Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-r5fsc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-02T16:57:35.507Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The multidimensionality of social health

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 November 2024

Ella Razital Cohn-Schwartz*
Affiliation:
Department of Epidemiology, Biostatistics, and Community Health Sciences, Faculty of Health Sciences, Ben-Gurion University, Beer-Sheva, Israel
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Commentary
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of International Psychogeriatric Association

The social health of older adults has been a major concern throughout the COVID-19 pandemic. Older adults are a main risk group for serious illness and death from the disease, and physical distancing has been one of the more effective ways to protect their health (Wu et al., Reference Wu2020). However, a potential negative side effect of physical distancing guidelines is reduced social contacts and increased loneliness among older individuals (Cohn-Schwartz et al., Reference Cohn-Schwartz, Vitman-Schorr and Khalaila2022; Hwang et al., Reference Hwang, Rabheru, Peisah, Reichman and Ikeda2020). Within this context, van der Velpen and colleagues (Reference van der Velpen2022) wrote an important paper, which sheds light on the implications of the pandemic for the social health of older adults. They examined the social health trajectories of older adults during the COVID-19 pandemic in the Netherlands and the associations between social health and sociodemographic factors, mental health, and brain structure. They measured social health via indicators of negative and positive social health; negative social health was measured as loneliness and perceived social isolation, while positive social health was reflected by social connectedness and support exchanges. The study took place over three points between April and July 2020, during a period when pandemic-related restrictions were gradually eased in the Netherlands. Trends in the data were compared to the pre-pandemic situation. The researchers applied linear mixed models and generalized estimating equations to quantify associations between the determinants and trajectories of social health.

The results of the study by van der Velpen and colleagues (Reference van der Velpen2022) indicate that loneliness doubled in prevalence at the start of the pandemic, when compared with previous years. However, this trend improved over the course of the study as social isolation and loneliness scores decreased during the three months of follow-up. Higher levels of loneliness and social isolation at the start of the pandemic were related to depressive symptoms during COVID-19 baseline, female sex, loneliness prior to the pandemic, living alone, and not owning a pet. However, despite baseline differences for specific determinants, older adults showed similar recovery from loneliness and social isolation such that their negative social health trajectories improved at a similar rate for all determinants. Social connectedness remained high throughout the first months of the pandemic. Feeling connected at the start of the pandemic was related to being older, female sex, low depressive symptoms during COVID-19 baseline and low loneliness prior to the pandemic. Larger intracranial volume was associated with increases in support exchanges. van der Velpen and colleagues conclude that studying determinants of social health trajectories during the COVID-19 pandemic may help to identify risk factors for an adverse response to change in the social environment, even outside of the context of the pandemic (Hwang et al., Reference Hwang, Rabheru, Peisah, Reichman and Ikeda2020).

This study offers several compelling advantages. One notable advantage is the use of longitudinal data from three points in time to measure the trajectories of social health during the pandemic. As the pandemic is dynamic and public health guidelines are often changing, longitudinal data are imperative for obtaining a comprehensive picture of the experiences of older adults throughout this crisis. The paper is also noteworthy for its examination of magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) of the brain performed prior to the pandemic. Indeed, the findings show that intracranial volume was associated with change in support exchanges over the course of the pandemic, such that individuals with larger intracranial volumes showed an increase in giving or receiving help, while persons with smaller intracranial volume showed a decrease in giving or receiving help. The role of brain health in later-life social functioning is receiving increasing research attention (Düzel et al., Reference Düzel2019). These findings suggest that brain reserve may play a role in how older individuals cope with challenging times and provide a basis for future research to consider how brain function is related to developments in other aspects of social health in late life.

As with any study, these findings also need to be interpreted within the context of limitations. One issue is the long-standing question of selection bias. Individuals who choose to participate in surveys might be more sociable in general and have better social health, compared to individuals with worst social health who may be less likely to participate in research and be under-represented in the study. Thus, the decline in negative social health at the pandemic onset might have been even steeper among the general population, more so than the decline observed among those who participated in the study. In addition, it is possible that not all adults have recuperated as quickly as indicated by the results. An additional issue is the need to expand this investigation into later stages of the pandemic. As restrictions were re-introduced and lifted several times since 2020 it would be intriguing to learn whether older adults experienced further changes in their social health over the course of the pandemic.

In the remainder of this commentary, I would like to reflect on some issues raised by these important findings in relation to the multidimensionality of social health. van der Velpen and colleagues (Reference van der Velpen2022) offer a comprehensive account of social health by examining both positive and negative social health. Such a perception is less often employed in studies on the social lives of older adults, as it is more common to focus only on negative social health, particularly loneliness, or only on positive social health, such as social contacts and support exchanges (Courtin and Knapp, Reference Courtin and Knapp2017; Santini et al., Reference Santini, Koyanagi, Tyrovolas, Mason and Haro2015). The studies that jointly examine both aspects often portray positive social health as a predictor of loneliness (e.g., Burholt et al., Reference Burholt, Winter, Aartsen, Constantinou and Dahlberg2019; Cohn-Schwartz et al., Reference Cohn-Schwartz, Vitman-Schorr and Khalaila2022). However, there might be merit in referring to these concepts as overlapping but different aspects of social health, similar to the concepts of positive and negative mental health (also termed mental health and mental illness; Keyes, Reference Keyes2007). Such a perception, in relation to mental illness, stipulates that having no mental illness does not necessarily entail being in good mental health, for example a person who is not depressed might not necessarily feel happy (Keyes, Reference Keyes2005). Implementing this perspective in relation to social health could entail that a person who does not feel lonely or isolated would not always be thriving socially or feel connected to others. This emphasizes the need for a more extensive accounting of social health and calls for further joint exploration of its different aspects.

This differentiation between positive and negative social health is nicely demonstrated by van der Velpen et al. (Reference van der Velpen2022). The two constructs display different trajectories – negative social health increased at the beginning of the pandemic and declined over time, while positive social health remained high throughout the study period. The two constructs were also associated with different factors. Living alone and not owning a pet emerged as risk factors for negative social health, but these factors were less relevant for positive social health. Thus, negative social health might be more strongly related to the lack of a relationship (Arpino et al., Reference Arpino, Mair, Quashie and Antczak2022), while positive social health could be related to the content of one’s relationships, which were not measured in the current study (Schwartz and Litwin, Reference Schwartz and Litwin2017). The social health of women presented a complex pattern – women reported more positive and more negative social health. Thus, women might have a lower “loneliness threshold” (Johnson and Mullins, Reference Johnson and Mullins1987) compared to men Reference Johnson and Mullinsand could need more social connections to feel less lonely.

Another avenue for future research, which will take into account the multidimensionality of social health, is disentangling the construct of support exchanges and separating it into giving and receiving support. van der Velpen et al. (Reference van der Velpen2022) combine both aspects into a single variable of giving/receiving help, following the process of factor analysis. However, it might be beneficial to refer to the two aspects separately (Nurullah, Reference Nurullah2012). While the help that adults receive is examined often in the literature (Mohd et al., Reference Mohd, Yunus, Hairi, Hairi and Choo2019), the help they provide is less frequently explored (with the exception of the extensive research on informal caregiving; Adelman et al., Reference Adelman, Tmanova, Delgado, Dion and Lachs2014). However, various forms of providing help, such as giving advice and support, may have positive implications for the emotional support provider by signaling that he or she is able and needed. Older adults who feel they are contributing to others may experience an increased sense of power and respect from their immediate environment, a sense which might otherwise be reduced as they grow older (Choi et al., Reference Choi, Stewart and Dewey2013; Matz-Costa et al., Reference Matz-Costa, Besen, Boone James and Pitt-Catsouphes2014). Examining the help that older adults give to others can emphasize the active role that adults have in their close social milieu and include beneficial psychological implications of being socially useful and productive (Thanakwang and Isaramalai, Reference Thanakwang and Isaramalai2013).

The exchange of support could further be divided into positive and negative support. While van der Velpen and colleagues (Reference van der Velpen2022) assume that giving and receiving support indicated positive social health, this might not always be the case. For example, providing support in the form of caregiving is often associated with stress and illness of a close individual and can have negative effects on mental health (Adelman et al., Reference Adelman, Tmanova, Delgado, Dion and Lachs2014). In a similar vein, receiving help can also be associated with a state of dependency, illness, and burden. Thus, future research could make more fine-grained differentiation between “negative” and “positive” support that is given and received. For example, mental health might deteriorate for adults who provide support in the form of intensive caregiving, but mental health could improve when these adults are providing support in the form of advice to younger family members.

In addition, the concept of positive social health could be expanded. Positive social health, as measured by van der Velpen and colleagues, focused on two specific aspects: feeling connected and support exchanges. However, social health is more extensive. Additional factors, such as the number of friends, social activities, and emotional closeness to others, are also significant indicators of how well a person is integrated into their social environment (Santini et al., Reference Santini, Koyanagi, Tyrovolas, Mason and Haro2015). For instance, while sense of connectedness remained high throughout the study period, frequency of social contact might have fluctuated as restrictions were issued and gradually lifted.

Nevertheless, the paper by van der Velpen and colleagues provides comprehensive insights into the state of social health during the COVID-19 pandemic. The work makes an important contribution to our understanding of the processes of social coping among older adults during this global health crisis, and it highlights the need for greater attention to the multidimensionality of social health in later life. A key practical message for clinicians is the importance of examining the different aspects of social health and how these aspects could be optimized in later life.

Conflict of interest

None.

References

Adelman, R. D., Tmanova, L. L., Delgado, D., Dion, S. and Lachs, M. S. (2014). Caregiver burden: a clinical review. JAMA, 311, 10521059. DOI 10.1001/jama.2014.304.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arpino, B., Mair, C. A., Quashie, N. T. and Antczak, R. (2022). Loneliness before and during the COVID-19 pandemic: are unpartnered and childless older adults at higher risk? European Journal of Ageing, 8, 31. DOI 10.1007/s10433-022-00718-x.Google Scholar
Burholt, V., Winter, B., Aartsen, M., Constantinou, C. and Dahlberg, L. (2019). A critical review and development of a conceptual model of exclusion from social relations for older people. European Journal of Ageing, 17, 319. DOI 10.1007/s10433-019-00506-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Choi, K.-S., Stewart, R. and Dewey, M. (2013). Participation in productive activities and depression among older Europeans: Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe (SHARE). International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 28, 11571165. DOI 10.1002/gps.3936.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohn-Schwartz, E., Vitman-Schorr, A. and Khalaila, R. (2022). Physical distancing is related to fewer electronic and in-person contacts and to increased loneliness during the COVID-19 pandemic among older Europeans. Quality of Life Research, 31, 10331042. DOI 10.1007/s11136-021-02949-4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Courtin, E. and Knapp, M. (2017). Social isolation, loneliness and health in old age: a scoping review. Health and Social Care in the Community, 25, 799812. DOI 10.1111/hsc.12311.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Düzel, S. et al. (2019). Structural brain correlates of loneliness among older adults. Scientific Reports, 9, 111. DOI 10.1038/s41598-019-49888-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hwang, T. J., Rabheru, K., Peisah, C., Reichman, W. and Ikeda, M. (2020). Loneliness and social isolation during the COVID-19 pandemic. International Psychogeriatrics, 32, 12171220. DOI 10.1017/S1041610220000988.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, D. P. and Mullins, L. C. (1987). Growing old and lonely in different societies: toward a comparative perspective. Journal of Cross-Cultural Gerontology, 2, 257275. DOI 10.1007/BF00160684.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keyes, C. L. M. (2005). Mental illness and/or mental health? Investigating axioms of the complete state model of health. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73, 539548. DOI 10.1037/0022-006X.73.3.539.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keyes, C. L. M. (2007). Promoting and protecting mental health as flourishing: a complementary strategy for improving national mental health. American Psychologist, 62, 95108. DOI 10.1037/0003-066X.62.2.95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matz-Costa, C., Besen, E., Boone James, J. and Pitt-Catsouphes, M. (2014). Differential impact of multiple levels of productive activity engagement on psychological well-being in middle and later life. The Gerontologist, 54, 277289. DOI 10.1093/geront/gns148.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mohd, T. A. M. T., Yunus, R. M., Hairi, F., Hairi, N. N. and Choo, W. Y. (2019). Social support and depression among community dwelling older adults in Asia: a systematic review. BMJ Open, 9, e026667. DOI 10.1136/bmjopen-2018-026667.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nurullah, A. (2012). Received and provided social support: a review of current evidence and future directions. American Journal of Health Studies, 27, 173188.Google Scholar
Santini, Z. I., Koyanagi, A., Tyrovolas, S., Mason, C. and Haro, J. M. (2015). The association between social relationships and depression: a systematic review. Journal of Affective Disorders, 175, 5365. DOI 10.1016/j.jad.2014.12.049.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, E. and Litwin, H. (2017). Are newly added and lost confidants in later life related to subsequent mental health? International Psychogeriatrics, 29, 20472057. DOI 10.1017/S1041610217001338.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thanakwang, K. and Isaramalai, S. A. (2013). Productive engagement in older adults: a concept analysis. Nursing and Health Sciences, 15, 124130. DOI 10.1111/nhs.12015.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van der Velpen, I. F. et al. (2022). Determinants of social health trajectories during the COVID-19 pandemic in older adults: the Rotterdam Study. International Psychogeriatrics, 115. DOI 10.1017/S1041610221002891.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wu, C. et al. (2020). Risk factors associated with acute respiratory distress syndrome and death in patients with coronavirus disease 2019 pneumonia in Wuhan, China. JAMA Internal Medicine, 180, 110. DOI 10.1001/jamainternmed.2020.0994.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed