-up were asked how many times they used psychedelics in the past two months (once, twice, three times or more). The same participants were asked to recall their most intense psychedelic experience during the study period and complete the Challenging Experiences Questionnaire (CEQ; Barrett, Bradstreet, Leoutsakos, Johnson, & Griffiths, Reference Barrett, Bradstreet, Leoutsakos, Johnson and Griffiths2016) and the Psychological Insight Questionnaire (PIQ; Davis et al., Reference Davis, Barrett, So, Gukasyan, Swift and Griffiths2021), which were developed to assess psychologically difficult states and the subjective experience of insight, respectively, during psychedelic experiences. The responses for CEQ were rated on a 0- (“‘None; not at all”) to 5-point (“Extreme”) Likert scale and the responses for PIQ were rated on a 0- (“No, not at all”) to 5-point (“Extremely [more than ever before in my life]”) Likert scale. The internal consistencies for CEQ and PIQ in the current sample were excellent (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.98 and 0.97, respectively). The same participants were also asked to report the type of psychedelic and the dose they used (i.e. low, moderate, large, very large, extremely large) for their most intense psychedelic experience during the study period, identify any applicable extra-pharmacological factors of interest (e.g. lack of psychological support; list from Simonsson, Hendricks, Chambers, Osika, & Goldberg, Reference Simonsson, Hendricks, Chambers, Osika and Goldberg2023c), indicate the date of the experience, and specify the city closest to where they had the experience. Depending on the laws and regulations applicable to the specified city at the date of their most intense psychedelic experience during the study period, we constructed a legal status variable with three levels (0 = no psychedelic use, 1 = psychedelic use in illegal context, 2 = psychedelic use in non-illegal [i.e. decriminalized or legal] context).

Demographics and other substance use

At baseline, all participants were asked to report their age (in years), gender identity, educational attainment, degree of religiosity, and political affiliation (see Supplemental Materials for survey items on demographics). At follow-up, participants were also asked to report past two-month use (yes, no) of alcohol, nicotine products (e.g. cigarettes, e-cigarettes, cigarillos, little cigars, and smokeless tobacco), cannabis products (e.g. weed, tetrahydrocannabinol (THC), cannabidiol (CBD), hemp oil), 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA), major stimulants (e.g. cocaine, methamphetamine), illicit narcotic analgesics/opioids (e.g. morphine, heroin, and oxycodone), illicit benzodiazepines and barbiturates (e.g. Diazepam [Valium], Alprazolam [Xanax]), inhalants (poppers, whip-its, nitrous oxide, and glue), and other substances.

Psychiatric history

At baseline, all participants were asked to report their personal psychiatric history. Specifically, participants were asked if they had a current or past diagnosis of any of the major diagnostic classes listed in the DSM-5-TR (see Supplemental Materials for full list). If participants reported a personal history of Schizophrenia Spectrum and Other Psychotic Disorders or Bipolar and Related Disorders, they were asked to select the specific diagnosis or diagnoses. Because the DSM-5-TR lists Schizotypal Personality Disorder under both the Schizophrenia Spectrum and Other Psychotic Disorders and Personality Disorders, participants were also asked to select the specific diagnosis or diagnoses if they reported a personal history of Personality Disorders. The same procedure was repeated at baseline with regard to the psychiatric history of family members (i.e. first-degree relatives and second-degree relatives). Because it is important to understand the relative contribution of varying degrees of genetic predisposition, we coded each psychiatric history variable (e.g. schizophrenia, bipolar I disorder) into four levels (0 = no psychiatric history, 1 = second-degree relative only, 2 = first-degree relative but no personal history, and 3 = personal history).

Psychotic symptoms

All participants were asked at both baseline and follow-up to complete a modified version of the Community Assessment of Psychic Experiences – Positive 15 (CAPE-P15) scale, which measures psychosis-like experiences (e.g. “have you heard voices when you are alone?”) that can be divided into three subscales: persecutory ideation, perceptual abnormalities, and bizarre experiences. The original CAPE-P15 scale asks participants about lifetime psychosis-like experiences (Capra, Kavanagh, Hides, & Scott, Reference Capra, Kavanagh, Hides and Scott2013), whereas other versions such as the Current CAPE-15 ask participants about recent psychosis-like experiences (e.g. in the past three months; Capra, Kavanagh, Hides, & Scott, Reference Capra, Kavanagh, Hides and Scott2017). In this study, we asked participants about psychosis-like experiences in the past week in order to increase the likelihood that the psychedelic use would precede the psychotic symptoms. Responses were rated on a Likert scale from 1 (“Never”) to 4 (“Nearly Always”). Internal consistency was excellent in the current sample (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.91 at baseline and 0.92 at non-imputed follow-up).

Manic symptoms

All participants were asked at both baseline and follow-up to complete the Altman Self-Rating Mania Scale (Altman, Hedeker, Peterson, & Davis, Reference Altman, Hedeker, Peterson and Davis1997), which assesses the presence and severity of manic (or hypomanic) symptoms using five items. The original questionnaire asks participants, for each item, to choose one of five statements (rated on a scale from 1 to 5) that best describes the way they have been feeling for the past week (e.g. “I feel happier or more cheerful than usual all of the time”), which was the same time frame used in this study. Internal consistency was acceptable in the current sample (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.78 at baseline and 0.80 at non-imputed follow-up).

Statistical analyses

As specified in the preregistration, we used multiple linear regression to examine whether there were significant differences in changes (i.e. follow-up scores minus baseline scores) in the severity of psychotic or manic symptoms between participants who reported psychedelic use during the study period and those who did not. For each model, we conducted sensitivity analyses using follow-up scores as the outcome variable while controlling for baseline scores. This allowed us to assessed whether the observed results remained consistent when accounting for baseline levels.

We also used multiple linear regression to test whether there were interactions between psychedelic use during the study period and specific psychiatric histories (i.e. schizophrenia, bipolar I disorder) on changes in the severity of psychotic or manic symptoms. We were primarily interested in schizophrenia and bipolar I disorder, but exploratory analyses were conducted on other specific diagnoses under the Schizophrenia Spectrum and Other Psychotic Disorders, Bipolar and Related Disorders, and Personality Disorders. Additional exploratory analyses investigated associations between the legal status and frequency of psychedelic use and changes in the severity of psychotic or manic symptoms, as well as associations of challenging experiences and subjective experience of insight during the most intense psychedelic experience during the study period with changes in the severity of psychotic or manic symptoms.

All analyses included broadly the same covariates used in a recent longitudinal study that investigated psychedelic use and psychotic symptoms (Honk et al., Reference Honk, Stenfors, Goldberg, Hendricks, Osika, Dourron and Simonsson2024): age (recoded: 18–24, 25–34, 35–44, 45–50), gender identity (recoded: male, female, and other), educational attainment (bachelor’s degree or higher and less than bachelor’s degree), degree of religiosity (not at all religious, a little religious, quite religious, moderately religious, very religious), political affiliation (Democratic Party and Republican Party), past two-month use (yes, no) of alcohol, nicotine products, cannabis products, MDMA, major stimulants, illicit narcotic analgesics/opioids, illicit benzodiazepines and barbiturates, inhalants, and other substances at follow-up, as well as psychedelic use in the past two months at baseline. The analyses of challenging psychedelic experiences and psychedelic-related insights also controlled for dose used (low, moderate, large, very large, and extremely large) during participants’ most intense psychedelic experience during the study period. Because we collected data across two years (i.e. 2023 and 2024), we controlled for the survey year in all analyses.

To handle missing data at follow-up, we employed Multivariate Imputation by Chained Equations (MICE; van Buuren & Groothuis-Oudshoorn, Reference Van Buuren and Groothuis-Oudshoorn2011; mice package version 3.15.0 in R Studio) to impute the missing data across twenty imputed data sets using random forest imputations. Before imputing the data, we split the dataset into two subsets: (1) a subset with survey items only asked to participants who reported psychedelic use during the study period, which did not have any missing data; and (2) a subset with survey items asked to all participants (i.e. without any missing-by-design structures). Results were pooled using Rubin’s (Reference Rubin1976) rules (i.e. across imputations and pooled using the “pool” function in the mice package; van Buuren & Groothuis-Oudshoorn, Reference Van Buuren and Groothuis-Oudshoorn2011). All continuous variables were standardized using the “scale” function. Descriptive statistics show non-imputed values only while results based on imputed data are presented for the multiple linear regression models unless otherwise specified. In all tests, a two-sided p<0.05 was used as the significance threshold. Because the study did not have predefined hypotheses and given the early, exploratory nature of work in this area, corrections for multiple comparisons were not applied to allow for the detection of potentially meaningful associations that could inform future research.

Results

21,990 participants completed the survey at baseline and 12,345 of these participants completed the follow-up survey (56% retention). Among the participants who completed the follow-up survey, 505 reported psychedelic use during the two-month study period, which represents approximately 4% of the sample that completed the follow-up survey. Table 1 shows sample characteristics at baseline among participants who completed the follow-up survey. As shown in the table, compared with those who did not report psychedelic use during the two-month study, participants who reported psychedelic use during the study period had a higher lifetime history of psychedelic use (82% versus 21%) as well as other types of drug use (i.e. alcohol, nicotine products, cannabis products, MDMA, major stimulants, illicit narcotic analgesics or opioids, illicit benzodiazepines and barbiturates, inhalants, and other substances). There were also differences between groups regarding age, gender identity, degree of religiosity, and political affiliation. Notably, however, among those who reported psychedelic use during the study period, there were more individuals with a current or past history of bipolar I disorder. Finally, among those who reported psychedelic use during the study, their most intense psychedelic experience commonly involved psilocybin (69%), moderate or higher doses (66%), negative mindset prior to the experience (19%) or no psychological support during the experience (18%; see Table S1), and taking place in an illegal context (97% of full subsample; 100% and 95% of those with a personal history of schizophrenia and bipolar I disorder, respectively; see Table S2).

Table 1. Sample characteristics

This table shows sample characteristics at baseline of participants who completed the follow-up survey and reported psychedelic use during the study period and participants who completed the follow-up survey and did not report psychedelic use during the same period. All percentages were calculated based on the total number (n) for each column and were rounded to the nearest 0.1%; cumulative percentages may not add to 100.0. Pearson’s chi-squared tests were used to examine the characteristics of psychedelic users versus non-users.

Table 2 shows results from the multiple linear regression models on the associations between psychedelic use during the study period and changes in the severity of psychotic and manic symptoms, including the models that evaluate interactions of psychedelic use with a psychiatric history of schizophrenia or bipolar I disorder. As demonstrated in the table, psychedelic use during the study period was associated with increases in the severity of both psychotic and manic symptoms (β=0.35, p<.001; β=0.14, p=.006; see Table 2 and Tables S3 and S4). However, compared with participants who did not report psychedelic use during the study period, exploratory analyses revealed that increases in the severity of both psychotic and manic symptoms were only observed among participants who reported psychedelic use in an illegal context (β=0.37, p<.001; β=0.15, p=.004; see Table S5). Exploratory analyses also suggested that the association between psychedelic use and psychotic (but not manic) symptoms was frequency-dependent (i.e. more frequent use associated with greater increases), at least for certain aspects of psychotic symptoms (e.g. perceptual abnormalities; see Table S6). Further exploratory analyses showed that more severe challenging psychedelic experiences were associated with increases in the severity of psychotic (but not manic) symptoms among those who reported psychedelic use during the study period (β=0.34, p<.001). At the same time, greater psychedelic-related insights were associated with increases in the severity of manic (but not psychotic) symptoms among those who reported psychedelic use during the study period (β=0.14, p=.015; see Table S7).

Table 2. Model estimates

This table shows associations between psychedelic use and changes in severity of psychotic or manic symptoms, as well as interactions between psychedelic use and specific psychiatric histories (i.e. schizophrenia, bipolar I disorder) on changes in severity of psychotic or manic symptoms. β = standardized coefficient; SE = standard error; p = p-value; SCZ = schizophrenia; BIP = bipolar I disorder. The models controlled for age, gender identity, educational attainment, degree of religiosity, political affiliation, past two-month use of alcohol, nicotine products, cannabis products, MDMA, major stimulants, illicit narcotic analgesics/opioids, illicit benzodiazepines and barbiturates, inhalants, and other substances at follow-up, psychedelic use in the past two months at baseline, and survey year. See Table S9 for results from the non-imputed data.

In the interaction models, a personal history of schizophrenia or bipolar I disorder interacted with psychedelic use on manic (but not psychotic) symptoms such that those with a personal history of these disorders who used psychedelics reported greater increases in the severity of manic symptoms (β=1.14, p=.027; β=0.68, p=.003; see Table 2 and Tables S3 and S4). Notably, although there were no significant interactions, for example, with conditions such as bipolar II disorder, additional exploratory analyses revealed that individuals with certain other psychiatric histories (e.g. schizotypal personality disorder; β=1.57, p=.015) who used psychedelics reported greater increases in the severity of manic symptoms (see Table S8). The results were broadly consistent in sensitivity analyses.

Discussion

This study investigated the longitudinal associations of naturalistic psychedelic use with psychotic and manic symptoms, especially among putatively vulnerable individuals with a psychiatric history of schizophrenia or bipolar I disorder. The study findings showed associations between naturalistic psychedelic use and increases in the severity of both psychotic and manic symptoms, with such increases only observed for those who reported naturalistic psychedelic use in an illegal context. While increases in the severity of psychotic symptoms appeared to depend on more frequent use and more severe challenging psychedelic experiences, increases in the severity of manic symptoms appeared to be moderated by a personal history of schizophrenia or bipolar I disorder and greater subjective experience of insight during a psychedelic experience. Notably, individuals with certain other psychiatric histories (e.g. schizotypal personality disorder) who used psychedelics also reported greater increases in the severity of manic symptoms.

The pattern of only observing increases in the severity of both psychotic and manic symptoms among those who reported naturalistic psychedelic use in an illegal context could have several potential explanations. For example, individuals who use psychedelic in an illegal context may generally experience more stress or anxiety during the psychedelic experience due to fear of legal repercussions, which may negatively impact post-acute outcomes. It is also possible that differences in the purity and potency of psychedelic substances used in illegal versus non-illegal contexts could partially explain the results. Future research should further interrogate this.

The association between naturalistic psychedelic use and increases in the severity of psychotic symptoms in this study contrasts with a recent longitudinal study that reported decreases in delusional ideation following naturalistic psychedelic use (Zhou, De Wied, Carhart-Harris, & Kettner, Reference Zhou, De Wied, Carhart-Harris and Kettner2022; see also Honk et al., Reference Honk, Stenfors, Goldberg, Hendricks, Osika, Dourron and Simonsson2024; Simonsson, Mosing, et al., Reference Simonsson, Mosing, Osika, Ullén, Larsson, Lu and Wesseldijk2024b). These differences may be explained by differences across studies in sample characteristics, research design, or measurement of psychotic symptoms. It is possible, for instance, that the samples differed in knowledge about psychedelic harm reduction or exposure to extra-pharmacological factors that have been associated with challenging psychedelic experiences in previous research (e.g. lack of psychological support; Simonsson, Hendricks, et al., Reference Simonsson, Hendricks, Chambers, Osika and Goldberg2023c). Such an explanation seems reasonable in light of the association in this study between the severity of challenging psychedelic experiences and increases in the severity of psychotic symptoms. In fact, a large portion of participants in the current study who reported psychedelic use also reported that many such extra-pharmacological factors were present during their most intense psychedelic experience during the study period, which indicates that circumstances may not have been ideal for positive outcomes in this sample. There did appear to be a frequency-dependent relationship between psychedelic use and psychotic symptoms, though, such that the more times psychedelics had been used in the study period, the larger the effect sizes. If there is indeed a frequency-dependent relationship between psychedelic use and psychotic symptoms, such findings could potentially be explained by a greater likelihood of having a more severe challenging psychedelic experience among those who used psychedelics on more occasions. Future research should explore this further.

Notably, however, individuals with a personal history of schizophrenia or bipolar I disorder did not appear to be at risk for increases in the severity of psychotic symptoms as a result of psychedelic use. Such null findings correspond with results from a recent cross-sectional study that evaluated interactions between polygenic scores for schizophrenia or bipolar I disorder and lifetime psychedelic use on self-reported psychotic symptoms among adolescents (Simonsson, Mosing, et al., Reference Simonsson, Mosing, Osika, Ullén, Larsson, Lu and Wesseldijk2024b; see also Sapienza et al., Reference Sapienza, Martini, Comai, Cavallaro, Spangaro, De Gregorio and Bosia2025). At the same time, the present findings contrast with results from a recent longitudinal study that evaluated interactions with a personal history of psychotic or bipolar disorders more generally and naturalistic psychedelic use during the study period on psychotic symptoms (Honk et al., Reference Honk, Stenfors, Goldberg, Hendricks, Osika, Dourron and Simonsson2024). It is possible that the contrast in findings can be explained by differences across studies in the measurement of psychotic symptoms (e.g. Current CAPE-15 versus psychotic ideation subscale of the Psychiatric Diagnostic Screening Questionnaire; Zimmerman & Mattia, Reference Zimmerman and Mattia2001) or the specificity of disorder categorization (e.g. schizophrenia specifically versus psychotic disorders more generally, bipolar I disorder specifically versus bipolar disorders more generally). Future research on psychedelic use and psychotic symptoms should aim for more consistency with regard to assessment procedures and more specificity with regard to disorder categorization.

Although the finding that naturalistic psychedelic use and the subjective experience of insight during a psychedelic experience were longitudinally associated with greater increases in the severity of manic symptoms is relatively novel, the results on manic symptoms from interaction models in this study partially replicate previous findings (Simonsson, Mosing, et al., Reference Simonsson, Mosing, Osika, Ullén, Larsson, Lu and Wesseldijk2024b). These findings suggest that a personal history of schizophrenia or bipolar I disorder may indeed be contraindicated for psychedelic use (Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Richards and Griffiths2008). Notably, however, those with a personal history of schizotypal personality disorder who used psychedelics also reported greater increases in the severity of manic symptoms. This supports the possibility that a specific genetic profile, more prevalent in certain psychiatric populations, may be contraindicated for psychedelic use. It is important to note, however, that among participants who reported psychedelic use in a non-illegal context during the study period, barely anyone had a personal history of schizophrenia (n=0), bipolar I disorder (n=1), or schizotypal personality disorder (n=0), which could potentially explain the greater increase in manic symptoms in these populations. Future research should nonetheless further explore the possibility of developing genetic biomarkers that can be used to recommend for or against psychedelic use.

Limitations

This study has several limitations that should be considered. First, the study used purposive sampling to maximize the number of participants who were likely to report psychedelic use during the study period. This may limit generalizations to the population at large. Second, there was only a small number of participants who reported both psychedelic use during the study period and a personal history of schizophrenia (n=4) or bipolar I disorder (n=22). Similarly, only a small number of participants reported psychedelic use in a non-illegal context (n=33). Results drawn from such a small proportion of the study sample should be interpreted cautiously. Third, the attrition rate at follow-up was relatively high. Fourth, no corrections were made for multiple comparisons, which increases the risk of Type 1 errors, though the main analyses were pre-registered. Fifth, due to the study design (i.e. observational), no conclusive causal inferences can be made. Sixth, given that all responses were self-reported, it is possible that recall or social desirability biases, for example, led participants to inaccurately report the types of substances used or psychiatric history and symptoms. Seventh, there was also no assessment of the number of relatives with a possible psychiatric history, which could have been important for understanding genetic vulnerability. Eighth, this study did not evaluate the clinical meaning of the significant associations. It is possible, for instance, that increases in psychotic symptoms reflect a beneficial change in creativity or that increases in manic symptoms capture improvements in mood from a previously depressed state (Dourron, Strauss, & Hendricks, Reference Dourron, Strauss and Hendricks2022). This should be investigated in future research. Future longitudinal studies should also investigate interactions between psychedelic use and self-reported psychiatric history or polygenic scores for schizophrenia, bipolar I disorder, or other disorders of concern (e.g. schizotypal personality disorder) on psychotic or manic symptoms, ideally with symptoms rated by a clinician. This work is crucial for ensuring that the risks associated with psychedelic use, both in naturalistic and clinical settings, can be minimized and mitigated.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291725000716.

Availability of data and materials

The unimputed data and R syntax for primary analyses are available at https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.28645415.v1.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Study procedures were determined to be exempt from review by the Institutional Review Board at the University of Wisconsin–Madison. All procedures involving human participants were conducted in accordance with the ethical standards of the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Informed consent was obtained digitally from all individual participants included in the study.

Funding

OS was supported by Olle Engvist Foundation, Ekhaga Foundation, and Norrsken Mind. SG was supported by a grant (K23AT010879) from the National Center for Complementary and Integrative Health and CS was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health of the National Institutes of Health (T32MH018931). Support for this research was also provided by the University of Alabama at Birmingham School of Public Health, the Swedish Research Council for Sustainable Development (FORMAS; FR-2018-0006; FR-2018-00246), Forte (2020-00977), Imperial College Business School, and the Building Psychological Capacity fund.

Competing interests

PSH was previously in paid advisory relationships with Eleusis Benefit Corporation, Journey Colab Corporation, Reset Pharmaceuticals Inc., and Silo Pharma and is currently in paid advisory relationships with Bright Minds Biosciences Ltd. and holds stock options in Beckley PsyTech. PSH is a co-founder of Equulus Therapeutics and Mycelial Health. OS was a co-founder of Eudelics AB and previously received a small payment from Mindfully Sweden AB for educational content.

References

Aaronson, S. T., van der Vaart, A., Miller, T., LaPratt, J., Swartz, K., Shoultz, A., … & Suppes, T. (2024). Single-dose synthetic psilocybin with psychotherapy for treatment-resistant bipolar type II major depressive episodes: A nonrandomized open-label trial. JAMA Psychiatry, 81(6), 555562. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2023.4685CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Altman, E. G., Hedeker, D., Peterson, J. L., & Davis, J. M. (1997). The Altman self-rating mania scale. Biological Psychiatry, 42(10), 948955. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(96)00548-3Google ScholarPubMed
Barber, G., Nemeroff, C. B., & Siegel, S. (2022). A case of prolonged mania, psychosis, and severe depression after psilocybin use: Implications of increased psychedelic drug availability. American Journal of Psychiatry, 179(12), 892896. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.22010073Google ScholarPubMed
Barrett, F. S., Bradstreet, M. P., Leoutsakos, J. M. S., Johnson, M. W., & Griffiths, R. R. (2016). The challenging experience questionnaire: Characterization of challenging experiences with psilocybin mushrooms. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 30(12), 12791295. https://doi.org/10.1177/0269881116678781CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bosch, O. G., Halm, S., & Seifritz, E. (2022). Psychedelics in the treatment of unipolar and bipolar depression. International Journal of Bipolar Disorders, 10(1), 18. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40345-022-00265-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Capra, C., Kavanagh, D. J., Hides, L., & Scott, J. (2013). Brief screening for psychosis-like experiences. Schizophrenia Research, 149(1–3), 104107. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2013.05.020CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Capra, C., Kavanagh, D. J., Hides, L., & Scott, J. G. (2017). Current CAPE-15: A measure of recent psychotic-like experiences and associated distress. Early Intervention in Psychiatry, 11(5), 411417. https://doi.org/10.1111/eip.12245CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, A. K., Barrett, F. S., So, S., Gukasyan, N., Swift, T. C., & Griffiths, R. R. (2021). Development of the psychological insight questionnaire among a sample of people who have consumed psilocybin or LSD. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 35(4), 437446. https://doi.org/10.1177/0269881120967878Google ScholarPubMed
Dourron, H. M., Strauss, C., & Hendricks, P. S. (2022). Self-entropic broadening theory: Toward a new understanding of self and behavior change informed by psychedelics and psychosis. Pharmacological Reviews, 74(4), 9841029. https://doi.org/10.1124/pharmrev.121.000514Google Scholar
Dos Santos, R. G., Bouso, J. C., & Hallak, J. E. (2017). Ayahuasca, dimethyltryptamine, and psychosis: A systematic review of human studies. Therapeutic Advances in Psychopharmacology, 7(4), 141157. https://doi.org/10.1177/2045125316689030CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, J., Robinson, O. C., Argyri, E. K., Suseelan, S., Murphy-Beiner, A., McAlpine, R., … & Prideaux, E. (2023). Extended difficulties following the use of psychedelic drugs: A mixed methods study. Plos One, 18(10), e0293349. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0293349CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gard, D. E., Pleet, M. M., Bradley, E. R., Penn, A. D., Gallenstein, M. L., Riley, L. S., … & Woolley, J. D. (2021). Evaluating the risk of psilocybin for the treatment of bipolar depression: A review of the research literature and published case studies. Journal of Affective Disorders Reports, 6, 100240. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadr.2021.100240CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodwin, G. M., Aaronson, S. T., Alvarez, O., Arden, P. C., Baker, A., Bennett, J. C., … & Malievskaia, E. (2022). Single-dose psilocybin for a treatment-resistant episode of major depression. New England Journal of Medicine, 387(18), 16371648. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMoa2206443CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Griffiths, R. R., Johnson, M. W., Carducci, M. A., Umbricht, A., Richards, W. A., Richards, B. D., … & Klinedinst, M. A. (2016). Psilocybin produces substantial and sustained decreases in depression and anxiety in patients with life-threatening cancer: A randomized double-blind trial. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 30(12), 11811197. https://doi.org/10.1177/0269881116675513CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Honk, L., Stenfors, C. U., Goldberg, S. B., Hendricks, P. S., Osika, W., Dourron, H. M., … & Simonsson, O. (2024). Longitudinal associations between psychedelic use and psychotic symptoms in the United States and the United Kingdom. Journal of Affective Disorders, 351, 194201. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2024.01.197CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, M. W., Richards, W. A., & Griffiths, R. R. (2008). Human hallucinogen research: Guidelines for safety. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 22(6), 603620. https://doi.org/10.1177/0269881108093587Google ScholarPubMed
Morton, E., Sakai, K., Ashtari, A., Pleet, M., Michalak, E. E., & Woolley, J. (2023). Risks and benefits of psilocybin use in people with bipolar disorder: An international web-based survey on experiences of ‘magic mushroom’consumption. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 37(1), 4960. https://doi.org/10.1177/02698811221131997CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nutt, D. & Carhart-Harris, R. (2021). The current status of psychedelics in psychiatry. JAMA Psychiatry, 78(2), 121122. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2020.2171CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raison, C. L., Sanacora, G., Woolley, J., Heinzerling, K., Dunlop, B. W., Brown, R. T., … & Griffiths, R. R. (2023). Single-dose psilocybin treatment for major depressive disorder: A randomized clinical trial. JAMA, 330(9), 843853. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2023.14530CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roscoe, J., & Lozy, O. (2022). Can psilocybin be safely administered under medical supervision? A systematic review of adverse event reporting in clinical trials. Drug Science, Policy and Law, 8, 20503245221085222. https://doi.org/10.1177/20503245221085222CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ross, S., Bossis, A., Guss, J., Agin-Liebes, G., Malone, T., Cohen, B., … & Schmidt, B. L. (2016). Rapid and sustained symptom reduction following psilocybin treatment for anxiety and depression in patients with life-threatening cancer: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 30(12), 11651180. https://doi.org/10.1177/0269881116675512CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rubin, D. B. (1976). Inference and missing data. Biometrika, 63(3), 581592. https://doi.org/10.2307/2335739CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sapienza, J., Bosia, M., Spangaro, M., Martini, F., Agostoni, G., Cuoco, F., … & Cavallaro, R. (2023). Schizophrenia and psychedelic state: Dysconnection versus hyper-connection. A perspective on two different models of psychosis stemming from dysfunctional integration processes. Molecular Psychiatry, 28(1), 5967. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-022-01721-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sapienza, J., Martini, F., Comai, S., Cavallaro, R., Spangaro, M., De Gregorio, D., & Bosia, M. (2025). Psychedelics and schizophrenia: A double-edged sword. Molecular Psychiatry, 30, 679692. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-024-02743-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siegel, J. S., Daily, J. E., Perry, D. A., & Nicol, G. E. (2023). Psychedelic drug legislative reform and legalization in the US. JAMA Psychiatry, 80(1), 7783. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2022.4101Google ScholarPubMed
Simonsson, O., Carlbring, P., Carhart-Harris, R., Davis, A. K., Nutt, D. J., Griffiths, R. R., … & Goldberg, S. B. (2023a). Assessing the risk of symptom worsening in psilocybin-assisted therapy for depression: A systematic review and individual participant data meta-analysis. Psychiatry Research, 327, 115349. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2023.115349CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simonsson, O., Goldberg, S. B., Chambers, R., Osika, W., Simonsson, C., & Hendricks, P. S. (2023b). Psychedelic use and psychiatric risks. Psychopharmacology, 326, 17. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-023-06478-5Google Scholar
Simonsson, O., Hendricks, P. S., Chambers, R., Osika, W., & Goldberg, S. B. (2023c). Prevalence and associations of challenging, difficult or distressing experiences using classic psychedelics. Journal of Affective Disorders, 326, 105110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2023.01.073CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simonsson, O., Johnson, M. W., & Hendricks, P. S. (2024a). Psychedelic and MDMA-related adverse effects–call for action. JAMA Health Forum, 5(11), e243630e243630. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamahealthforum.2024.3630Google ScholarPubMed
Simonsson, O., Mosing, M. A., Osika, W., Ullén, F., Larsson, H., Lu, Y., & Wesseldijk, L. W. (2024b). Adolescent psychedelic use and psychotic or manic symptoms. JAMA Psychiatry, 81(6), 579585. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2024.0047CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simonsson, O., Osika, W., Stenfors, C. U., Goldberg, S. B., Honk, L., & Hendricks, P. S. (2023d). Longitudinal associations between psychedelic use and meditation practices in the United States and the United Kingdom. Psychological Medicine, 54(6), 12281234. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291723003082CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strassman, R. J. (1984). Adverse reactions to psychedelic drugs. A review of the literature. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 172(10), 577595. https://doi.org/10.1097/00005053-198410000-00001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Buuren, S., & Groothuis-Oudshoorn, K. (2011). mice: Multivariate imputation by chained equations in R. Journal of Statistical Software, 45, 167. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v045.i03Google Scholar
Walsh, C. A., Gorfinkel, L., Shmulewitz, D., Stohl, M., & Hasin, D. S. (2024). Use of Lysergic acid Diethylamide by major depression status. JAMA Psychiatry, 81(1), 8996. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2023.3867CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolf, G., Singh, S., Blakolmer, K., Lerer, L., Lifschytz, T., Heresco-Levy, U., … & Lerer, B. (2023). Could psychedelic drugs have a role in the treatment of schizophrenia? Rationale and strategy for safe implementation. Molecular Psychiatry, 28(1), 4458. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-022-01832-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhou, K., De Wied, D., Carhart-Harris, R., & Kettner, H. (2022). Predictors of hallucinogen persisting perception disorder symptoms, delusional ideation and magical thinking following naturalistic psychedelic use. PsyArXiv. https://osf.io/preprints/psyarxiv/txzhaCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zimmerman, M., & Mattia, J. I. (2001). A self-report scale to help make psychiatric diagnoses: The psychiatric diagnostic screening questionnaire. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58(8), 787794. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.58.8.787CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Sample characteristics

Figure 1

Table 2. Model estimates

Supplementary material: File

Simonsson et al. supplementary material

Simonsson et al. supplementary material
Download Simonsson et al. supplementary material(File)
File 62.4 KB