Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-mlc7c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-14T01:27:43.987Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Ageism: we are our own worst enemy

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2016

Kerry Sargent-Cox*
Affiliation:
Centre for Research on Ageing, Health and Wellbeing, Research School of Population Health, Australian National University, Canberra, ACT, 0200, Australia
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Extract

In 1969, Butler (1969) first coined the term “ageism” to confront “prejudice by one age group toward other age groups” (pp. 243). As with other “isms,” such as racism and sexism, ageism leads to bigotry and discrimination, though it is a very distinct beast in that, for the most part, other “isms” refer to those different from ourselves: distinct, mutually exclusive, and impervious groups. Conversely, age is a fluid social construct in which we are all intimately bound as we move through the lifespan, transitioning in and out of different age-groups. Unlike other “isms,” individuals negotiate shifts from the “in-group” of youth to the “out-group” of old age. Yet we are all immersed, largely unconsciously, in this ubiquitous but too often unrecognized “ism” that needs to be named and challenged.

Type
Guest Editorial
Copyright
Copyright © International Psychogeriatric Association 2016 

Introduction

In 1969, Butler (Reference Butler1969) first coined the term “ageism” to confront “prejudice by one age group toward other age groups” (pp. 243). As with other “isms,” such as racism and sexism, ageism leads to bigotry and discrimination, though it is a very distinct beast in that, for the most part, other “isms” refer to those different from ourselves: distinct, mutually exclusive, and impervious groups. Conversely, age is a fluid social construct in which we are all intimately bound as we move through the lifespan, transitioning in and out of different age-groups. Unlike other “isms,” individuals negotiate shifts from the “in-group” of youth to the “out-group” of old age. Yet we are all immersed, largely unconsciously, in this ubiquitous but too often unrecognized “ism” that needs to be named and challenged.

Scientific evidence underscores the seriousness of ageism as a social issue. This was highlighted in a recent paper examining age stereotypes in social media (Levy et al., Reference Levy, Chung, Bedford and Navrazhina2014a). Levy and colleagues found that 98.8% of Facebook group descriptions of older adults reflected negative stereotypes, with 74% of descriptions directly attacking older adults. For example, “. . . Old People . . .are a burden on society. I hate everything about them. . .” (pp.173). What is of most concern is that these descriptors were not considered in breach of Facebook's policy on hate speech, which prohibits the singling out of individuals based on race, ethnicity, national origin, religion, sex, gender, sexual orientation, disease, or disability, but not age .

Ageism reflects our infatuation with youth and beauty that buffer our fears of the losses paired with “growing old,” such as the loss of beauty, health, functioning, independence, usefulness, and ultimately, life. Palmore (Reference Palmore2015) likens ageism to a “social disease” (pp. 874), that continues to contaminate generations through oversimplified representations of older adults in our culture, media, literature, and social discourse. These representations, or age stereotypes, are predominantly negative and can reduce older people to a homogenous group defined by their weakness, sickness, and senility.

Ageism is found in all walks of life, such as healthcare, the work-place, social policy, and everyday interactions, and can directly impact older adult's health and well-being. A full review is beyond the scope here and has been previously well covered (e.g. Robb et al., Reference Robb, Chen and Haley2002; Bugental and Hehman, Reference Bugental and Hehman2007; Finklestein and Farrell, Reference Finklestein, Farrell, Shultz and Adams2007; Eymard and Douglas, Reference Eymard and Douglas2012; North and Fiske, Reference North and Fiske2012; Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Richardson, Fields and Harootyan2013; North and Fiske, Reference North and Fiske2013). Instead, I want to focus on the lesser known, indirect effects of ageism. That is, how social and cultural norms reflected in ageism and age stereotypes impact upon our attitudes, expectations, and perceptions of our aging, often in harmful and self-fulfilling ways. In other words, have we become our own worst enemy?

Impacts of ageism on health and well-being

Attitudes and assumptions surrounding health and aging are heavily embedded within social cognitions. A consistent and normative assumption is that with age comes inevitable physical and cognitive decay (Golub and Langer, Reference Golub, Langer, Aldwin, Park and Spiro2007). Golub and Langer argue such assumptions can lead to a “self-fulfilling prophecy of decline” (pp. 15). In a similar vein, Levy (Reference Levy2009) argues, in her Stereotype Embodiment theory, that social cognitions and age stereotypes become internalized and reinforced across the lifespan developing into beliefs about, expectations for, and self-perceptions of, one's own aging process. These negative attitudes toward aging are hypothesized to manifest into poor health outcomes through three pathways: psychological (through poor expectations regarding aging), physiological (for example, through suppression of the autonomic nervous system), and behavioral (reducing motivation to perform preventive health behaviors). There is strong evidence in support for this multiple pathway hypothesis. For instance, priming negative age stereotypes has been found to adversely affects older adults’ performance in a number of domains including cognitive functioning and driving performance (Levy, Reference Levy2003; Hess and Hinson, Reference Hess and Hinson2006), with this effect being maximized when the stereotype matches the task (Levy and Leifheit-Limson, Reference Levy and Leifheit-Limson2009). Exposure to negative age-stereotypes results in poorer subjective health, higher feelings of loneliness and more frequent dependency behaviors (Coudin and Alexopoulos, Reference Coudin and Alexopoulos2010), and reduced will-to-live (Marques et al., Reference Marques, Lima, Abrams and Swift2014), in older adults. It has also been shown that holding negative age-stereotypes is associated with depression and poor well-being (Rothermund, Reference Rothermund, Greve, Rothermund and Wentura2005), increases the risk of hospitalization over a ten-year period (Levy et al., Reference Levy, Slade, Chung and Gill2015), heightens cardiovascular response to stress (Levy et al., Reference Levy, Haursdorff, Hencke and Wei2000), adversely influences recovery from cardiovascular events (Levy et al., Reference Levy, Slade, May and Caracciolo2006), and is even associated with greater evidence of Alzheimer's pathology such as higher hippocampal-volume loss and greater accumulation of neurofibrillary tangles and amyloid plaques (Levy et al., Reference Levy, Ferrucci, Zonderman, Slade, Troncoso and Resnick2016).

Levy's (Reference Levy2009) theory, and others in this burgeoning field of research (e.g. Diehl's (Reference Diehl2010) Awareness of Age-Related Change theory, and Montepare's (Reference Montepare2009), subjective age and age-identity work) have led to a body of work showing the numerous manifestations of internalized age-stereotypes. Poor self-perceptions of aging have been shown to predict poor physical functioning and functional status (Levy et al., Reference Levy, Slade and Kasl2002), subjective health (Jang et al., Reference Jang, Poon and Martin2004; Moor et al., Reference Moor, Zimprich, Schmitt and Kliegel2006), cardiovascular health (Levy et al., Reference Levy, Zonderman, Slade and Ferrucci2009), and cognition (Robertson et al., Reference Robertson, King-Kallimanis and Kenny2016). In addition, a decline in self-perceptions of age have been linked to an increased risk in mortality in adults over 65 years of age over a 16-year period (Sargent-Cox et al., Reference Sargent-Cox, Anstey and Luszcz2014). The impact of poorer expectations remain after controlling for other risk factors such as age, gender, physical health, cognitive functioning, and well-being (as in Sargent-Cox et al., Reference Sargent-Cox, Anstey and Luszcz2014).

The helplessness that many older adults feel when it comes to aging and health is highlighted in a recent study of adults 80 years and over (Stewart et al., Reference Stewart, Chipperfield, Perry and Weiner2011). The authors found that participants who attributed causes of chronic illness to “old age” (i.e. heart disease, cancer, and diabetes) had significantly poorer outcomes including lower positive health behaviors (e.g. diet, exercise, rest), higher perceived health symptoms, and increased risk of mortality over two years. Of great interest, the participants of this study were more likely to rate the cause of their chronic illness to “old age” (52.9%), than to unhealthy behaviors (20.2%), providing support for the notion that, not only is the stereotype of the ill and helpless older person widespread, but that it self-perpetuates into maladaptive and harmful attitudes. As Stewart and colleagues conclude, the adage “to be old is to be ill” (pp.9) is alive and well.

Another way in which aging attitudes influence late life health and well-being is through behaviors and health beliefs. For example, stronger endorsement of negative age-stereotypes is associated with a higher likelihood of attributing health and health outcomes to an external locus of control (Sargent-Cox and Anstey, Reference Sargent-Cox and Anstey2015), an attribution process that is linked to poor health behaviors (Sarkisian et al., Reference Sarkisian, Prohaska, Wong, Hirsch and Mangione2005; Gotz et al., Reference Gotz, Hapke and Lampert2011; Helmer et al., Reference Helmer, Kramer and Mikolajcz2012; Scheffer et al., Reference Scheffer2012). Wurm and colleagues (Reference Wurm, Warner, Ziegelmann, Wolff and Schuz2013) examined the influence of self-perceptions of aging on self-regulation strategies in the face of a serious health event and found that those who had negative perceptions were less likely to use strategies that promote a healthy lifestyle and recovery.

Negative attitudes and perceptions of aging have also been shown to manifest as barriers to seeking timely healthcare treatment, particularly with conditions that are considered age related. For instance, older adults who attributed depression to aging, as opposed to illness, were more than four times likely to believe that depressive symptoms were not important to discuss with a doctor (Sarkisian et al., Reference Sarkisian, Lee-Henderson and Mangione2003).

The flip side of this phenomenon is that those who are able to maintain positive attitudes, expectations, and perceptions of their own aging are more likely to have better health and well-being outcomes in late life. Positive aging attitudes have been associated with better recovery from poor health events such as myocardial infarction (Levy et al., Reference Levy, Slade, May and Caracciolo2006) and severe disability (Levy et al., Reference Levy, Slade, Murphy and Gill2012) in older adults. Undertaking regular physical activity is more likely for middle-aged and older adults who hold more positive self-perceptions of aging (Wurm et al., Reference Wurm, Tomasik and Tesch-Römer2010). In fact, the majority of studies that have examined the influence of perceptions of aging on health and longevity have found that more positive attitudes were associated with better health outcomes and higher longevity in older adults, with effect sizes ranging between 1.025 and 3.772 (Westerhof et al., Reference Westerhof2014).

Of course, it could be argued that the direction of these associations are such that those in good health will naturally be able to maintain positive attitudes toward their own aging. However, there is evidence to suggest that, as Levy (Reference Levy2009) predicts, it is the attitudes toward aging that drive the outcome. For example, using longitudinal data to test directional hypotheses, we showed that the best-fitting model for adults 65 years and over was that which allowed self-perceptions of aging to predict change in physical functioning over time (measured as an objective physical performance battery), rather than the opposite direction (i.e. physical decline predicting self-perceptions of aging) (Sargent-Cox et al., Reference Sargent-Cox, Anstey and Luszcz2012b). Further, these results remained after controlling for demographic and health factors known to impact on both self-perceptions of aging and physical functioning in older adults. In other words, positive self-perceptions of aging were shown to be protective against decline in physical functioning over a 16-year period.

In summary, there is a growing body of evidence indicating that cultural beliefs and stereotypes that are perpetuated and reinforced across the lifespan manifest as personal beliefs and attitudes regarding aging and the aging process. In turn, these personal beliefs and attitudes are impacting health and well-being outcomes in older adults, most likely through several pathways, such as cognitive, psychological, and behavioral. Of course, the question then becomes; if our beliefs about how we age are having serious consequences for how we actually age, what can we do to ensure that we have positive and productive aging attitudes?

Combating ageism

This brief overview of the literature highlights the need to address ageism not only at a societal level, but also at the individual level. This suggests different strategies are necessary; that is, addressing negative and overly simplified representations of older adults and the aging process, while also promoting positive personal views, attitudes, and expectations of growing older.

Challenging and changing stereotypes

Once formed, stereotypes have been shown to be resistant to change (Baron-Epel and Kaplan, Reference Baron-Epel and Kaplan2001; Klein et al., Reference Klein, Council and McGuire2005), though research is providing promising interventions, strategies, and techniques. A strategy that has been shown to be highly efficacious for improving attitudes to stereotyped groups is intergroup contact (Pettigrew, Reference Pettigrew2008). It is argued that ageism is perpetuated by societal segregation where children, young- and middle-aged adults, as well as older adults are relegated to school, the workforce, and retirement or care, respectively, with limitations for contact and interaction between groups (Grefe, Reference Grefe2011). Quantity of contact between age groups (e.g. children and older adults) has been shown to improve implicit attitudes and associations, while quality of contact improves explicit attitudes (e.g. Tam et al., Reference Tam, Hewstone, Harwood, Voci and Kenworthy2006). A limitation to intergroup contact is the availability of high quality and positive direct contact between groups. Recent research has overcome this constriction by investigating the efficacy of imagined contact in changing attitudes. Improved behavioral propensities (Turner and West, Reference Turner and West2012), more projection of positive traits between groups (Stathi and Crisp, Reference Stathi and Crisp2008), and increased confidence in successful future intergroup interactions (Stathi et al., Reference Stathi, Crisp and Hogg2011) have generally been shown in imagined contact research. More specifically, imagined contact with older adults has been shown to reduce implicit (Turner and Crisp, Reference Turner and Crisp2010) and explicit negative attitudes toward older adults, as well as inter age-group anxiety and bias (Turner et al., Reference Turner, Crisp and Lambert2007).

Another well-researched strategy for reducing stereotypes is through an educational approach that aims to correct inaccurate beliefs and provide more precise information on the aged and aging through seminars, literature, and education programs. Mixed results have been found (e.g. Lohr et al., Reference Lohr, Essex and Klein1988; Fonesca et al., Reference Fonesca, Goncalves and Martin2009; Samra et al., Reference Samra, Griffiths, Cox and Conroy2013); however, it is becoming a popular approach for including in health professional curriculums (Samra et al., Reference Samra, Griffiths, Cox and Conroy2013; Sarabia-Cobo and Pfeiffer, Reference Sarabia-Cobo and Pfeiffer2015).

Improving attitudes toward own aging

Challenging negative age-stereotypes and representing the heterogeneous experiences of aging in the media is an essential step in improving the health and well-being of all of us as we age. However, this is not an overnight fix and it is necessary to identify ways to improve individuals’ attitudes and expectations of aging, particularly for older cohorts who have already internalized a lifetime of negative stereotypes. A recent meta-analysis showed that the effects of negative age-stereotypes on behaviors and performance in older adults are nearly three times larger than positive age-stereotypes (Meisner, Reference Meisner2012), highlighting the difficulty of simply offsetting negative age-stereotypes by boosting positive ones. Nonetheless, Levy et al. (Reference Levy, Pilver, Chung and Slade2014b) have shown that strengthening positive stereotypes is possible. In adults 60 years and over, they found that an implicit positive age-stereotype intervention significantly strengthened positive age-stereotypes, weakened negative age-stereotypes, improved self-perceptions of aging, and over an eight-week period, improved physical functioning.

Intervention studies that have focused on self-perceptions of aging also provide hope that change is possible and meaningful. For example, two studies have shown that positive feedback can not only improve perceptions of age, but also physical performance (Stephan et al., Reference Stephan, Chalabaev, Kotter-Grühn and Jaconelli2013) and cognitive functioning (Miche and Wahl, Reference Miche and Wahl2013).

Another avenue of research examines the impact of education on older adults’ attitudes. Providing information on the positive aspects of aging as well as correcting misconception and false beliefs can significantly improve middle-aged and older adults expectations and attitudes toward aging (Bardach et al., Reference Bardach, Gayer, Clinkinbeard, Zanjani and Watkins2010; Wolff et al., Reference Wolff, Warner, Ziegelmann and Wurm2014). Further, a randomized control trial showed that improvements in attitudes through an education intervention was also related to improvement in physical activity levels over time (Wolff et al., Reference Wolff, Warner, Ziegelmann and Wurm2014).

The notion of imagined contact theory has also been used to investigate change in attitudes toward own aging. For example, imaging interaction with a 75-year old person was associated with more positive expectations of aging for male university students, compared to a control condition (Prior and Sargent-Cox, Reference Prior and Sargent-Cox2014). In particular, this study found that the relationship between imagined contact and expectation of aging was mediated by aging anxiety.

Another potential avenue for improving attitudes toward aging may be through fostering psychological resources that have been shown to protect or promote positive attitudes. Wurm and Benyamini (Reference Wurm and Benyamini2014) found in adults aged between 40 years and 85 years, the relationship between negative self-perceptions of aging and poorer self-rated health, physical, and psychological health was moderated by optimism. In other words, those who had higher optimism had better health outcomes regardless of self-perception of aging scores. In another longitudinal study, the psychological resources of self-esteem and expectancy of control were shown to protect positive self-perceptions of aging in the face of declining physical functioning (Sargent-Cox et al., Reference Sargent-Cox, Anstey and Luszcz2012a). Similarly, high internal health locus of control have been found to be associated with lower endorsement of negative age-stereotypes (Sargent-Cox and Anstey, Reference Sargent-Cox and Anstey2015). The major limitation to this research is the ambiguity regarding directionality of the relationships, but they do provide promise and a path for future research.

The goals of addressing age-stereotypes and challenging negative attitudes regarding the aging process are important and necessary components in tackling the numerous direct and indirect effects of ageism. Nevertheless, we are still a long way from understanding the best way to tackle these. As Kotter-Grühn (Reference Kotter-Gruhn2015) points out, there remains a large gap in our knowledge. For instance, we need more research on prevention and intervention strategies to determine whether attitudes can be changed individually and at the broader societal level and the stability of effects of these interventions on change in attitudes and behaviors. We need to better understand between-person differences in the vulnerability of internalizing age stereotypes and the mechanisms that drive potential within-person changes in attitudes and self-stereotyping. We need to better understand who the most appropriate targets for interventions are, and how we can best reach those groups. And we need to decide how would we could implement and fund these programs.

However, all of this becomes a moot point until ageism itself is taken seriously. Until then, it is unlikely that the necessary knowledge will be gathered nor the necessary changes will take place. Until we really know that ageism is seriously affecting all of us and are prepared to invest resources necessary to tackle it, then we will continue to live in a society where negative views of aging go unchallenged: and as such, we all suffer the consequences.

Where to from here?

So where to from here? It is my belief that as professionals who work in the aging industry we need to take a leading role in challenging ageism. We already do this in a multitude of ways, through research, teaching, and our professional dealings every day. However, we can do more by naming ageism, challenging it, and bringing greater awareness to the repercussions.

We are embedded in an agist society, therefore we ourselves have been exposed to, and have internalized, agist beliefs to some degree. A crucial step in fighting ageism is firstly acknowledging and challenging our own attitudes and views about aging. As recently highlighted by John Beard, Director of the World Health Organization's Department of Ageing and Life Course, the majority of us are unaware of the age-stereotypes we subconsciously hold (World Health Organisation, 2016). Introspection has been shown to be efficacious in attitudinal change (Wilson and Hodges, Reference Wilson, Hodges, Martin and Tesser1992), and while stereotypes are often automatically activated, this activation can be moderated through a number of techniques including mindfulness (Djikic et al., Reference Djikic, Langer and Stapleton2008). Without acknowledging our own views, we are susceptible to confirmation bias, whereby new information we receive is interpreted within our established belief structure, thus perpetuating and deepening our views.

Another benefit of acknowledging and challenging our own agist attitudes is that we can then monitor our language and behavior. Language reflects our assumptions, judgments, and attitudes, whether implicitly or explicitly, and conveys these beliefs to others (Gendron et al., Reference Gendron, Welleford, Inker and White2015). In fact, Ng (Reference Ng2007) argues that through our language we normalize prejudice and discrimination through verbal scripts that become part of everyday speech. For instance, the term “senior moment,” a phrase that perpetuates the idea that to be old is to be cognitively incompetent, has become normalized and acceptable in our everyday lexicon (Bonnesen and Burgess, Reference Bonnesen and Burgess2003).

A further step we can each take is to challenge and call out ageism, discrimination, and the perpetuation of age stereotypes around us. A good example comes from a recent paper by Pachana and colleagues (Pachana et al., Reference Pachana, Liddle, Peel, Beattie, Juang and Knight2015), who surveyed 157 researchers across Australia and the USA about ethics proposals for aging research. A major theme was that older populations were becoming the lost voices of research, with 53% of researchers citing problems gaining ethics due to consent and information requirements, and a further 37% lamenting concerns regarding assessing capacity and informed consent. Ethical review boards clustered aging research participants as particularly “vulnerable,” raising concerns of stereotypical assumptions of older people's capacity to participate due to fatigue, distress, or an inability to recall information. Research such as this helps us understand the bias that has, with all good intention, crept into aging research and challenges researchers to address and change these attitudes.

Conclusion

Ageism is pervasive, widely accepted, and normative in many cultures and societies (e.g. Boduroglu et al., Reference Boduroglu, Yoon, Luo and Park2006). More importantly, ageism is a form of prejudice that goes unchallenged, and even celebrated in many fields, such as in the media, advertising, through jokes and everyday language. The direct consequences of ageism that results in discrimination and prejudice in a variety of fields including the workplace, social policy, and healthcare, are reasonably well known and largely acknowledged. These consequences are multifaceted, multidimensional, and complicated, and I by no means suggest that being mindful and naming ageism and prejudice is a panacea to the problem. However, gaining insight and understanding into the more insidious indirect effects of ageism is an important step in improving our own aging process, and creating the impetus for us to be more proactive in research and action for challenge and change.

Conflict of interest

None.

References

Anderson, K. A., Richardson, V. E., Fields, N. L. and Harootyan, R. A. (2013). Inclusion or exclusion? Exploring barriers to emplyment for low-income older adults. Journal of Gerontological Social Work, 56, 318334. doi:10.1080/01634372.2013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bardach, S. H., Gayer, C. C., Clinkinbeard, T., Zanjani, F. and Watkins, J. F. (2010). The malleability of possible selves and expectations regarding aging. Educational Gerontology, 36, 407424. doi:10.1080/03601270903212393.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Epel, O. and Kaplan, G. (2001). General subjective health status or age-related subjective health status: does it make a difference? Social Science and Medicine, 53, 13731381.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boduroglu, A., Yoon, C., Luo, T. and Park, D. C. (2006). Age-related stereotypes: a comparison of American and Chinese cultures. Gerontology, 52, 324333. doi:10.1159/000097614.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bonnesen, J. L. and Burgess, E. O. (2003). Senior moments: the acceptability of an ageist phrase. Journal of Aging Studies, 18, 123142. doi:10.1016/j.jaging.2004.01.008.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bugental, D. B. and Hehman, J. A. (2007). Ageism: a review of research and policy implications. Social Issues and Policy Review, 1, 173216. doi:10.1111/j.1751-2409.2007.00007.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butler, R. N. (1969). Age-ism: another form of bigotry. The Gerontologist, 9, 243246. doi:10.1093/geront/9.4_Part_1.243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coudin, G. and Alexopoulos, T. (2010). 'Help me! I'm old!' How negative aging stereotypes create dependency in older adults. Aging and Mental Health, 14, 516523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diehl, M. (2010). Awareness of age-related change: examination of a (mostly) unexplored concept. Journal of Gerontology; Social Sciences, 65B, 340350.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Djikic, M., Langer, E. J. and Stapleton, S. F. (2008). Reducing stereotyping through mindfulness: effects on automatic stereotype-activated behaviors. Journal of Adult Development, 15, 16111. doi:10.1007/s10804-008-9040-0.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eymard, A. S. and Douglas, D. H. (2012). Ageism among health care providers and interventions to improve their attitudes toward older adults: an integrative review. Journal of Gerontological Nursing, 38, 2635. doi:10.3928/00989134-20120307-09.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Finklestein, L. M. and Farrell, S. K. (2007). An expanded view of age bias in the workplace. In Shultz, K. S. and Adams, G. A. (eds.), Ageing and Work in the 21st Century (pp. 73108). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Asc.Google Scholar
Fonesca, A., Goncalves, D. and Martin, I. (2009). Changing attitudes towards ageing and the aged amongst psychology students. European Journal of Education, 44, 455466. doi:10.1111/j.1465-3435.2009.01396.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gendron, T. L., Welleford, E. A., Inker, J. and White, J. T. (2015). The language of ageism: why we need to use words carefully. The Gerontologist, 110. doi:10.1093/geront/gnv066.Google ScholarPubMed
Golub, S. A. and Langer, E. J. (2007). Challenging assumptions about adult development: implications for the health of older adults. In Aldwin, C. M., Park, C. L. and Spiro, A. III (eds.), Handbook of Health Psychology and Aging (pp. 929). New York: The Guildford Press.Google Scholar
Gotz, M., Hapke, U. and Lampert, T. (2011). Health locus of control and health behavior: results from a nationally representative survey. Psychology, Health and Medicine, 16, 129140. doi:10.1080/13548506.2010.521570.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grefe, D. (2011). Combating ageism with narrative and intergroup contact: possibilities of intergenerational connections. Pastoral Psychology, 60, 99105. doi:10.1007/s11089-010-0280-0.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Helmer, S. M., Kramer, A. and Mikolajcz, R. T. (2012). Health-related locus of control and health behavior among university students in North Rhine, Westphalia, Germany. BMC Research Notes, 5, 703. doi:10.1186/1756-0500-5-703.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hess, T. M. and Hinson, J. T. (2006). Age-related variation in the influences of aging stereotypes on memory in adulthood. Psychology & Aging, 21, 621625.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jang, Y., Poon, L. W. and Martin, P. (2004). Individual differences in the effects of disease and disability on depressive symptoms: the role of age and subjective health. International Journal of Aging & Human Development, 59, 125137. doi:10.2190/RT1W-2HD7-KG5X-K1FB.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klein, D. A., Council, K. J. and McGuire, S. L. (2005). Education to promote postive attitudes about aging. Educational Gerontology, 31, 591601. doi:10.1080/30601270591003355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kotter-Gruhn, D. (2015). Changing negative views on aging: implications for intervention and translational research. Annual Review of Gerontology & Geriatrics, 35, 167. doi:org/10.1891/0198-8794.35.167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levy, B. (2003). Mind matters: cognitive and physical effects of aging self-stereotypes. The Journals of Gerontology, 58B, P203–P211. doi:10.1093/geronb/58.4.P203.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levy, B. (2009). Stereotype embodiment: a psychosocial approach to aging. Current Psychological Directions in Science, 18, 332336. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01662.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levy, B., Chung, P. H., Bedford, T. and Navrazhina, K. (2014a). Facebook as a site for negative age stereotypes. The Gerontologist, 54, 172176. doi:10.1093/geront/gns194.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levy, B., Ferrucci, L., Zonderman, A. B., Slade, M. D., Troncoso, J. and Resnick, S. M. (2016). A culture-brain link: negative age stereotypes predict Alzheimer's disease biomarkers. Psychology & Aging, 31, 82. doi:10.1037/pag0000062.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levy, B., Haursdorff, J. M., Hencke, R. and Wei, J. Y. (2000). Reducing cardiovascular stress with positive self-stereotypes of aging. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 55B, P205–P213. doi:10.1093/geronb/55.4.P205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levy, B. and Leifheit-Limson, E. (2009). The stereotype-matching effect: greater influence on functioning when age steretoypes correspond to outcomes. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B Psychological Science Social Science, 55B, P205–P213. doi:10.1093/geronb/55.4.P205.Google Scholar
Levy, B., Pilver, C., Chung, P. H. and Slade, M. D. (2014b). Subliminal strengthening: improving older individuals physical function over time with an implict-age-stereotype intervention. Psychological Science, 25, 21272135. doi:10.1177/0956797614551970.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levy, B., Slade, M., May, J. and Caracciolo, E. (2006). Physical recovery after acuty myocardial infarction: positive age self-stereotypes as a resource. International Journal of Aging & Human Development, 62, 285301. doi:10.2190/EJK1-1Q0D-LHGE-7A35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levy, B., Slade, M. D., Chung, P. H. and Gill, T. M. (2015). Resliency over time of elders' age stereotypes after encountering stressful events. Journals of Gerontology: B Psychological and Social Sciences, 70, 886890. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbu082.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levy, B., Slade, M. D. and Kasl, S. V. (2002). Longitudinal benefits of positive self-perception of aging on functional health. The Journals of Gerontology, 57, P409–P417. doi:10.1093/geronb/57.5.P409.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levy, B., Slade, M. D., Murphy, T. E. and Gill, T. M. (2012). Association between positive age stereotypes and recovery from disability in older persons. JAMA, 308, 19721973. doi:10.1001/jama.2012.14541.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levy, B., Zonderman, A., Slade, M. and Ferrucci, L. (2009). Age stereotypes held earlier in life predict cardiovascular events in later life. Psychological Science, 20, 296298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lohr, M. J., Essex, M. J. and Klein, M. H. (1988). The relationships of coping responses to physical health status and life satisfaction among older women. Journal of Gerontology, 43, P54–P60. Retrieved from http://E:/PhD/Journalarticles/Lohr,Essex&Klein,1988.doc.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marques, S., Lima, M. L., Abrams, D. and Swift, H. (2014). Will to live in older people's medical decisions: immediate and delayed effects of aging stereotypes. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 44, 399408. doi:10.1111/jasp.12231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meisner, B. A. (2012). A meta-analysis of positive and negative age stereotype priming effects on behavior among older adults. Journals of Gerontology: B Psychological and Social Sciences, 67, 1317. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbr062.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miche, M. and Wahl, H.-W. (2013). “Its because of my age.” The influence of experimentally increased salience of age-related changes in cogntive functioning on self-perceptions of ageing. Paper presented at the 66th Annual Sientific Meeting of the Gerontological Society of American, New Orleans, LA.Google Scholar
Montepare, J. M. (2009). Subjective Age: toward a guiding lifespan framework. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 33, 4246. doi:10.1177/0165025408095551.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moor, C., Zimprich, D., Schmitt, M. and Kliegel, M. (2006). Personality, aging self-perceptions and subjective health: a mediation model. International Journal of Aging & Human Development, 63, 241. doi:10.2190/AKRY-UM4K-PB1V-PBHF.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ng, S. H. (2007). Language-based discrimination: blatant and sublte forms. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 26, 106122. doi:10.1177/0261927X07300074.Google Scholar
North, M. S. and Fiske, S. T. (2012). An inconvenienced youth? Ageism and its potential intergenerational roots. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 982997. doi:10.1037/a0027843.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
North, M. S. and Fiske, S. T. (2013). Subtyping agesim: policy issues in succession and consumption. Social Issues and Policy, 7, 3657. doi:10.1111/j.1751-2409.2012.01042.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pachana, N. A., Liddle, J., Peel, N. M., Beattie, E., Juang, C. and Knight, B. G. (2015). Can we do better? Researchers experiences with ethical review boards on projects with later life focus. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease, 43, 701707. doi:10.3233/JAD-141956.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Palmore, E. (2015). Ageism comes of age. Journal of Gerontology: B Psychological and Social Sciences, 70, 873875. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbv079.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pettigrew, T. F. (2008). Future directions for intergroup contact theory and research. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 32, 187199. doi:10.1016/j.ijintrel.2007.12.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prior, K. and Sargent-Cox, K. (2014). Student's expectations of ageing: an evaluation of the impact of imagined intergenerational contact and the mediating role of ageing anxiety. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 55, 99104. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2014.06.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robb, C., Chen, H. and Haley, W. E. (2002). Ageism in mental health adn health care: a critical review. Journal of Clinical Gerypsychology, 8, 112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robertson, D. A., King-Kallimanis, B. and Kenny, R. A. (2016). Negative perceptions of aging predict longituidnal decline in cognitive function. Psychology & Aging, 31, 7181. doi:10.1037/pag0000061.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothermund, K. (2005). Effects of age stereotypes on self-views and adaptation. In Greve, W., Rothermund, K. and Wentura, D. (eds.), The Adaptive Self. Personal Continuity and Intentional Self-development (pp. 223242). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Samra, R., Griffiths, A., Cox, T. and Conroy, S. (2013). Changes in medical student and doctor attitudes toward older aduls after an intervention: a systematic review. Journal of American Geriatrics Society, 61, 11881196. doi:10.1111/jgs.12312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sarabia-Cobo, C. M. and Pfeiffer, C. C. (2015). Changing negative stereotypes regarding ageing in undergraduate nursing students. Nurse Education Today, 35, e60–e64. doi:10.1016/j.nedt.2015.06.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sargent-Cox, K. and Anstey, K. J. (2015). The relationship between age-steretoypes and health locus of control across adult age-groups. Psychology and Health, 30, 652670. doi:10.1080/08870446.2014.974603.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sargent-Cox, K., Anstey, K. J. and Luszcz, M. A. (2012a). Change in health and self-perceptions of ageing over 16 years: the role of psychological resources. Health Psychology, 31, 423432. doi:10.1037/a0027464.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sargent-Cox, K., Anstey, K. J. and Luszcz, M. A. (2012b). The relationship between change in self-perceptions of aging and physical functioning in older adults. Psychology and Aging, 27, 750.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sargent-Cox, K., Anstey, K. J. and Luszcz, M. A. (2014). Longitudinal change of self-perceptions of aging and mortality. Journals of Gerontology: Psychological Science, 69, 168173. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbt005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sarkisian, C. A., Lee-Henderson, M. H. and Mangione, C. M. (2003). Do depressed older adults who attribute depression to “old age” believe it is important to seek care? Journal of General Internal Medicine, 18, 10011005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sarkisian, C. A., Prohaska, T. R., Wong, M. D., Hirsch, S. H. and Mangione, C. M. (2005). The relationship between expectations of aging and physical activity among older adults. Journal of Internal Medicine, 20, 911915. doi:10.1111/j.1525-1497.2005.0204.x.Google ScholarPubMed
Scheffer, C. et al. (2012). Delay discounting, locus of controland cognitive impulsiveness independelty predict tobacco dependence treatment outcomes ina a highly dependent, lower socioeconomic group of smokers. The American Journal of Addictions, 21, 221232. doi:10.1111/j.1521-0391.2012.00224.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stathi, S. and Crisp, R. J. (2008). Imagining intergroup contact promote projection to outgroups. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 943957. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2008.02.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stathi, S., Crisp, R. J. and Hogg, M. A. (2011). Imagining intergroup contact enables member-to-group generalization. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 15, 275284. doi:10.1037/a0023752.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stephan, Y., Chalabaev, A., Kotter-Grühn, D. and Jaconelli, A. (2013). "Feeling younger, being stronger": an experimental study of subjective age and physical functioning among older adults. Journals of Gerontology: B Psychological and Social Sciences, 68, 17. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbs037.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stewart, T. L., Chipperfield, J. G., Perry, R. P. and Weiner, B. (2011). Attributing illness to 'old age:' consequences of a self-directed stereotype for health and mortality. Psychology and Health, iFirst, 117. doi:10.1080/08870446.2011.630735.Google ScholarPubMed
Tam, T., Hewstone, M., Harwood, J., Voci, A. and Kenworthy, J. (2006). Intergroup contact and grandparent-grandchild communication: the effects of self-disclosure on implicit and explicit biases against older people. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 9, 413429. doi:10.1177/1368430206064642.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turner, R. N. and Crisp, R. J. (2010). Imagining intergroup contact reduces implicit prejudice. British Journal of Social Psychology, 49, 129142. doi:10.1348/014466609X419901.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turner, R. N., Crisp, R. J. and Lambert, E. (2007). Imagining intergroup contact can improve intergroup attitudes. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 10, 427441. doi:10.1177/1368430207081533.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turner, R. N. and West, K. (2012). Behavioral consequences of imaging intergroup contact with stgmatized outgroups. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 15, 193202. doi:10.1177/1368430211418699.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Westerhof, G. J. et al. (2014). The influence of subjective aging on health and longevity: a meta-analysis of longitudinal data. Psychology & Aging, 29, 793802.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, T. D. and Hodges, S. D. (1992). Attitudes as temporary constructions. In Martin, L. L. and Tesser, A. (eds.), The Construction of Social Judgements (pp. 3766). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Wolff, J. K., Warner, L. M., Ziegelmann, J. P. and Wurm, S. (2014). What do targeting positive views on ageing add to a physical activity intervention in older adults? Results from a randomised controlled trial. Psychology & Health, 29, 915932. doi:10.1080/08870446.2014.896464.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
World Health Organisation. (2016). Discrimination and negative attitudes about ageing are bad for your health [Press release]. Available at: http://www.who.int/mediacentre/news/releases/2016/discrimination-ageing-youth/en/.Google Scholar
Wurm, S. and Benyamini, Y. (2014). Optimism buffers the detrimental effect of negative self-perceptions of ageing on physical and mental health. Psychology & Health, 29, 832848. doi:10.1080/08870446.2014.891737.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wurm, S., Tomasik, M. J. and Tesch-Römer, C. (2010). On the importance of a positive view on ageing for physical exercise among middle-aged and older adults: cross-sectional and longitudinal findings. Psychology & Health, 25, 2542. doi:10.1080/08870440802311314.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wurm, S., Warner, L. M., Ziegelmann, J. P., Wolff, J. K. and Schuz, B. E. C. (2013). How do negative self-perceptions of aging become a self-fullfilling prophecy?. Psychology & Aging, 28, 10881097. doi:10.1037/a0032845.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed