Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dk4vv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T03:30:38.902Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Do no harm: can school mental health interventions cause iatrogenic harm?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 February 2023

Lucy Foulkes*
Affiliation:
University of Oxford, UK
Argyris Stringaris
Affiliation:
UCL, UK
*
Correspondence to Lucy Foulkes ([email protected])
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

In recent years, there have been extensive efforts in secondary schools to prevent, treat and raise awareness of adolescent mental health problems. For some adolescents, these efforts are essential and will lead to a reduction in clinical symptoms. However, it is also vital to assess whether, for others, the current approach might be causing iatrogenic harm. A growing body of quantitative research indicates that some aspects of school-based mental health interventions increase distress or clinical symptoms, relative to control activities, and qualitative work indicates that this may be partly due to the interventions themselves.

Type
Against the Stream
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of the Royal College of Psychiatrists

There is a widely held belief among academics, clinicians and policy makers that secondary schools should help to prevent and treat adolescent mental health problems. This perspective has been bolstered by an ongoing government initiative to train senior mental health leads, promote whole-school approaches to mental health and increase access to low-intensity psychological interventions in all schools. This ‘therapeutic turn’ in education makes intuitive sense, for a number of reasons. Adolescents spend many of their waking hours in school. School-based approaches increase access for those who might not otherwise seek help and mean adolescents do not need to travel to a clinical setting for treatment, which can be time-consuming or stigmatising. It is also what adolescents themselves want: a 2021 survey found that 93% of 11–19-year-olds thought the topic of mental health should be taught at school.Reference Cortina, Linehan and Sheppard1 Last, trial evidence suggests that school-based interventions can be effective at reducing mental health problems.Reference Werner-Seidler, Spanos, Calear, Perry, Torok and O'Dea2 Effect sizes are generally small but could still equate to meaningful, impactful change if scaled up across hundreds or thousands of schools.Reference Carey, Ridler, Ford and Stringaris3 Given that rates of adolescent mental health problems have increased in the past decade and that such problems often start during this age period, it makes good sense that secondary schools are viewed as ideal sites of prevention, treatment and support.

The problem with school mental health interventions

Unfortunately, this vision has not yet been translated into a reality, and we argue here that the potential benefits of school mental health interventions can also be their weaknesses. This is particularly relevant for universal interventions and approaches, in which all students are exposed to the same content (for example, whole-class lessons or school-wide awareness-raising initiatives). We argue that the generalised and widespread nature of these efforts means that some students could be taught information or strategies that are not only unhelpful or irrelevant to them but that may actively cause harm. Indeed, this concern is still relevant for some targeted small-group or one-to-one interventions. Below, we lay out emerging evidence that school-based approaches can cause harm in at least some adolescents and consider the mechanisms by which this might happen.

A growing body of quantitative research indicates that some school-based mental health interventions can cause iatrogenic harm (adverse effects from the treatment approach itself). Psychological interventions more generally can lead to a range of harms,Reference Bonell, Jamal, Melendez-Torres and Cummins4 but this research in schools specifically demonstrates an increase in internalising symptoms relative to control groups. A meta-analysis of anti-bullying interventions found that, in some studies, students who were taught cognitive–behavioural therapy (CBT) skills experienced an increase in internalising symptoms relative to control groups.Reference Guzman-Holst, Zaneva, Chessell, Creswell and Bowes5 A randomised control trial of another CBT-based school intervention also found an increase in internalising symptoms in the intervention group compared with those who had their usual lessons.Reference Andrews, Birrell, Chapman, Teesson, Newton and Allsop6 These findings tell us there were instances when, on average, a participant was worse off receiving the intervention than not receiving it – i.e. this is evidence of iatrogenic harm.

It is also important to consider whether there are subgroups of adolescents who will experience harms from interventions, which may be masked when findings are averaged. For example, a recent trial assessing mindfulness lessons in secondary schools found that overall there was no change in depressive symptoms in the intervention (or control) group, but that adolescents with elevated levels of mental health symptoms at baseline experienced a small increase in depressive symptoms after the intervention, relative to those who had their usual social-emotional teaching.Reference Montero-Marin, Allwood, Ball, Crane, De Wilde and Hinze7 This should indicate to all researchers and clinicians that even if there is evidence that a school-based intervention is effective or ineffective on average, there may still be a minority of participants to whom it can actively cause harm. Powering trials sufficiently to allow for testing of such subgroup effects is as important as it is challenging.

These findings should not be surprising. It has long been recognised that the psychological therapies on which interventions are based can cause harm in a minority of individuals, including adolescents.Reference Lilienfeld8,Reference Hayes and Za'ba9 There is also an established body of literature demonstrating harms from public health interventions.Reference Bonell, Jamal, Melendez-Torres and Cummins4 As school-based mental health interventions similarly aim to change adolescents’ thoughts, feelings or behaviours, it is reasonable that this too might have negative effects for some individuals.

Possible mechanisms

To date, there has been very little investigation into why harms such as symptom increase occur in school-based mental health interventions. Here, we speculate that one relevant mechanism might be that interventions inadvertently encourage adolescents to ruminate on their negative thoughts and emotions. Indeed, qualitative studies highlight that although some adolescents find school mental health interventions helpful, others say the focus on negative thoughts made them feel more stressed and unhappy.Reference Bastounis, Callaghan, Lykomitrou, Aubeeluck and Michail10 Relatedly, if an adolescent is encouraged to label their negative thoughts and emotions with psychological or psychiatric terminology in school interventions, this might lead to changes in self-concept (e.g. ‘I have anxiety’) and changes in behaviour (e.g. avoidance) that ultimately increase distress and other symptoms in some adolescents.Reference Foulkes and Andrews11

The unique developmental features of adolescence may also be relevant. Adolescents are especially susceptible to peer influence, and school-based mental health interventions commonly occur in groups. It is well established that adolescents can influence each other's negative moods and can learn problematic behaviour from each other (sometimes known as ‘deviancy training’). It is therefore a reasonable hypothesis that encouraging adolescents to discuss negative thoughts, feelings and behaviours in group settings, as is so common in school-based interventions, could lead to an increase in these experiences in others via peer influence and social learning.

Public health concern

The risk of iatrogenic harm and adverse effects from school-based mental interventions, even in a minority of adolescents, amounts to a potentially vast public health problem. There are over 3.5 m secondary school pupils in England. In clinical settings, approximately 3–10% of patients experience symptom deterioration during therapy (this can be due to number of factors, including iatrogenic harm but also the natural course of the disorder in those for whom the treatment is ineffective).Reference Lilienfeld8 As we have shown in a recent simulation-based study,Reference Carey, Ridler, Ford and Stringaris3 even if the number who experience symptom deterioration as a result of school-based mental health interventions is relatively small, if these approaches are scaled up nationally – as is being encouraged – this could affect hundreds of thousands of adolescents. In other words, just as statistically small positive effects can lead to large benefits for society as a whole, statistically small negative effects can lead to considerable harms at scale.

Even if school-based interventions are only ineffective, as is often the case with universal approaches in particular,Reference Caldwell, Davies, Hetrick, Palmer, Caro and López-López12 this is still a serious concern, as it amounts to an opportunity cost (i.e. foregone benefits of options not chosen). Time is taken away from other activities that could potentially be more enjoyable or more conducive to better mental health for adolescents, such as physical exercise, extra time to sleep in the morning or free time to socialise. We should be very cautious about the idea that providing any mental health intervention in a school is always better than not providing one at all.

Conclusions

There is currently a pervasive assumption that school-based mental health interventions are beneficial for all adolescents. The possibility that some individuals may deteriorate or experience harm as a result of such efforts has been almost entirely neglected. As a matter of urgency, research should begin that explores and documents what intervention harms might look like in school settings and which adolescents are most at risk. In time, all studies assessing school-based mental health interventions should measure and report cases of symptom deterioration and other adverse effects as standard, as happens with clinical trials.Reference Hayes and Za'ba9 More importantly, it should become standard to have a plan of what to do with adolescents who deteriorate during these interventions – for example, to conduct follow-up assessments and offer group or individual interventions as necessary. When there is an evidence base demonstrating which individuals are more likely to experience harm from school interventions, then more tailored, effective support can be offered. Future research should also explore the mechanisms by which iatrogenic harm and adverse effects might happen in school settings. Together, such studies will allow the field to develop school-based mental health interventions that are the most beneficial, and least harmful, for all adolescents.

About the authors

Lucy Foulkes is a Prudence Trust research fellow in the Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, UK. She is also an honorary lecturer in the Department of Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology, UCL, UK and a senior research fellow at the Anna Freud National Centre for Children and Families, UK. Argyris Stringaris is a Child and Adolescent Psychiatry in the Divisions of Psychiatry and Psychology and Language Sciences, UCL, UK, and at the 1st Department of Psychiatry, National and Kapodistrian University of Athens, Athens, Greece.

Author contributions

L.F. was responsible for the original conceptualisation of the article and wrote the first draft. Both L.F. and A.S. reviewed and edited subsequent drafts of the article. The final version of the article was seen and approved by both L.F. and A.S.

Funding

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.

Declaration of interest

None.

References

Cortina, M, Linehan, T, Sheppard, K. Working Towards Mentally Healthy Schools and FE Colleges: The Voice of Students. Anna Freud Centre, 2021.Google Scholar
Werner-Seidler, A, Spanos, S, Calear, AL, Perry, Y, Torok, M, O'Dea, B, et al. School-based depression and anxiety prevention programs: an updated systematic review and meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev 2021: 89; 102079.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carey, E, Ridler, I, Ford, T, Stringaris, A. When is a ‘small effect’ actually large and impactful? PsyArXiv [Preprint] 2023. Available from: https://psyarxiv.com/v3fjk/.Google ScholarPubMed
Bonell, C, Jamal, F, Melendez-Torres, GJ, Cummins, S. ‘Dark logic’: theorising the harmful consequences of public health interventions. J Epidemiol Community Health 2015; 69: 95–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guzman-Holst, C, Zaneva, M, Chessell, C, Creswell, C, Bowes, L. Research review: do antibullying interventions reduce internalizing symptoms? A systematic review, meta-analysis, and meta-regression exploring intervention components, moderators, and mechanisms. J Child Psychol Psychiat 2022: 63(12): 1454–65.10.1111/jcpp.13620CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andrews, JL, Birrell, L, Chapman, C, Teesson, M, Newton, N, Allsop, S, et al. Evaluating the effectiveness of a universal eHealth school-based prevention programme for depression and anxiety, and the moderating role of friendship network characteristics. Psychol Med [Epub ahead of print] 15 Jul 2022. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291722002033.Google ScholarPubMed
Montero-Marin, J, Allwood, M, Ball, S, Crane, C, De Wilde, K, Hinze, V, et al. School-based mindfulness training in early adolescence: what works, for whom and how in the MYRIAD trial? Evid Based Ment Health 2022; 25: 117–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lilienfeld, SO. Psychological treatments that cause harm. Perspect Psychol Sci 2007; 2: 5370.10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00029.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayes, D, Za'ba, N. What metrics of harm are being captured in clinical trials involving talking treatments for young people? A systematic review of registered studies on the ISRCTN. Couns Psychother Res 2022; 22: 108–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bastounis, A, Callaghan, P, Lykomitrou, F, Aubeeluck, A, Michail, M. Exploring students’ participation in universal, depression and anxiety, prevention programmes at school: a meta-aggregation. School Ment Health 2017; 9: 372–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foulkes, L, Andrews, J. Are mental health awareness efforts contributing to the rise in reported mental health problems? A call to test the prevalence inflation hypothesis. New Ideas Psychol 2023; 69: 101010.10.1016/j.newideapsych.2023.101010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caldwell, DM, Davies, SR, Hetrick, SE, Palmer, JC, Caro, P, López-López, JA, et al. School-based interventions to prevent anxiety and depression in children and young people: a systematic review and network meta-analysis. Lancet Psychiatry 2019; 6: 1011–20.10.1016/S2215-0366(19)30403-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.