Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7fkt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-27T20:34:45.706Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Parents' use of harsh punishment and young children's behaviour and achievement: a longitudinal study of Jamaican children with conduct problems

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 October 2018

H. Baker-Henningham*
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, Bangor University, Bangor, LL57 2AS, UK Caribbean Institute for Health Research, University of the West Indies, Kingston 7, Jamaica
T. Francis
Affiliation:
Caribbean Institute for Health Research, University of the West Indies, Kingston 7, Jamaica
*
*Address for correspondence: Dr Helen Baker-Henningham, School of Psychology, Bangor University, Bangor LL57 2AS, UK and Caribbean Institute for Health Research, University of the West Indies, Mona, Kingston 7, Jamaica. (Email: [email protected] and [email protected])
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Introduction.

Harsh punishment by parents is common in low- and middle-income countries (LMIC), yet there is limited evidence from LMIC of the effects of harsh punishment on child outcomes.

Methods.

A longitudinal, prospective study was conducted with children with conduct problems to examine the associations between parents’ use of harsh punishment during the preschool years on child behaviour and school achievement in grade one of primary school. As part of an efficacy trial in 24 preschools, 225 children with the highest level of teacher-reported conduct problems were evaluated and their parents reported on how often they used harsh punishment. Outcome measures in grade one included child conduct problems by independent observation, teacher and parent report, child social skills by teacher and parent report, direct tests of children's academic achievement and language skills, and tester ratings of child attention and impulse control.

Results.

Children had a mean age of 6.92 years and 61% were boys. All parents reported using harsh punishment. After controlling for child age and sex, socio-economic status, parents’ involvement with child and maternal education, frequency of harsh punishment was associated with growth in child conduct problems by independent classroom observations (p  =  0.037), parent (p  =  0.018) and teacher (p  =  0.044) report, a reduction in child social skills by teacher (p  =  0.024) and parent (p  =  0.014) report and poorer attention during the test session (p  =  0.049).

Conclusion.

The associations between frequency of parents’ use of harsh punishment with their preschoolers with conduct problems and later child behaviour indicate a need to train parents in non-violent behaviour management.

Type
Original Research Paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s) 2018

Background

Violence against children is a global public health problem and is highly prevalent in low- and middle-income countries (LMIC). Violence against children within the family is one of the most common forms of child maltreatment and often occurs due to harsh punishment methods being used to discipline children (UNICEF, 2010). Parent discipline practices can be classified into three main categories: (1) non-violent (e.g. removing privileges, explaining why something is wrong), (2) psychologically aggressive (e.g. name-calling, yelling) and (3) physically violent (e.g. slapping, beating with an object). Harsh punishment includes the use of psychologically aggressive and physically violent practices and these are considered violence against children or child maltreatment (Straus et al. Reference Straus, Hamby, Finkelhor, Moore and Runyan1998).

Harsh punishment in childhood is associated with multiple negative outcomes, which persist into adulthood. In a meta-analytical review of 88 studies, Gershoff (Reference Gershoff2002) found that corporal punishment was associated with increased aggression [effect size (ES): 0.36], poor mental health (ES = −0.49), negative parent–child relationships (ES = −0.58) and delinquent and antisocial behaviour (ES  =  0.42) in childhood. Effects of corporal punishment persisted into adulthood leading to increased aggression (ES  =  0.57), poor mental health (ES = −0.09), criminality and antisocial behaviour (ES  =  0.42) and an increased risk for perpetrating child or spouse abuse (ES  =  0.13) (Gershoff, Reference Gershoff2002). A more recent meta-analytical review of 124 studies found significant associations between child physical and emotional abuse and depression [physical abuse: odds ratio (OR)  =  1.54, emotional abuse: OR = 3.06), drug use (physical abuse: OR = 1.92, emotional abuse: OR = 1.41), suicide attempts (physical abuse: OR = 3.40, emotional abuse: OR = 3.37) and sexually transmitted infections and risky sexual behaviour (physical abuse: OR = 1.78, emotional abuse: OR = 1.75) (Norman et al. Reference Norman, Byambaa, De, Butchart, Scott and Vos2012). Studies examining associations between harsh punishment and academic achievement show mixed results (Romano et al. Reference Romano, Babchishin, Marquis and Fréchette2015). There is some evidence that the effect of harsh punishment on achievement is moderated by risk status with more vulnerable children at particular risk, for example, children in out-of home care (Romano et al. Reference Romano, Babchishin, Marquis and Fréchette2015) and children with HIV (Sherr et al. Reference Sherr, Hensels, Skeen, Tomlinson, Roberts and Macedo2016).

Theoretical and empirical models of the mechanisms through which harsh punishment affects child outcomes are numerous and include observational learning, social control theory, social informational processing theory and emotional and sensory arousal (Gershoff, Reference Gershoff2002). The models share a common theme, which is that the effect of harsh punishment on child outcomes is mediated through its effects on children's internal cognitive and affective processes (Gershoff, Reference Gershoff2002; Straus & Paschall, Reference Straus and Paschall2009; Maguire-Jack et al. Reference Maguire-Jack, Gromoske and Berger2012). That is, harsh punishment is hypothesised to affect children's thoughts and emotions which in turn influences their behaviour and makes them more susceptible to further negative experiences and/or interactions from others, including parents, teachers and peers.

In the Jamaican context, the use of harsh punishment as a disciplinary strategy is pervasive at home and at school. In the UNICEF Multiple Indicator Cluster Study, 84% of caregivers of 2–4 years old children reported that they or someone in their household had used physical violence with their child in the past month while 71% had used psychological aggression (Lansford & Deater-Deckard, Reference Lansford and Deater-Deckard2012). In a study of 1300 children in grade five of primary school, 92.5% of children had been reported being physically punished at school during the school year and exposure to physical punishment was associated with children's academic achievement in a dose–response manner: maths (high exposure: ES = −0.23; moderate exposure: ES = −0.09), reading (high exposure: ES = −0.44; moderate exposure: ES = −0.17) and spelling (high exposure: ES = −0.24; moderate exposure: ES = −0.14) (Baker-Henningham et al. Reference Baker-Henningham, Meeks Gardner, Chang and Walker2009). A further study of 1171 children aged 11–12 years reported a lifetime experience of verbal aggression and physical violence of 97.2% at home and 86.2% at school and poly-victimisation (including harsh punishment, exposure to community violence and witnessing domestic violence) was associated with poor intellectual functioning and for boys, increased behavioural risk (Samms-Vaughan & Lambert, Reference Samms-Vaughan and Lambert2017).

In this study, we investigate the associations between harsh punishment and later child outcomes for young children with high levels of conduct problems. Harsh punishment is considered a causal risk factor not only for the development, but also for the persistence of child behaviour problems (Jaffee et al. Reference Jaffee, Strait and Odgers2012). The prevalence of conduct problems in children ranges from 7% to 25% and children with conduct problems are at heightened risk for developing disruptive behaviour disorders in later childhood (Webster-Stratton & Hammond, Reference Webster-Stratton and Hammond1998). Disruptive behaviour disorders (encompassing conduct disorder and oppositional deviant disorder), are one of the most common forms of child psychopathology with a worldwide prevalence estimate of 5.7% (Polanczyk et al. Reference Polanczyk, Salum, Sugaya, Caye and Rohde2015). They predict negative outcomes across the lifespan including continued antisocial behaviour, school failure and involvement in crime and violence (Moffitt & Scott, Reference Moffitt, Scott, Rutter, Bishop, Pine, Scott, Stevenson, Taylor and Thapar2008). In Jamaica, 12% of 5–6 years old children were identified as having externalizing disorders and none of these children had accessed mental health services (Samms-Vaughan, Reference Samms-Vaughan2005). Furthermore, in a study in 24 inner city Jamaican preschools, 21% (364/1733) of 3–6 years old children had four or more symptoms of conduct problems as reported by the teacher (Baker-Henningham et al. Reference Baker-Henningham, Scott, Jones and Walker2012).

We are aware of no longitudinal studies with prospective measures of parent discipline and child behaviour from Jamaica or the wider Caribbean and the majority of studies in LMIC involve older children with limited evidence of the effect of harsh punishment in the early childhood years. Early childhood is a particularly sensitive period, and experiences in early childhood have long-term effects on brain function, cognition and psychosocial functioning thus providing the foundation for future physical and mental health (Walker et al. Reference Walker, Wachs, Grantham-McGregor, Black, Nelson, Huffman, Baker-Henningham, Chang, Hamadani, Lozoff, Meeks Gardner, Powell, Rahman and Richter2011; Black et al. Reference Black, Walker, Fernald, Andersen, DiGirolamo, Lu, Fink, Shawar, Shiffman, Devercelli, Wodon, Vargas-Baron and Grantham-McGregor2016). We hypothesised that the frequency of parents’ use of harsh punishment, defined as psychologically and physically violent discipline practices, during the preschool years with disadvantaged children with high levels of conduct problems would be associated with: (1) worsening behaviour over time including increased conduct problems and decreased social skills at school and at home between preschool and grade one of primary school and (2) poorer academic achievement, oral language and self-regulation skills in grade one of primary school.

Methods

Sampling

Data for this paper were collected as part of a follow-up study of a previously conducted efficacy trial in 24 community preschools situated in disadvantaged areas of Kingston, Jamaica (Baker-Henningham et al. Reference Baker-Henningham, Scott, Jones and Walker2012). The sampling procedure for the preschools involved surveying all preschools in a specified geographical location to determine the number of classes and number of children per class and selecting preschools that fit the following inclusion criteria: (1) consisted of 3–4 classes of children, (2) had a minimum of 20 children per class and (3) all teachers consented to participate in the trial. Fifty preschools were assessed for eligibility: 25 preschools had too few children per class and/or did not have 3–4 classes and one preschool refused to participate. All classrooms in each preschool were included in the study giving a total of 73 classrooms. Children at high risk of developing conduct problems were selected in preschool and were evaluated as part of the efficacy trial. In the summer term, teachers in the 24 preschools rated all children in their class on a 10-question screen for conduct problems using a four-point scale. Questions were based on age-appropriate items for a diagnosis of conduct disorder from the ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Diagnostic Criteria for Research (WHO, 1993) and included: loses temper, back chats, disobedient/breaks rules, annoys others, blames others, easily annoyed, often angry, spiteful to others, fights or bullies and destroys property. The responses were summed and the three children scoring the highest on the screen from each class were selected. Six children were kept back in their previous class after the summer holiday and these children were retained in the study and additional children enrolled (Baker-Henningham et al. Reference Baker-Henningham, Scott, Jones and Walker2012). A total of 225 children were recruited and 214 of these children were tracked into primary school. Three parents were unable to be contacted for interview; the final sample included 211 children in primary school.

This paper includes data collected at two time points: (i) on enrolment, when the child was in preschool (time 1) and (ii) in the final term of grade one in primary school (time 2). Jamaican preschools cater to children aged 3–6 years and children transition to primary school at age 6. We enrolled children from all classes in preschool, and hence the outcome data in grade one were collected over 4 separate years until all of the children had transitioned to primary school. Data were collected in a total of 50 primary schools and 149 classrooms. See Fig. 1 for the study profile.

Fig. 1. Study profile.

The study was approved by the University of the West Indies ethics committee, and written informed consent was obtained from all school principals, teachers and parents in the study. Child assent was not obtained due to the young age of the children in the study (Wendler, Reference Wendler2006).

Measures

All data were collected as part of an intervention trial, and outcomes across multiple informants in school and home environments were used to evaluate intervention effectiveness (Scott, Reference Scott2001). Baseline measures of child behaviour were conducted when the children were in preschool and included conduct problems by independent observation, teacher and parent report and social skills by teacher and parent report. Parents also reported on their use of harsh punishment and their involvement with their child at time 1. Baseline measures of child behaviour at home and parents’ use of harsh punishment were conducted in the summer term (May/June) and the measures of child behaviour at school were conducted in the autumn term (October/November). Outcome measures at time 2 included conduct problems by independent observation, teacher and parent report, social skills by teacher and parent report, academic achievement and language skills by direct testing and self-regulation by tester ratings of children's behaviour during the test session (Table 1). All outcome measures were conducted in the summer term of grade one of primary school.

Table 1. Child assessments conducted in preschool and/or primary school.

a Full details of assessment is given in text.

b For the child achievement and language tests, the raw scores were used, rather than the grade based norms, as the tests have not been normed in Jamaica.

All questionnaires were interviewer administered. Eleven research assistants collected the outcome data for this study. Two research assistants administered the parent interviews, three conducted teacher interviews, three conducted child tests and three conducted child observations. Inter-rater reliabilities were calculated between the trainer and data collectors for all measures during training and for a minimum of 10% of all measures during the study. For the questionnaires and child tests, inter-interviewer/tester intraclass correlation coefficients (ICCs) were >0.95. Details of inter-observer reliabilities for observations are provided below. The parent interview was conducted with the primary caregiver, defined as the mother if she spent a minimum of four nights a week with the child. Parent interviews were conducted at the parents’ home; teacher interviews and child tests were conducted at school. All measures were conducted by research staff blind to the study hypothesis and design.

Table 1 provides information of the measures used at each time point. All outcome measures had good internal consistency (Cronbach's α: mean  =  0.86, range 0.71–0.97) and test–retest reliability over 2 weeks (ICC: mean  =  0.90, range 0.75–0.99). Full details of the psychometric properties of the measures are given in web tables 1 and 2. Further details of the measures of observed conduct problems and parents’ use of harsh punishment are provided below.

Observed conduct problems

Preschool

Within each class, three children were observed for 5 min each on a rotational basis for a total of 15 min per day per child over 4 days to give a total of 1 hr of observation. Event sampling was used to record aggressive/disruptive behaviours (e.g. hitting, throwing objects) and expressed as frequency per hour. Instantaneous sampling (i.e. recording whether or not the behaviour occurred at each sample point) was used to code disruptive behaviours (e.g. out of seat, shouting) at 15-s intervals, with a maximum possible score of 240. The aggressive/destructive and disruptive behaviours chosen were based on the Dyadic Parent–Child Interaction Coding System (DPICS) (Eyberg & Robinson, Reference Eyberg and Robinson1981) and Multi-Option Observation System for Experimental Studies (MOOSES) (Tapp et al. Reference Tapp, Wehby and Ellis1995) behaviour categories, operationalised for the Jamaican context. All behaviours were defined in a manual. In addition to the measures of aggressive and disruptive behaviours described above, at the end of each 5-min period, observers rated child behaviour on four seven-point rating scales (see Table 1). Higher scores indicate higher levels of the behaviours. The mean score over 12 5-min intervals for each rating scale was used in the analyses. Observations were conducted by four research assistants and inter-observer reliabilities (ICCs) were: median 0.83 (0.67–0.91) during training and 0.83 (0.67–0.91) during the study.

Primary school

The observation schedule in primary school used the same measures as those described above except that observations were conducted over 2 school days for 30 min each day (rather than over 4 school days for 15 min each day). When there was more than one target child in the class, children were observed in 5-min intervals on a rotational basis with either two or three children being observed during each observation period until each child had been observed for a total of 30 min. This was then repeated on day 2. When there was only one target child per class, children were observed for 5 min out of every 10–15 min. Observations were conducted by three research assistants with only one observer present in a classroom at a time. Inter-observer reliabilities were calculated for 5-min observation intervals and the ICCs were median 0.93 (range 0.90–0.95) during training and [0.93 (0.84–0.97)] during the study.

Parents’ use of harsh punishment

Harsh punishment was defined as the use of psychologically and physically violent discipline practices and was measured using items adapted from the Parent–Child Conflict Tactics Scale (CTSPC) (Straus et al. Reference Straus, Hamby, Finkelhor, Moore and Runyan1998). The psychological aggression, corporal punishment and physical abuse subscales of the CTSPC were piloted with mothers of children who were not in the study to ensure they were appropriate and understandable. After piloting, several questions were adapted and two items were removed as they were considered too offensive to parents (and could lead to reduced cooperation in the intervention trial). The final scale included four physically violent behaviours and three psychologically aggressive responses. Parents were asked if there was anyone else in the home who disciplines their child regularly, and if so, their name was included in the question – e.g. ‘Do you (or grandma) ever pinch (child's name)?’ Parents were then asked whether they used each form of punishment and if so, how often they had used it in the past month. Scores ranged from 0  =  no, 1  =  not in the past month, 2  =  a few times in the past month, 3  =  once or twice a week and 4  =  almost every day. Internal reliability of the scale was adequate (α = 0.64).

Analysis

Multilevel multiple regression analyses were used to determine the association between frequency of parents’ use of harsh punishment on child outcomes in grade one of primary school, controlling for child age and sex, socio-economic status (SES), mothers’ involvement with child, maternal education and baseline score where available. Multilevel multiple regression models are the appropriate form of analysis for clustered data where outcomes are observed at level 1 (children), who are taught in classrooms (level 2), nested in schools (level 3). Random intercept models were used. The dependent variables were child conduct problems by observation, teacher and parent report, child social skills by teacher and parent report, child academic achievement and oral language skills by direct testing and tester ratings of child attention and child impulse control. In all analyses, child age and sex, SES and mothers’ involvement with child at time 1 (preschool), maternal education, dummy variables to control for data collector, intervention group, baseline score (for measures of child conduct problems and social skills at home and at school) and parents’ use of harsh punishment at time 1 were entered as fixed effects, and school and classroom were entered as random effects. All multilevel analyses were conducted in MLWin (version 2.10) (Rasbash et al. Reference Rasbash, Charlton, Browne, Healy and Cameron2009).

Exploratory factor analysis with varimax rotation was used to reduce the number of outcome variables with separate factor analyses conducted for child observed behaviour, academic achievement and self-regulation during the test. One factor was produced from the observed child behaviour variables (web Table 3) and the academic achievement tests (web Table 4). Factor analysis of the rating scales from the Preschool Self-Regulation Assessment (PSRA), using varimax rotation, produced two factors: a factor reflecting child attention and a factor reflecting child impulse control (web Table 5). The factor scores for these four outcome variables [(1) observed conduct problems, (2) academic achievement, (3) attention and (4) impulse control) were saved as regression scores in SPSS (v. 23) and used in the analyses (DiStefano et al. Reference DiStefano, Zh and Mîndrilă2009). Teacher and parent-reported child conduct problems and social skills were each measured using a single scale and the raw score of each scale was used in the analyses. The scores of the oral language scales (following direction and story recall) were standardised and summed to give a measure of child oral language skills (Woodcock et al. Reference Woodcock, McGrew and Mather2001).

Covariates were computed as follows. A factor score of three variables measured in preschool, number of possessions, sanitation and crowding, was used as a measure of SES. Mothers were asked if they had completed primary school, middle school or high school (Table 1), and maternal education was coded as a binary variable representing completed high school or not (0  =  no, 1  =  yes). Dummy variables were created to represent tester, observer, parent interviewer and teacher interviewer at time 2. These variables were entered as control variables in all regressions.

Results

Sample characteristics

Children had a mean age of 6.9 years and over 60% were boys (Table 2). The mother was the primary caregiver for 79% of children. A relatively high proportion of these children were currently in the clinical range for conduct problems by teacher and parent report (approximately one-third of the sample). These children were selected because they had the highest levels of behaviour problems during the preschool years (nearly 55% were in the clinical range for conduct problems by teacher report in preschool). Children had a mean age of 4.24 years when baseline assessments were conducted. Raw scores on all outcome measures are given in Table 3.

Table 2. Child and caregiver characteristics at time 1 (preschool) and time 2 (grade 1 of primary school).

a Above cut-off (>150) on Sutter–Eyberg Student Behaviour Inventory (SESBI) intensity scale.

b Above cut-off (>130) on Eyberg Child Behaviour Inventory (ECBI) intensity scale.

c Above cut-off on SESBI and ECBI intensity scales.

d Number of possessions from a list of 15 items: stove, fridge, washing machine, sofa or soft chair, mobile telephone, landline, radio, CD player, TV, Cable TV, DVD player, computer, bicycle, motorbike, motor car.

e Number of people per room.

f Type of toilet and water supply.

g Min score  =  0, max score  =  28.

h Min score  =  0, max score  =  32.

Table 3. Raw scores of child outcomes measured in grade 1 of primary school.

SESBI, Sutter–Eyberg School Behavior Inventory; SSBS, Student School Behaviour Scales; SDQ, Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire; ECBI, Eyberg Child Behavior Inventory.

a Event sampling: number of events in 1 h.

b Instantaneous sampling at 15 s intervals over a total of 1 h (max possible score  =  240).

c Mean of 12 ratings conducted every 5 min on a seven-point scale, where 0 is low and 7 is high.

d Raw scores are presented and were used in the analyses.

e Sum of five ratings on a four-point scale (0–3): not distracted, pays attention, sustains concentration, does not daydream and careful.

f Sum of five ratings on a four-point scale (0–3): does not interrupt, refrains from touching, thinks and plans, no difficulty waiting, remains in seat.

Parents’ use of harsh punishment

All parents reported using harsh punishment with their child when they were in preschool (time 1) and scores ranged from 1 to 23. The most commonly used punishment was slapping on the bottom, hand, arm or leg with approximately 50% of parents reporting slapping their child at least once a week in the past month (Table 4).

Table 4. Parents’ use of harsh punishment with 3–6 years old children.

Values are n (%).

Parents’ use of harsh punishment at time 1 and child outcomes in primary school

More frequent harsh punishment at time 1 significantly predicted growth in conduct problems over time by independent observation (p  =  0.037), teacher (p  =  0.044) and parent (p  =  0.018) report (Table 5). More frequent use of harsh punishment by parents at time 1 also significantly predicted declining social skills from preschool to grade one of primary school by teacher (p  =  0.024) and parent (p  =  0.014) report. Frequent harsh punishment by parents during the preschool years also predicted poorer attention skills (p  =  0.049) but no associations were found with child impulse control (p  =  0.738), academic achievement (p  =  0.225) and oral language skills (p  =  0.703) (Table 5).

Table 5. Multilevel regression analyses showing longitudinal associations between parent's use of harsh punishment at time 1 (preschool) and child behaviour, academic achievement, oral language skills and executive function at time 2 (primary school) n  =  211 children.

B = regression coefficient.

*p < 0.05. All analyses control for child age and sex, intervention group, mothers’ education, SES, parent involvement with their child and data collector as fixed effects and school and classroom as random effects.

a Intra-cluster correlation coefficient.

b Potential covariates include child sex, SES, maternal involvement and mothers’ education.

c Controlling for baseline score.

d Controlling for baseline score=Preschool and Kindergarten Behaviour Scale.

1 Factor score of 6 child observational measures (see Table 2); 2Sutter–Eyberg Student Behaviour Scale; 3Eyberg Child Behaviour Scale; 4School Social Behaviour Scales 2; 5Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire Prosocial Scale; 6sum of the standardised raw scores for story recall and following directions; 7factor score of letter word ID, reading comprehension, maths calculation, maths reasoning and spelling; 8factor score of 5 items from Preschool Self-Regulation Assessment (PSRA) rating scale: attention; 9factor score of 5 items from PSRA rating scale: impulse control.

Significant covariates for child outcomes in primary school

SES, maternal education and maternal involvement with her child at time 1and child sex were controlled for in each analysis. Higher SES was associated with reduced conduct problems at school by observation (p  =  0.037), increased social skills by teacher report (p  =  0.037) and better academic achievement (p  =  0.007), oral language skills (p  =  0.017) and attention (p  =  0.001) in grade one of primary school (Table 5). Maternal involvement with her child was associated with increased social skills by parent report (p  =  0.047) and teachers reported increased social skills for girls (p  =  0.02).

Discussion

This is the first longitudinal prospective study to examine the associations between harsh punishment during the preschool years and later child outcomes for children with conduct problems in LMIC. We found that higher frequency of parent's use of harsh punishment in preschool was associated with worsening child behaviour outcomes as children transition into grade one of primary school. The children were selected as being at high risk for developing conduct problems and children were scoring in the top 12.5% in their classroom for conduct problems in preschool. Higher frequency of parents’ use of harsh punishment predicted growth in child conduct problems and a reduction in child social skills at school and at home; harsh punishment also predicted poorer attention, although there were no effects on achievement, language skills and impulse control.

Parents’ use of harsh punishment with these high-risk children was pervasive: 99% of parents used physical violence and 84% used psychological aggression. This high prevalence of harsh punishment corresponds to other studies with parents of young children in Jamaica and the wider Caribbean (Lansford & Deater-Deckard, Reference Lansford and Deater-Deckard2012; Meeks Gardner et al. Reference Meeks Gardner, Henry-Lee, Chevannes, Thomas, Baker-Henningham, Coore, Henry-Lee and Meeks Gardner2008). When harsh punishment is normative within a culture, the effects on child outcomes may be less severe (Gershoff et al. Reference Gershoff, Grogan-Kaylor, Lansford, Chang, Zelli, Deater-Deckard and Dodge2010); however, in this study, we showed that even though harsh punishment is highly normative in Jamaica, frequency of harsh punishment by parents was associated with growth in conduct problems and deteriorating social skills over time for children with high levels of conduct problems at baseline. These results were robust across measurements of child behaviour using multiple informants and across home and school settings.

The children who participated in this study (1) had high levels of conduct problems and (2) were exposed to harsh punishment at baseline. Hence, we are unable to assess whether harsh punishment played a causal role in the development of children's conduct problems; we can only state that harsh punishment led to worsening behaviour over time. We also have no data to indicate whether these high-risk young children are particularly vulnerable to the negative effects of harsh punishment or whether similar associations would be evident with children without behaviour problems. This study also demonstrates the importance of operationalizing the measurement of harsh punishment in terms of frequency and/or severity, rather than using measures of prevalence or incidence, when conducting research in contexts in which harsh punishment is widely used.

Child SES in preschool was controlled for in all analyses and higher SES was associated with a fewer child conduct problems at school through observation and higher social skills by teacher report. Higher SES was also associated with children's academic achievement, language skills and attention. Poverty is a well-established risk factor for child development (Grantham-McGregor et al. Reference Grantham-McGregor, Cheung, Cueto, Glewwe, Richter and Strupp2007) and for mental health (Lund et al. Reference Lund, Breen, Flisher, Kakuma, Corrigall, Joska, Swartz and Patel2010).

Strengths and limitations of the study

This study has several strengths. Parents reported on their use of harsh punishment; child behaviour was measured through multiple informants including independent observations, teacher and parent report, and school achievement, oral language, child attention and child impulse control were assessed by trained research personnel. We chose well-validated and widely used teacher and parent questionnaires to measure child conduct problems and social skills. Frequency measures rather than categorical measures were used as these child behaviours fall along a continuum in the population and using frequency scores maintains the individual variability between children, increases sensitivity and reduces measurement error, especially for children with scores in a borderline range. The achievement tests were delivered in a standardised way and are likely to be more valid than using school-administered tests and more sensitive than binary and categorical measures such as grade retention, special education placement and school drop-out. All measures had good test–retest and adequate internal reliabilities, and quality control of all measures was maintained throughout the study. Attrition was low; only 6% were lost to follow-up. Several important covariates were controlled for in all analyses including child age and sex, SES, maternal education, frequency of parent involvement with child. Baseline scores for children's behaviour at school and at home were also controlled in the analyses on child behaviour.

The study also has some limitations. The small sample size limits power and there are other potential confounders that were not measured. For example, frequency of harsh punishment may covary with other risks for child behaviour problems (e.g. maternal depression, low maternal warmth and sensitivity) and children experiencing higher levels of harsh punishment may also be exposed to higher levels of other forms of violence (e.g. domestic abuse, community violence, harsh punishment at school) which are also risk factors for poor child outcomes (Baker-Henningham et al. Reference Baker-Henningham, Meeks Gardner, Chang and Walker2009; Samms-Vaughan & Lambert, Reference Samms-Vaughan and Lambert2017). We had no baseline measures for child academic achievement, oral language skills and child attention and impulse control, and hence these analyses are less robust as they do not measure change in child functioning over time. Measures of child behaviour at school were conducted in the autumn term in preschool and in the summer term in grade one. It is possible that child behaviour varies across the school year and this was not controlled for in the analyses. However, all children were evaluated over the same period at both time 1 and time 2 and baseline measures were controlled in the analysis. Parents self-reported on their use of harsh punishment and they may have under- or over-reported. However, the consistency of the results suggests that the measure had reasonable validity. The measures used in this study have not been normed in the Jamaica context; however, all outcome measures have been used previously in Jamaica, have good psychometric properties and have been shown to be sensitive to change with intervention (Baker-Henningham et al. Reference Baker-Henningham, Scott, Jones and Walker2012) or sensitive to differences between children (Baker-Henningham et al. Reference Baker-Henningham, Walker and Chang-Lopez2007). The sample included children with the highest level of conduct problems in community preschools situated in urban, disadvantaged areas, and hence the results are not generalisable to the general population.

Study implications

The high prevalence of harsh punishment by parents of young Jamaican, from this study and from previous studies (Lansford & Deater-Deckard, Reference Lansford and Deater-Deckard2012; Samms-Vaughan & Lambert, Reference Samms-Vaughan and Lambert2017), demonstrates the urgent need for appropriate parenting interventions to train parents in alternative discipline strategies and prevent violence against children. This is important not only for moral reasons, but also because of the negative effects on child development and the associated social and economic costs (Ward et al. Reference Ward, Sanders, Gardner, Mikton and Dawes2016). There is evidence that parenting programmes can reduce the risk of child maltreatment and improve parenting competencies (Chen & Chan, Reference Chen and Chan2016); however, evidence from LMIC is limited (Knerr et al. Reference Knerr, Gardner and Cluver2013). Integrating such programmes into existing services would increase access and promote programme sustainability and the most common services accessed by young children are the health and education sector. In Jamaica, over 98% of young children attend preschool and hence integrating a parenting intervention to prevent harsh punishment into the preschool network is a promising approach with potential for near universal coverage. Training Jamaican preschool teachers in appropriate discipline techniques has shown benefits to teachers’ child management practices, including reductions in harsh punishment (Baker-Henningham et al. Reference Baker-Henningham, Walker, Powell and Meeks Gardner2009b; Baker-Henningham & Walker, Reference Baker-Henningham and Walker2018) and a similar approach could be developed to train parents of preschool children as they enter school. Previous qualitative work with parents of preschool children in Jamaica has shown that although parents report frequent use of corporal punishment with their young child, they believe it is undesirable and ineffective (Baker-Henningham & Walker, Reference Baker-Henningham and Walker2009), suggesting that they would be receptive to training in behaviour management.

Conclusion

Disadvantaged, inner city, Jamaican children identified as high risk for developing conduct problems during the preschool years were exposed to high levels of harsh punishment (defined as physical violence and psychological aggression) at home. This study presents preliminary evidence that the frequency of parents’ use of harsh punishment leads to worse behaviour trajectories over time, in terms of increased conduct problems and reduced social skills at home and at school, for these young children. The high prevalence of harsh punishment and the evidence for its negative effect on children's development demonstrate an urgent need for parenting programmes to train parents in alternative discipline strategies and to prevent violence against young children in Jamaica.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/gmh.2018.21

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by The Wellcome Trust (grant numbers 080534/Z/06/Z and 094307/Z/10/Z). The authors thank the teachers, parents and children for their participation and the research assistants for data collection.

Declaration of Interest

None.

Ethical standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

References

Baker-Henningham, H, Meeks Gardner, J, Chang, S, Walker, S. (2009). Experiences of violence and deficits in academic achievement among primary school aged children in Jamaica. Child Abuse and Neglect 33, 296306.Google Scholar
Baker-Henningham, H, Scott, S, Jones, K, Walker, S (2012). Reducing child conduct problems and promoting social skills in a middle income country: cluster randomised controlled trial. British Journal of Psychiatry 201, 101108.Google Scholar
Baker-Henningham, H, Walker, S (2009). A qualitative study of teachers’ perceptions of an intervention to prevent conduct problems in Jamaican pre-schools. Child Care Health and Development 35, 632642.Google Scholar
Baker-Henningham, H, Walker, S (2018). Transporting an evidence-based, violence prevention intervention to Jamaican preschools: a cluster randomized trial. Global Mental Health 5, e7.Google Scholar
Baker-Henningham, H, Walker, S, Chang-Lopez, S (2007). Special Education Needs Study: End of Study Report. Primary Education Support Project Office, Ministry of Education and Youth: Kingston, Jamaica.Google Scholar
Baker-Henningham, H, Walker, S, Powell, C, Meeks Gardner, JM (2009b). A pilot study of the Incredible Years Teacher Training Programme and a curriculum unit on social and emotional skills in community preschools in Jamaica. Child Care Health and Development 35, 624631.Google Scholar
Black, M, Walker, S, Fernald, L, Andersen, C, DiGirolamo, A, Lu, C, Fink, G, Shawar, Y, Shiffman, J, Devercelli, A, Wodon, Q, Vargas-Baron, E, Grantham-McGregor, S for the Lancet Early Childhood Development Series Steering Committee (2016). Early childhood development coming of age: science through the life course. The Lancet 389, 7790.Google Scholar
Chen, M, Chan, KL (2016). Effects of parenting programs on child maltreatment prevention: a meta-analysis. Trauma, Violence and Abuse 17, 88104.Google Scholar
DiStefano, C, Zh, M, Mîndrilă, D (2009). Understanding and using factor scores: considerations for the applied researcher. Practical Assessment, Research & Evaluation 14, 111.Google Scholar
Eyberg, S, Robinson, EA (1981). Dyadic Parent–Child Interaction Coding System. Parenting Clinic, University of Washington: Seattle, Washington, USA.Google Scholar
Eyberg, S, Ross, A (1978). Assessment of child behavior problems: the validation of a new inventory. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology 7, 113116.Google Scholar
Gershoff, ET (2002). Corporal punishment by parents and associated child behaviors and experiences: a meta-analytic and theoretical review. Psychological Bulletin 128, 539579.Google Scholar
Gershoff, ET, Grogan-Kaylor, A, Lansford, JE, Chang, L, Zelli, A, Deater-Deckard, K, Dodge, KA (2010). Parent discipline practices in an international sample: associations with child behaviours and moderation by perceived normativeness. Child Development 81, 487502.Google Scholar
Goodman, R (1999). The extended version of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire as a guide to child psychiatric caseness and consequent burden. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 40, 791799.Google Scholar
Grantham-McGregor, S, Cheung, Y, Cueto, S, Glewwe, P, Richter, L, Strupp, B (2007). The International Child Development Steering Group. Developmental potential in the first 5 years for children in developing countries. The Lancet 369, 6070.Google Scholar
Jaffee, SR, Strait, LB, Odgers, CL (2012). From correlates to causes: can quasi-experimental studies and statistical innovations bring us closer to identifying the causes of antisocial behavior. Psychological Bulletin 138, 272295.Google Scholar
Knerr, W, Gardner, F, Cluver, L (2013). Improving positive parenting skills and reducing harsh and abusive parenting in low and middle-income countries: a systematic review. Prevention Science 14, 352363.Google Scholar
Lansford, JE, Deater-Deckard, K (2012). Childrearing discipline and violence in developing countries. Child Development 83, 6275.Google Scholar
Lund, C, Breen, A, Flisher, AJ, Kakuma, R, Corrigall, J, Joska, JA, Swartz, L, Patel, V (2010). Poverty and common mental disorders in low- and middle-income countries: a systematic review. Social Science and Medicine 7, 517528.Google Scholar
Maguire-Jack, K, Gromoske, AN, Berger, LM (2012). Spanking and child development during the first five years of life. Child Development 83, 19601977.Google Scholar
Meeks Gardner, J, Henry-Lee, A, Chevannes, P, Thomas, J, Baker-Henningham, H, Coore, C (2008). Violence against children in the Caribbean: a desk review. Promoting Child Rights Through Research: Proceedings of the Caribbean Child Research Conference 2006 (eds. Henry-Lee, A. and Meeks Gardner, J.), pp. 149165. SALISES, UWI: Kingston, Jamaica.Google Scholar
Merrell, KW (2002). School Social Behavior Scales, 2nd Edition (SSBS-2). Assessment Intervention Resources: Eugene, OR.Google Scholar
Merrell, KW (1996). Assessment of social skills and behavior problems in early childhood: the Preschool and Kindergarten Behavior Scales. Journal of Early Intervention 20, 132145.Google Scholar
Moffitt, T, Scott, S (2008). Conduct disorders in childhood and adolescence. Rutter's Child and Adolescent Psychiatry (eds. Rutter, M., Bishop, D., Pine, D., Scott, S., Stevenson, J., Taylor, E. and Thapar, A.), pp. 543564. Blackwell Publishing Inc: Oxford.Google Scholar
Norman, R, Byambaa, M, De, R, Butchart, A, Scott, J, Vos, T (2012). The long-term health consequences of child physical abuse, emotional abuse, and neglect: a systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS Medicine 9, e1001349.Google Scholar
Polanczyk, GV, Salum, GA, Sugaya, LS, Caye, A, Rohde, LA (2015). Annual Research Review: a meta-analysis of the worldwide prevalence of mental disorders in children and adolescents. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 56, 345365.Google Scholar
Rasbash, J, Charlton, C, Browne, WJ, Healy, M, Cameron, B (2009). MLwiN Version 2.10. Centre for Multilevel Modelling: University of Bristol: Bristol, UK.Google Scholar
Rayfield, A, Eyberg, S, Foote, R (1998). Revision of the Sutter-Eyberg Student Behavior Inventory: teacher ratings of conduct problem behavior. Educational and Psychological Measurement 58, 8898.Google Scholar
Romano, E, Babchishin, L, Marquis, R, Fréchette, S (2015). Childhood maltreatment and educational outcomes. Trauma, Violence & Abuse 16, 418437.Google Scholar
Samms-Vaughan, M (2005). Profiles: The Jamaican Preschool Child. The Status of Early Childhood Development in Jamaica. Planning Institute of Jamaica: Kingston, Jamaica.Google Scholar
Samms-Vaughan, M, Lambert, M (2017). The impact of polyvictimisation on children in LMICs: the case of Jamaica. Psychology, Health & Medicine 22(Supp. 1), 114.Google Scholar
Scott, S (2001). Deciding whether interventions for antisocial behaviour work: principles of outcome assessment, and practice in a multicentre trial. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 10(S1), I/59-I/70.Google Scholar
Sherr, L, Hensels, IS, Skeen, S, Tomlinson, M, Roberts, KJ, Macedo, A (2016). Exposure to violence predicts poor educational outcomes in young children in South Africa and Malawi. International Health 8, 3643.Google Scholar
Smith-Donald, R, Raver, C, Hayes, T, Richardson, B (2007). Preliminary construct and concurrent validity of the Preschool Self-Regulation Assessment (PSRA) for field-based research. Early Childhood Research Quarterly 22, 173187.Google Scholar
Straus, M, Hamby, S, Finkelhor, D, Moore, D, Runyan, D (1998). Identification of child maltreatment with the parent-child conflict tactics scales: development and psychometric data for a national sample of American parents. Child Abuse & Neglect 22, 249270.Google Scholar
Straus, M, Paschall, MJ (2009). Development of children's cognitive ability: a longitudinal study of two nationally representative age cohorts. Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment & Trauma 18, 459483.Google Scholar
Tapp, J, Wehby, JH, Ellis, D (1995). A multiple observation system for experimental studies: MOOSES. Behaviour Research Methods Instrument Computing 27, 2531.Google Scholar
UNICEF (2010). Child Disciplinary Practices at Home: Evidence from a Range of Low and Middle-Income Countries. UNICEF: NY.Google Scholar
Walker, S, Wachs, T, Grantham-McGregor, S, Black, M, Nelson, C, Huffman, S, Baker-Henningham, H, Chang, S, Hamadani, J, Lozoff, B, Meeks Gardner, J, Powell, C, Rahman, A, Richter, L (2011). Inequality begins in early childhood: risk and protective factors for early child development. The Lancet 3781, 325338.Google Scholar
Ward, C, Sanders, MR, Gardner, F, Mikton, C, Dawes, A (2016). Preventing child maltreatment in low and middle-income countries. Child Abuse and Neglect 54, 97107.Google Scholar
Webster-Stratton, C, Hammond, M (1998). Conduct problems and level of social competence in Head Start children: prevalence, pervasiveness and associated risk factors. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review 1, 101124.Google Scholar
Wendler, DS (2006). Assent in pediatric research: theoretical and practical considerations. Journal of Medical Ethics 32, 229234.Google Scholar
Woodcock, R, McGrew, K, Mather, N (2001). Woodcock-Johnson III Tests of Cognitive Abilities, 1st edn. Riverside Publishing: Itasca, IL.Google Scholar
Woodcock, R, McGrew, K, Werder, J (1994). Woodcock-McGrew-Werder Mini-Battery of Achievement. Riverside Publishing: Itasca, IL.Google Scholar
World Health Organization (1993). The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders. Diagnostic Criteria for Research. World Health Organisation: Geneva.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Study profile.

Figure 1

Table 1. Child assessments conducted in preschool and/or primary school.

Figure 2

Table 2. Child and caregiver characteristics at time 1 (preschool) and time 2 (grade 1 of primary school).

Figure 3

Table 3. Raw scores of child outcomes measured in grade 1 of primary school.

Figure 4

Table 4. Parents’ use of harsh punishment with 3–6 years old children.

Figure 5

Table 5. Multilevel regression analyses showing longitudinal associations between parent's use of harsh punishment at time 1 (preschool) and child behaviour, academic achievement, oral language skills and executive function at time 2 (primary school) n  =  211 children.

Supplementary material: File

Baker-Henningham and Francis supplementary material

Baker-Henningham and Francis supplementary material 1

Download Baker-Henningham and Francis supplementary material(File)
File 24.2 KB