Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gbm5v Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-29T17:16:28.408Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Delusional content at initial presentation to a catchment-based early intervention service for psychosis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 September 2020

Ann-Catherine Lemonde
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, McGill University Montreal, Quebec, Canada
Ridha Joober
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, McGill University; and Program for Early Intervention and Prevention of Psychoses (PEPP-Montreal), Douglas Mental Health University Institute, Quebec, Canada
Ashok Malla
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Department of Epidemiology and Biostatistics, McGill University; and Program for Early Intervention and Prevention of Psychoses (PEPP-Montreal), Douglas Mental Health University Institute, Quebec, Canada
Srividya N. Iyer
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, McGill University; and Program for Early Intervention and Prevention of Psychoses (PEPP-Montreal), Douglas Mental Health University Institute, Quebec, Canada
Martin Lepage
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, McGill University; and Douglas Mental Health University Institute, Quebec, Canada
Patricia Boksa
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, McGill University; and Douglas Mental Health University Institute, Quebec, Canada
Jai L. Shah*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, McGill University; and Program for Early Intervention and Prevention of Psychoses (PEPP-Montreal), Douglas Mental Health University Institute, Quebec, Canada
*
Correspondence: Jai L. Shah. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

During a psychotic episode, patients frequently suffer from severe maladaptive beliefs known as delusions. Despite the abundant literature investigating the simple presence or absence of these beliefs, there exists little detailed knowledge regarding their actual content and severity at the onset of illness.

Aims

This study reports on delusions during the initiation of indicated treatment for first-episode psychosis (FEP).

Method

Data were systematically collected from a sample of 636 patients entering a catchment-based early intervention service for FEP. The average severity and frequency of each delusional theme at baseline was reported with the Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms. Delusional severity (globally and per theme) was examined across a number of sociodemographic and clinical variables.

Results

Delusions were present in the vast majority of individuals experiencing onset of FEP (94%), with persecutory (77.7%) being the most common theme. Persecutory delusions remained consistent in severity across diagnoses, but were more severe with older age at onset of FEP. No meaningful differences in delusional severity were observed across gender, affective versus non-affective psychosis, or presence/absence of substance use disorder. Globally, delusion severity was associated with anxiety, but not depression. Delusions commonly referred to as passivity experiences were related to hallucinatory experiences.

Conclusions

This community sample offers a rare clinical lens into the severity and content of delusions in FEP. Although delusional severity was consistent across certain sociodemographic and clinical variables, this was not always the case. Future research should now consider the course of delusion themes over time.

Type
Paper
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of the Royal College of Psychiatrists

In psychiatric settings, delusions have traditionally been defined as false but fixed beliefs even in the presence of clear and contradictory evidence.1 There is a long history of examining delusional content,Reference Sass, Pieknos, Fulford, Davies, Gipps, Graham, Sadler, Stanghellini and Thornton2 which is known to reflect socially salient themes (e.g. persecution, grandiosity) and typically cannot be explained by one's cultural background alone. Although delusions are a core feature of schizophrenia, they are also seen in other psychotic and psychiatric disorders. More recent literature has demonstrated that defining features of delusions are also apparent in strongly held beliefs seen in healthy individuals (e.g. political ideologies),Reference Bortolotti3 leading some to abandon the long-standing definition of false but fixed beliefs, instead focusing on components such as help-seeking, distress and risk. The current debate surrounding the definition of delusions in part indicates the need for improved understanding of this phenomenon.

Well-established delusional systems seen in later episodes, or chronic psychoses, may contain ‘secondary’ delusions,Reference Oyebode4 whereas the early phases of delusion formation are, at least in theory, more likely to include ‘primary’ delusions, emerging from intuitions, percepts, memory and atmosphere.Reference Oyebode4,Reference Jaspers5 However, despite the considerable attention delusions have received in clinical settings, far less has been reported about their underlying content and severity during the formative stages of illness. Investigating delusions in early clinical samples is critical because relatively young, treatment-naïve presentations are less likely to be confounded by the effects of long-term illness or previous interventions. Improved knowledge on the content and severity of delusions may also have implications for diagnostic classification and personalising therapeutic options.Reference Picardi, Fonzi, Pallagrosi, Gigantesco and Biondi6Reference Corlett, Taylor, Wang, Fletcher and Krystal8

Delusions in early psychosis

The limited work previously undertaken has found relatively high rates of delusions during first-episode psychosis (FEP), with the most common themes being persecutory, referential and grandiose.Reference Paolini, Moretti and Compton7,Reference Rajapakse, Garcia-Rosales, Weerawardene, Cotton and Fraser9,Reference Ellersgaard, Mors, Thorup, Jorgensen, Jeppesen and Nordentoft10 However, this literature is limited by small sample sizes, selected samples and potential confounds (such as treatment effects or lack of clarity regarding antipsychotic medication exposure). A brief Australian report examining delusions in FEP was catchment-based, but had a modest study sample (N = 143) and no mention of prior antipsychotic medication use.Reference Rajapakse, Garcia-Rosales, Weerawardene, Cotton and Fraser9 Studies with larger samples (N = 245) have had disproportionately high numbers of Black menReference Paolini, Moretti and Compton7 or focused on nonaffective psychoses alone.Reference Ellersgaard, Mors, Thorup, Jorgensen, Jeppesen and Nordentoft10Reference Vazquez-Barquero, Lastra, Cuesta Nunez, Herrera Castanedo and Dunn12 In contrast, an inclusive approach that encompasses both non-affective and affective psychoses (e.g. bipolar affective disorder or major depressive disorder with psychotic features) may provide valuable insight into how symptoms such as delusions emerge and are maintained, and whether diagnostic grouping affects these processes: although certain delusional content has been associated with mood, delusions of persecution appear to be broadly dispersed across diagnostic categories in adult patients.Reference Picardi, Fonzi, Pallagrosi, Gigantesco and Biondi6 Regarding sociodemographic factors, FEP reports have shown a link between persecutory delusions and older age at onset of FEP, but not gender.Reference Paolini, Moretti and Compton7,Reference Hafner, Maurer, Loffler and Riecher-Rossler13 In summary, previous literature on delusional content and severity would be notably strengthened by data from a large, representative and well-characterised early psychosis population.

Although the links between symptoms (e.g. anxiety, hallucinations) and persecutory delusions or delusions as a whole have been researched,Reference Hartley, Barrowclough and Haddock14 few have rigorously examined the association between other delusion themes (e.g. grandiosity, mind reading) and symptoms,Reference Kimhy, Goetz, Yale, Corcoran and Malaspina15,Reference Gutierrez-Lobos, Schmid-Siegel, Bankier and Walter16 and even fewer within FEP populations.Reference Paolini, Moretti and Compton7 For example, a systematic review demonstrated links between depression, anxiety and overall delusion severity; however, they also called for future research to consider specific subtypes of psychotic symptoms experienced.Reference Hartley, Barrowclough and Haddock14 Furthermore, certain delusion themes that have commonly been considered as passivity experiences and/or thought alienation may emerge and persist differently than other themes.Reference Oyebode4 One indicator is that these experiences appear to have a distinct association with hallucinations.Reference Paolini, Moretti and Compton7,Reference Kimhy, Goetz, Yale, Corcoran and Malaspina15 A more detailed view of the association between clinical factors and delusion severity, both globally and per theme, in a larger and more representative sample may improve psychological models and, ultimately, interventions.

Aims

The aim of this study was to systematically examine and describe delusional content and severity, and its clinical and sociodemographic characteristics, in a large, minimally medicated and catchment-based clinical population of individuals experiencing an affective or nonaffective FEP. We hypothesized that (a) persecutory delusions would be the most common delusion theme across non-affective and affective psychoses in this representative sample, and that their severity would be positively associated with age at onset of FEP; (b) severity of anxiety and depression would be positively associated with global severity of delusions and (c) that passivity experiences and/or thought alienation (e.g. thought insertion, thought withdrawal, thought broadcasting and being controlled) would be positively correlated with hallucinations.

Method

Setting

The study took place at the Prevention and Early Intervention Program for Psychosis (PEPP-Montreal). PEPP-Montreal is open to youths who are experiencing an FEP within a geographically defined catchment area of roughly 350,000 individuals in an urban setting in south-west Montreal, Canada.Reference Iyer, Jordan, MacDonald, Joober and Malla17 There are no competing public or private early intervention services in the same catchment; all individuals identified as needing treatment for an early-phase psychosis are referred to PEPP-Montreal, thus forming a near-treated incidence sample.Reference Edwards, Rodrigues and Anderson18 Following intake to the service, PEPP-Montreal provides a comprehensive standardised assessment battery, with longitudinal follow-up for 2 years of treatment. Written informed consent was obtained from all patients to participate in an evaluation and care protocol and as part of various longitudinal FEP outcomes studies, approved by the Institutional Review Board for Human Subjects Research of the Douglas Hospital Research Centre. For those under 18 years of age, consent was obtained from parents or guardians.

Study population

Inclusion criteria for PEPP-Montreal are age 14–35 years at the time of referral, diagnosis of an affective or non-affective psychotic illness with the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID-IV), and fluency in either English or French. Patients, both in-patient and out-patient, accepted to PEPP-Montreal may have received antipsychotic medications for no more than 30 days before referral. For the present sample, the average number of days on antipsychotic medication before date of entry was 2.6 days, with the mode being 0 days. Exclusion criteria were IQ < 70, psychotic illness solely related to substance intoxication or withdrawal, or an organic mental disorder. Only data collected at baseline were used for the current analyses.

Instruments and assessments

Initial assessment interviews took place within 1 month of first intake to the PEPP-Montreal clinical service. Demographic variables collected included assessments for gender (male/female), age of entry, relationship status (in partnership yes/no), education level (completed high school yes/no) and visible minority status. Visible minority status was self-reported as one of six options (Table 1)Reference Maraj, Veru, Morrison, Joober, Malla and Iyer19 that, for further analysis, was collapsed to create a binary variable for visible minority status (yes/no). Age at onset of FEP and duration of untreated psychosis (DUP) were obtained through the Circumstances of Onset and Relapse Schedule;Reference Norman, Malla, Verdi, Hassall and Fazekas20 DUP was defined as the number of weeks between the onset of threshold-level psychosis and the initiation of antipsychotic medication. The SCID-IV was used to classify each patient's diagnosis as either non-affective (schizophrenia, schizoaffective, delusional disorder, schizophreniform, brief psychotic disorder or psychosis not otherwise specified) or affective psychosis (bipolar disorder type 2, bipolar disorder type 1 manic/depressed/mixed, bipolar disorder not otherwise specified or major depressive disorder) and to determine if the individual had a comorbid substance use disorder.Reference First, Spitzer, Gibbon and Williams21 Antipsychotic medication use (number of days) before date of entry was acquired during the interview, and corroborated with available prescription information in clinical files.Reference Cassidy, Rabinovitch, Schmitz, Joober and Malla22

Table 1 Sociodemographic and clinical characteristics

Delusional content was systematically measured with the Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS).Reference Andreasen23 The scale measures severity of the following 12 delusion themes: persecutory, jealousy, sin or guilt, grandiose, religious, somatic, reference, being controlled, mind reading, thought broadcasting, thought insertion and thought withdrawal. The severity of each delusion theme is rated on a scale from zero to five, as is a global measure of severity. Severity reflects certain aspects of delusional symptoms, such as frequency, complexity, conviction, preoccupation and interference with functioning.

The SAPS was further used to measure the global rating of positive psychiatric symptoms, and separate ratings were compiled for hallucinations, bizarre behaviours and positive formal thought disorder. The Scale for Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS)Reference Andreasen24 was used to determine the global severity of negative psychiatric symptoms, along with separate ratings for affective flattening, alogia, avolition/apathy and anhedonia/asociality. Attention was excluded as it reflects cognition,Reference Vadhan, Serper, Harvey, Chou and Cancro25 and this exclusion has been shown to improve the reliability of the SANS.Reference Peralta, de Leon and Cuesta26 Furthermore, the ‘inappropriate affect’ item was removed as it has been shown to poorly correlate with the overall subscale score.Reference Andreasen27 Depression was measured with the total score from the Calgary Depression Scale (CDS) for schizophrenia.Reference Addington, Addington and Maticka-Tyndale28 Anxiety was measured with the total score from the Hamilton Rating Scale for Anxiety.Reference Riskind, Beck, Brown and Steer29

All diagnostic and symptom data were collected by rigorously trained research assistants with at least an undergraduate degree in psychology or a related health science field. The SAPS training occurred full-time over a 4- to 6-week period and included orientation, rating videotapes, role-playing, observing experienced staff leading interviews and conducting interviews under supervision. Yearly interrater reliability sessions are held to calculate intraclass correlations, and serve as continuing education for staff. Intraclass correlations for the SAPS have consistently been high over the 15 years of data collection (0.73–0.80), indicating good to excellent reliability.Reference Cicchetti30

Statistical analyses

Data from 686 consenting patients entering the programme between January 2003 and March 2018 were available for analysis. Those with no SAPS or SANS data collected (n = 11; see Supplementary Table 1 available at https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.2020.157), over the age of 35 years (n = 1), with a purely substance-induced psychosis (n = 9) or an IQ < 70 (n = 3) were excluded from further analyses. Those on medication for >30 days before the referral date were not included (n = 26), to reduce the potentially confounding effects of prolonged medication exposure on delusional context and severity; nonetheless, a post hoc analysis revealed that there were no significant differences in global delusion severity between those who had taken medication for >30 days and those below this cut-off (Supplementary Table 2). Our final sample therefore includes 636 patients.

All statistical testing was performed with SPSS Statistics version 24 for Windows.31 Patient characteristics, both sociodemographic and clinical, were summarised with appropriate descriptive analyses. To further ensure that our final sample was representative of the total population, we identified demographic variables for which >10% of information was missing, and examined global delusional severity between subgroups with and without that variable present.

Descriptive statistics were also used to report delusion severity and frequency (globally and per theme) and the co-occurrence of themes. To assess frequencies, we used the same threshold for non-remission (SAPS global scores ≥3) recommended by the Working Group on Remission in Schizophrenia.Reference Andreasen, Carpenter, Kane, Lasser, Marder and Weinberger32 For the most common delusion themes (those with ≥100 patients with SAPS global scores ≥3), Mann–Whitney U-tests and Spearman rank-order correlations were used to describe patterns between delusion severity (both globally and per theme), and demographic and clinical variables (Supplementary Table 4). A Poisson regression was used to analyse the relationship between global delusion severity and the number of delusion themes endorsed, and to determine if DUP (log transformed) predicts the number of delusion themes present (exponentiated coefficients presented). We further utilized a Spearman rank-order correlation test to demonstrate the relationship between log-transformed DUP and global delusion severity. Where appropriate, confidence intervals were calculated to better represent uncertainty in the results. All analyses were interpreted with the appropriate Bonferroni-corrected alpha levels.

Results

Sample characteristics

Sociodemographic characteristics, as well as diagnosis, comorbid substance use disorder, and antipsychotic medication use (days before date of entry) are summarised in Table 1. Of the 636 patients, 191 were female and 444 were male. The average age was 23.8 ± 4.75 years (range 14–35 years). A total of 65% were diagnosed with a non-affective psychosis (n = 412) and 27% were diagnosed with an affective psychosis (n = 172). Further, 45% (n = 286) of patients had a comorbid substance use disorder. We found no significant differences in global rating of delusion severity when missing data were >10% (Supplementary Table 3).

Delusional content

Using a SAPS global score ≥3 cut-off, representing moderate-to-severe delusions, 598 patients (94%) experienced at least one type of delusion. The most commons themes, in order, were persecutory, reference and grandiose; the least common themes were thought broadcasting, thought withdrawal and jealousy (Table 2 and Fig. 1). Delusional themes rarely occurred in isolation, with only 11.9% of patients presenting with one theme (Table 3 and Supplementary Fig. 1).

Fig. 1 Percentage of participants with FEP with a specific delusion theme. SAPS, Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms.

Table 2 Descriptive statistics of Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS) delusion items

Table 3 Descriptive statistics for multiple delusion themes

SAPS, Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms.

Delusion severity

Globally, delusions tended to present at the more severe end of the spectrum, with an overall score that was “marked” (mean 4.05, s.d. 0.96; Supplementary Fig. 2). Per theme, however, there were noticeable differences in the distribution of severity (see Supplementary Fig. 3). Although persecutory and referential delusions were left-skewed, most other themes presented as right-skewed distributions.

Delusions and sociodemographic variables

Older age at onset of FEP was associated with more severe global (rs = 0.15, 95% CI 0.06–0.22) and persecutory delusions (rs = 0.12, 95% CI 0.03–0.20), but not delusions of reference, grandiosity, religiosity, mind reading, being controlled and somatic. Visible minority status and gender were not significant predictors of global or thematic delusion severity (Supplementary Table 4).

Delusions and clinical variables

Analyses of global or thematic delusional severity with respect to affective versus non-affective psychosis, or presence/absence of substance use disorder, revealed no differences (Supplementary Table 4); similarly, the severity of all delusion themes, including persecutory, remained consistent across non-affective and affective psychoses.

The global rating for delusion severity was positively associated with anxiety, but not with depression. Anxiety was positively associated with persecutory, referential, control and somatic delusions, but not with grandiose, religious or mind reading delusions. Although there was no association between depression and global delusion severity, depression was positively associated with the severity of referential, mind reading, control and somatic delusions. Grandiose delusions were negatively associated with depression, whereas persecutory and religious delusions had no associations with depression (Table 4).

Table 4 Correlations between delusion severity and symptoms

The SANS total score was calculated minus attention and inappropriate affect. SANS, Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms.

* P < 0.006 (Bonferroni corrected).

Mind-reading delusions and delusions of control were positively correlated with hallucinations. Somatic delusions were the only other delusional theme associated with hallucinations.

For every one-unit increase in global delusion severity on the SAPS, the number of delusional themes present increased by 1.47 (95% CI 1.32–1.56, P < 0.001). However, log-transformed DUP was not associated with the global rating of delusion severity at baseline (rs = 0.04, 95% CI −0.05 to 0.12, P > 0.05), nor with the number of delusion themes present (B = 0.97, 95% CI 0.91–1.03, P > 0.05).

Discussion

In this inventory of systematically collected delusional content and severity in a catchment-based and medication-naïve sample of patients with FEP who were entering early intervention services, the vast majority of individuals presented with delusions of moderate severity, and 80.7% had at least two delusions. As hypothesised, persecutory delusions were the most common theme; these remained consistent in severity across affective and nonaffective diagnoses, and tended to be more severe with older age at onset of FEP. Global severity of delusions was associated with anxiety but not with depression, with specific relationships emerging per theme. Finally, we found that certain delusional themes that are commonly considered as passivity experiences and/or thought alienation were correlated with hallucinatory experiences.

Our results confirm that persecutory delusions are indeed the most common delusion theme in this near-treated incidence sample of patients with FEP. Unlike the selective samples, potential confounding owing to treatment effects, and/or varying levels of chronicity seen in previous reports,Reference Paolini, Moretti and Compton7,Reference Rajapakse, Garcia-Rosales, Weerawardene, Cotton and Fraser9,Reference Ellersgaard, Mors, Thorup, Jorgensen, Jeppesen and Nordentoft10,Reference Kimhy, Goetz, Yale, Corcoran and Malaspina15,Reference Gutierrez-Lobos, Schmid-Siegel, Bankier and Walter16 this early-stage, catchment-based clinical population offers a unique context for the examination of delusions. It has been hypothesised that persecutory delusions are a maladaptive defence mechanism in response to chronic environmental stress.Reference Paolini, Moretti and Compton7 Corlett et al suggested that paranoia and persecutory ideation arise from aberrant prediction errors that create waves of fear and hypervigilance,Reference Corlett, Taylor, Wang, Fletcher and Krystal8 whereas Gold and Gold explain paranoia in a social-evolutionary framework that accounts for the high prevalence of socially salient themes.Reference Gold and Gold33 It may be that persecutory beliefs emerge in an atmosphere of tension, uncertainty and uneasiness, which often accompanies the prodromal period of FEPReference Bortolotti3,Reference Jaspers5 and the potential fear and/or stigma associated with this.

As hypothesized, persecutory delusions remained consistent in severity across affective and non-affective diagnoses. Picardi et al similarly found that persecutory and somatic delusions were evenly distributed across diagnoses.Reference Picardi, Fonzi, Pallagrosi, Gigantesco and Biondi6 However, their consideration of the polarity of mood within affective psychoses may explain their finding that grandiose delusions varied over diagnostic categories. Similar patterns in delusional themes across diagnoses demonstrates the potential utility of examining their development and maintenance as transdiagnostic phenomena with common underlying cognitive mechanisms.Reference Picardi, Fonzi, Pallagrosi, Gigantesco and Biondi6 Combining this with phenomenological and qualitative analyses that incorporate a more in-depth assessment of delusional content may also be important.

Previous studies have reported that persecutory delusions are more common and severe in older patients,Reference Hafner, Maurer, Loffler and Riecher-Rossler13 even when investigating early psychosis samples with younger age ranges;Reference Paolini, Moretti and Compton7 our analyses raise the possibility that this association may be unique to persecutory delusions. Current theories suggest that such delusions are the product of a hyper-inferential state, involving the ability to surmise the intentions of others, a process that is not completely developed by early adolescence.Reference Paolini, Moretti and Compton7,Reference Galdos and van Os34 At a neurobiological level, paranoia is postulated to be associated with prediction error dysfunction in certain brain regions, which then plays a role in the ability to infer the intentions of others.Reference Corlett, Taylor, Wang, Fletcher and Krystal8 Consistent with our findings, Hafner et al showed that adolescents tend to present with undifferentiated delusions, as opposed to systematised persecutory delusions in later life.Reference Hafner, Maurer, Loffler and Riecher-Rossler13

Neurobiological and cognitive models have also suggested that delusional beliefs are continually reconsolidated and strengthened because of aberrations of synaptic plasticity.Reference Corlett, Taylor, Wang, Fletcher and Krystal8,Reference Corlett, Krystal, Taylor and Fletcher35 It is plausible that these extended prediction errors in turn influence the development of more systematised delusions. Thus, a prognostic factor such as DUP might affect the initial development and severity of delusional themes. However, in our study, DUP was not related to the global severity of baseline delusions nor to the number of delusional themes present in FEP. Future work may wish to consider a different proxy for the complexity and systematisation of the delusional system, including incorporating an analysis of the longitudinal course of delusions in early psychosis.

A systematic review previously highlighted that anxiety and depression were related to the severity, distress and content of overall psychotic symptoms.Reference Hartley, Barrowclough and Haddock14 However, the majority of research has focused on global ratings for delusions and/or solely on persecutory delusions. Although our representative sample and findings further strengthen the notion that anxiety and persecutory delusions are related, this association was not present for grandiose, religious or mind reading themes. Within the context of depression, there are mixed findings regarding the association with persecutory delusions,Reference Paolini, Moretti and Compton7,Reference Hartley, Barrowclough and Haddock14 which may reflect different approaches to assessing symptoms (both persecutory delusions and negative affect). For example, previous studies have included measures of anxiety in composite scores for depression,Reference Drake, Pickles, Bentall, Kinderman, Haddock and Tarrier36,Reference Bentall, Rowse, Shryane, Kinderman, Howard and Blackwood37 or examined specific content within the overarching persecutory theme.Reference Green, Garety, Freeman, Fowler, Bebbington and Dunn38,Reference Chadwick, Trower, Juusti-Butler and Maguire39 In addition, although the CDS distinguishes depressive and negative symptoms, previous studies measured depression through self-report questionnaires.Reference Bentall, Rowse, Shryane, Kinderman, Howard and Blackwood37Reference Chadwick, Trower, Juusti-Butler and Maguire39 Our sample also included affective psychoses whereas the CDS was designed for schizophrenia and non-affective psychoses, perhaps influencing our findings. While we did not find an association with persecutory delusions, depression was related to other themes (Table 4). Future work may wish to investigate the onset and course of these clinical states over time, to better understand their role in the early development of delusions and to study whether interventions targeting negative affect can mediate delusional severity.Reference Opoka and Lincoln40

As expected, the severity of mind reading delusions and delusions of control were positively correlated with hallucinations. Passivity experiences, characterised by the belief that one's thoughts or actions are influenced or controlled by an external agent, have traditionally been viewed as delusions. However, unlike other delusional themes, these experiences may be more consistent with perceptual disturbances.Reference Casey and Kelly41 Others have also pointed to a potential source-monitoring bias in which hallucinations and delusions of influence share certain cognitive mechanisms.Reference Paolini, Moretti and Compton7

Strength, limitations and future directions

The major strengths of this study are the unique data collected from a large, catchment-based sample with no competing public or private services, meaning that it is a reasonably representative, near-treated incidence sample of all individuals aged 14–35 years who were identified as having FEP in a defined geographic catchment. Furthermore, because of our cut-off of 30 days for medication use, the results are relatively uninfluenced by confounding factors, such as long-term treatment effects or chronicity of illness.

Limitations include the fact that although data were collected as systematically as possible, 20% of patients receiving care did not provide consent to their data being used for research purposes, and were therefore excluded from this analysis (n = 181). Unlike those with missing data, this lack of consent means that we could not examine differences between this group and those included in the study. Symptom assessments also reflect the most acute state of symptoms within the past 3 months, with corresponding potential for recall bias. A total of 21% of baseline assessments were reconstructed at a later date, using detailed chart notes. Finally, this study is based on data collected prospectively over a 15-year period, opening up the possibly of inconsistencies in data collection as a result of multiple raters. However, PEPP-Montreal minimises these limitations by using standardised assessments, rigorously training research staff and periodically establishing interrater reliability.

Future work may wish to investigate the course of delusions over time, including focusing on specific themes and/or their overlaps. Few have examined how early life factors, such as socioenvironmental context, relate to the content and severity of delusions at initial presentation for FEP. This also involves integrating smaller samples with in-depth, phenomenologically oriented interviews. Investigating associations between delusional content and sociocultural variables in FEP might prove important for identifying groups that are particularly at risk, and could therefore benefit from improved prevention and early intervention efforts.

Supplementary material

Supplementary material for this paper is available online at http://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.2020.157

Data availability

Data were requested from the curated database at the Prevention and Early Intervention Program for Psychosis (PEPP-Montreal) at the Douglas Mental Health University Institute. Once access was granted to relevant data, they were and continue to be accessible to all authors in a shared and password-protected drive. This drive is only accessible through servers at the Douglas Mental Health University Institute.

Acknowledgements

The completion of this project would not have been possible without technical support from Nicole Pawliuk and Kevin MacDonald regarding data. We sincerely appreciate the support of thesis committee members Dr Ian Gold and Dr Geneviève Gariépy.

Author contribution

A.-C.L., J.L.S., P.B. and A.M. substantially contributed to the conception or design of the manuscript. A.M., S.N.I., M.L., R.J. and J.L.S. were involved with the curation of the PEPP-Montreal database (data acquisition). A.-C.L. analysed the data under the supervision of J.L.S. and P.B., and all three authors were involved with the interpretation of the data. All authors critically drafted and revised the work for important intellectual content; furthermore, all authors take full accountably for all aspects of the manuscript, and approved the submitted version for publication.

Funding

This work was supported by the Joseph Armand Bombardier Canadian Graduate Scholarship-Master's from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (A.-C.L.), and a student award from the Fonds de Recherche du Quebec-Sante (A.-C.L); and by salary awards from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (S.N.I.), the Fonds de Recherche du Quebec–Santé (R.J., M.L., J.L.S.) and the Canada Research Chairs programme (A.M.).

Declaration of interest

None.

ICMJE forms are in the supplementary material, available online at https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.2020.157

References

American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th edn). American Psychiatric Association, 2013.Google Scholar
Sass, LA, Pieknos, E. Delusion: the phenomenological approach. In The Oxford Handbook of Philosophy and Psychiatry (eds Fulford, KWM, Davies, M, Gipps, RGT, Graham, G, Sadler, JZ, Stanghellini, G, Thornton, T): 632–57. Oxford University Press, 2013.Google Scholar
Bortolotti, L (ed.). Delusions in context. Palgrave Macmillan, 2018.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oyebode, F. Sims’ Symptoms in the Mind : Textbook of Descriptive Psychopathology (5th edn). Saunders, 2015.Google Scholar
Jaspers, K. General Psychopathology: Volume 2. Johns Hopkins University Press, 1997.Google Scholar
Picardi, A, Fonzi, L, Pallagrosi, M, Gigantesco, A, Biondi, M. Delusional themes across affective and non-affective psychoses. Front Psychiatry 2018; 9: 132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paolini, E, Moretti, P, Compton, MT. Delusions in first-episode psychosis: principal component analysis of twelve types of delusions and demographic and clinical correlates of resulting domains. Psychiatry Res 2016; 243: 513.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corlett, PR, Taylor, JR, Wang, XJ, Fletcher, PC, Krystal, JH. Toward a neurobiology of delusions. Prog Neurobiol 2010; 92: 345–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rajapakse, T, Garcia-Rosales, A, Weerawardene, S, Cotton, S, Fraser, R. Themes of delusions and hallucinations in first-episode psychosis. Early Interv Psychiatry 2011; 5: 254–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellersgaard, D, Mors, O, Thorup, A, Jorgensen, P, Jeppesen, P, Nordentoft, M. Prospective study of the course of delusional themes in first-episode non-affective psychosis. Early Interv Psychiatry 2014; 8: 340–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Compton, MT, Potts, AA, Wan, CR, Ionescu, DF. Which came first, delusions or hallucinations? An exploration of clinical differences among patients with first-episode psychosis based on patterns of emergence of positive symptoms. Psychiatry Res 2012; 200: 702–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vazquez-Barquero, JL, Lastra, I, Cuesta Nunez, MJ, Herrera Castanedo, S, Dunn, G. Patterns of positive and negative symptoms in first episode schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry 1996; 168: 693701.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hafner, H, Maurer, K, Loffler, W, Riecher-Rossler, A. The influence of age and sex on the onset and early course of schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry 1993; 162: 80–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hartley, S, Barrowclough, C, Haddock, G. Anxiety and depression in psychosis: a systematic review of associations with positive psychotic symptoms. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2013; 128: 327–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kimhy, D, Goetz, R, Yale, S, Corcoran, C, Malaspina, D. Delusions in individuals with schizophrenia: factor structure, clinical correlates, and putative neurobiology. Psychopathology 2005; 38: 338–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gutierrez-Lobos, K, Schmid-Siegel, B, Bankier, B, Walter, H. Delusions in first-admitted patients: gender, themes and diagnoses. Psychopathology 2001; 34: 17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iyer, S, Jordan, G, MacDonald, K, Joober, R, Malla, A. Early intervention for psychosis: a Canadian perspective. J Nerv Ment Dis 2015; 203: 356–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edwards, J, Rodrigues, R, Anderson, KK. Framing the incidence of psychotic disorders: the case for context. Psychol Med 2019; 49: 2637–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maraj, A, Veru, F, Morrison, L, Joober, R, Malla, A, Iyer, S, et al. Disengagement in immigrant groups receiving services for a first episode of psychosis. Schizophr Res 2018; 193: 399405.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norman, RM, Malla, AK, Verdi, MB, Hassall, LD, Fazekas, C. Understanding delay in treatment for first-episode psychosis. Psychol Med 2004; 34: 255–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
First, MB, Spitzer, RL, Gibbon, M, Williams, JBW. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Patient Edition with Psychotic Screen (SCID-I/P W/PSY SCREEN). New York State Psychiatric Institute, 2002.Google Scholar
Cassidy, CM, Rabinovitch, M, Schmitz, N, Joober, R, Malla, A. A comparison study of multiple measures of adherence to antipsychotic medication in first-episode psychosis. J Clin Psychopharmacol 2010; 30: 64–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andreasen, NC. Scale for the assessment of positive symptoms. Iowa City, University of Iowa, 1984.Google Scholar
Andreasen, NC. Scale for the assessment of negative symptoms. Iowa City, University of Iowa, 1984.Google Scholar
Vadhan, NP, Serper, MR, Harvey, PD, Chou, JC, Cancro, R. Convergent validity and neuropsychological correlates of the Schedule for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS) attention subscale. J Nerv Ment Dis 2001; 189: 637–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peralta, V, de Leon, J, Cuesta, MJ. Are there more than two syndromes in schizophrenia? A critique of the positive-negative dichotomy. Br J Psychiatry 1992; 161: 335–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andreasen, NC. Negative symptoms in schizophrenia. Definition and reliability. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1982; 39: 784–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Addington, D, Addington, J, Maticka-Tyndale, E. Assessing depression in schizophrenia: the Calgary Depression Scale. Br J Psychiatry Suppl 1993; 22: 3944.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riskind, JH, Beck, AT, Brown, G, Steer, RA. Taking the measure of anxiety and depression. Validity of the reconstructed Hamilton scales. J Nerv Ment Dis 1987; 175: 474–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, DV. Guidelines, criteria, and rules of thumb for evaluating normed and standardized assessment instruments in psychology. Psychological Assessment 1994; 6: 284–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 24.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp; Released 2016.Google Scholar
Andreasen, NC, Carpenter, WT Jr., Kane, JM, Lasser, RA, Marder, SR, Weinberger, DR. Remission in schizophrenia: proposed criteria and rationale for consensus. Am J Psychiatry 2005; 162: 441–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gold, J, Gold, I. Suspicious Minds: How Culture Shapes Madness. Free Press, 2014.Google Scholar
Galdos, P, van Os, J. Gender, psychopathology, and development: from puberty to early adulthood. Schizophr Res 1995; 14: 105–12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corlett, PR, Krystal, JH, Taylor, JR, Fletcher, PC. Why do delusions persist? Front Hum Neurosci 2009; 3: 12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Drake, RJ, Pickles, A, Bentall, RP, Kinderman, P, Haddock, G, Tarrier, N, et al. The evolution of insight, paranoia and depression during early schizophrenia. Psychol Med 2004; 34: 285–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bentall, RP, Rowse, G, Shryane, N, Kinderman, P, Howard, R, Blackwood, N, et al. The cognitive and affective structure of paranoid delusions: a transdiagnostic investigation of patients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders and depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2009; 66: 236–47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, C, Garety, PA, Freeman, D, Fowler, D, Bebbington, P, Dunn, G, et al. Content and affect in persecutory delusions. Br J Clin Psychol 2006; 45: 561–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chadwick, PD, Trower, P, Juusti-Butler, TM, Maguire, N. Phenomenological evidence for two types of paranoia. Psychopathology 2005; 38: 327–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Opoka, SM, Ludwig L, Lincoln, TM. A systematic review of trails targeting depression and anxiety in patients with delusions. Zeitschrift für Psychologie 2018; 226: 142–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casey, P, Kelly, B. Fish's Clinical Psychopathology: Signs and Symptoms in Psychiatry. Cambridge University Press, 2019.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Sociodemographic and clinical characteristics

Figure 1

Fig. 1 Percentage of participants with FEP with a specific delusion theme. SAPS, Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms.

Figure 2

Table 2 Descriptive statistics of Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS) delusion items

Figure 3

Table 3 Descriptive statistics for multiple delusion themes

Figure 4

Table 4 Correlations between delusion severity and symptoms

Supplementary material: File

Lemonde et al. Supplementary Materials

Lemonde et al. Supplementary Materials

Download Lemonde et al. Supplementary Materials(File)
File 8.5 MB
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.