Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-g8jcs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T23:39:56.423Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Parents make the difference: a randomized-controlled trial of a parenting intervention in Liberia

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 August 2015

E. S. Puffer*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University, Box 90086, 417 Chapel Drive, Durham, NC, USA Duke Global Health Institute, Box 90519, Durham, NC, USA
E. P. Green
Affiliation:
Duke Global Health Institute, Box 90519, Durham, NC, USA
R. M. Chase
Affiliation:
Center for Child & Family Health, Duke University Medical Center, 1121 W. Chapel Hill Street, Suite 100, Durham, NC, USA
A. L. Sim
Affiliation:
International Rescue Committee, 122 East 42nd Street, New York, NY, USA
J. Zayzay
Affiliation:
International Rescue Committee, Monrovia, Liberia
E. Friis
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University, Box 90086, 417 Chapel Drive, Durham, NC, USA
E. Garcia-Rolland
Affiliation:
International Rescue Committee, 122 East 42nd Street, New York, NY, USA
L. Boone
Affiliation:
International Rescue Committee, 122 East 42nd Street, New York, NY, USA
*
*Address for correspondence: E. S. Puffer, Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University, Box 90086, 417 Chapel Drive, Durham, NC, USA and Duke Global Health Institute, Box 90519, Durham, NC, USA. (Email: [email protected])
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background.

The objective of this study was to evaluate the impact of a brief parenting intervention, ‘Parents Make the Difference‘(PMD), on parenting behaviors, quality of parent-child interactions, children's cognitive, emotional, and behavioral wellbeing, and malaria prevention behaviors in rural, post-conflict Liberia.

Methods.

A sample of 270 caregivers of children ages 3–7 were randomized into an immediate treatment group that received a 10-session parent training intervention or a wait-list control condition (1:1 allocation). Interviewers administered baseline and 1-month post-intervention surveys and conducted child-caregiver observations. Intent-to-treat estimates of the average treatment effects were calculated using ordinary least squares regression. This study was pre-registered at ClinicalTrials.gov (NCT01829815).

Results.

The program led to a 55.5% reduction in caregiver-reported use of harsh punishment practices (p < 0.001). The program also increased the use of positive behavior management strategies and improved caregiver–child interactions. The average caregiver in the treatment group reported a 4.4% increase in positive interactions (p < 0.05), while the average child of a caregiver assigned to the treatment group reported a 17.5% increase (p < 0.01). The program did not have a measurable impact on child wellbeing, cognitive skills, or household adoption of malaria prevention behaviors.

Conclusions.

PMD is a promising approach for preventing child abuse and promoting positive parent-child relationships in low-resource settings.

Type
Original Research Paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s) 2015

Introduction

Children and adolescents worldwide experience high rates of verbal, physical, and sexual abuse, often perpetrated by their caregivers. Young people in certain parts of the world seem to be at increased risk, with multi-country studies documenting the highest rates in Africa (Akmatov, Reference Akmatov2011; Stoltenborgh et al. Reference Stoltenborgh, van IJzendoorn, Euser and Bakermans-Kranenburg2011). Rates of family conflict, abuse, and a range of poor developmental outcomes seem to be even higher in post-conflict settings where children and families are faced with exposure to community-based violence and other risk factors for family stress and mental health problems, including poverty, displacement, loss of or separation from loved ones, and uncertainty about the future (Mels et al. Reference Mels, Derluyn, Broekaert and Rosseel2010; Reed et al. Reference Reed, Fazel, Jones, Panter-Brick and Stein2012). Daily stressors experienced in post-conflict settings may have as much, or potentially more, effects on mental health as war-related stress. Some findings suggest that family violence is a major predictor of mental health outcomes (Miller & Rasmussen, Reference Miller and Rasmussen2010; Panter-Brick et al. Reference Panter-Brick, Goodman, Tol and Eggerman2011), perhaps even more predictive of children's mental health problems than exposure to war violence among children (Miller & Rasmussen, Reference Miller and Rasmussen2010). Thus targeting a broader range of stressors, including mental health and family well-being, may buffer the harmful effects of conflict (Bolton & Betancourt, Reference Bolton and Betancourt2004; Akinsulure-Smith & Smith, Reference Akinsulure-Smith and Smith2012).

Child abuse has been associated with lasting emotional, behavioral, physical, and cognitive problems (Moylan et al. Reference Moylan, Herrenkohl, Sousa, Tajima, Herrenkohl and Russo2010; Mills et al. Reference Mills, Alati, O'Callaghan, Najman, Williams, Bor and Strathearn2011; Widom et al. Reference Widom, Czaja, Bentley and Johnson2012). Overly harsh and inconsistent parenting, with or without abusive behavior, are also associated with lower self-esteem and social skills, as well as higher rates of anxiety and disruptive behavior (Boudreault-Bouchard et al. Reference Boudreault-Bouchard, Dion, Hains, Vandermeerschen, Laberge and Perron2013; Yap et al. Reference Yap, Pilkington, Ryan and Jorm2013; Uji et al. Reference Uji, Sakamoto, Adachi and Kitamura2014). In post-conflict settings, harsh parenting may even moderate the effects of exposure to war on children's mental health (Catani et al. Reference Catani, Schauer and Neuner2008; Miller & Rasmussen, Reference Miller and Rasmussen2010). Parents who mistreat their children typically demonstrate limited positive interactions with their child, poor understanding of child development, low tolerance of misbehavior, and ineffective discipline strategies. Behavioral parenting programs address these risk factors by increasing skills for positive parent–child interactions and effective discipline and by providing parents with education on child development and behavior (Kaminski et al. Reference Kaminski, Valle, Filene and Boyle2008).

Parenting interventions that address abuse and evidence-based child and family psychosocial interventions in general, are scarce or non-existent in many low-income countries and particularly in conflict-affected settings (Kakuma et al. Reference Kakuma, Minas, van Ginneken, Dal Poz, Desiraju, Morris, Saxena and Scheffier2011; Kieling et al. Reference Kieling, Baker-Henningham, Belfer, Conti, Ertem, Omigbo-dun, Rohde, Srinath, Ulkuer and Rahman2011). The most commonly available services provide general psychosocial support, often using approaches that have not been empirically validated or grounded in evidence-based practices (Tol et al. Reference Tol, Barbui, Galappatti, Silove, Betancourt, Souza, Golaz and Van Ommeren2011). Prevention and early interventions are needed at the family and community levels to reduce abuse and foster positive parenting practices – protective factors that can buffer against mental health problems and other negative developmental outcomes (Engle et al. Reference Engle, Black, Behrman, Cabral de Mello, Gertler, Kapiriri, Martorell and Young2007; Betancourt & Khan, Reference Betancourt and Khan2008; Kieling et al. Reference Kieling, Baker-Henningham, Belfer, Conti, Ertem, Omigbo-dun, Rohde, Srinath, Ulkuer and Rahman2011).

Behavioral parenting interventions

Empirical evidence suggests that behavioral parent training programs can improve a range of positive caregiver and child outcomes, including child internalizing and externalizing symptoms, parent-child attachment, parenting stress, and parental self-efficacy (Eisenstadt et al. Reference Eisenstadt, Eyberg, McNeil, Newcomb and Funderburk1993; Chase & Eyberg, Reference Chase and Eyberg2008; Eyberg et al. Reference Eyberg, Nelson and Boggs2008). These programs are grounded in behavioral theory; parents are taught to use positive reinforcement strategies, such as praise, to increase appropriate child behaviors and consistent, non-corporal discipline strategies. The goal is to provide parents with concrete techniques to decrease harsh parenting, improve interactions, encourage child prosocial behavior, and promote positive child development. Among interventions backed by the strongest evidence for use with young children are Triple P (Nowak & Heinrichs, Reference Nowak and Heinrichs2008), Parent–Child Interaction Therapy (Eisenstadt et al. Reference Eisenstadt, Eyberg, McNeil, Newcomb and Funderburk1993), Incredible Years (Webster-Stratton et al. Reference Webster-Stratton, Rinaldi and Reid2011), and the Nurturing Parenting Program (Maher et al. Reference Maher, Marcynyszyn, Corwin and Hodnett2011). The magnitude of effects across these varies based on the target population and version of the program. While all primarily behavioral, these programs have unique elements in content and delivery. For delivery, they each require different combinations of inputs, such as varying training models, facilitator requirements, or materials; this influences the settings for which they are appropriate and the degree to which adaptations to implementation plans are required across contexts.

Most of the research supporting parent training programs has been in high-income countries. Of the limited studies in low-income countries, few have targeted reductions in harsh parenting or prevention of emotional and behavioral problems (Klein & Rye, Reference Klein and Rye2004; Jin et al. Reference Jin, Sun, Jiang, Ma, Morgan and Shen2007; Oveisi et al. Reference Oveisi, Ardabili, Dadds, Majdzadeh, Mohammadkhani, Rad and Shahrivar2010; Mejia et al. Reference Mejia, Calam and Sanders2012), while more have focused on caregivers of infants and children under age 3 and outcomes related to early mother–child interactions (Knerr et al. Reference Knerr, Gardner and Cluver2013). Further research is needed to establish the feasibility and efficacy of such interventions in low income, conflict-affected settings and to determine the best strategies for implementation and wide-scale dissemination.

Current study

We add to the growing evidence based on parenting interventions designed for low-resource settings by conducting a randomized trial of a 10-session program for caregivers of young children in post-conflict Liberia called ‘Parents Make the Difference’ (PMD). PMD was developed for caregivers of children aged 3–7 years. The program is grounded in behavioral theory and shares core concepts with parenting programs backed by the strongest evidence from high-income countries. The content was designed to be culturally relevant and appropriate for lay providers to deliver. We hypothesized that the program would be feasible with limited resources, reduce harsh discipline, increase positive parenting, and improve caregiver–child interactions. Secondary outcomes related to child cognition, emotional and behavioral well-being, and caregiver-child communication. Another program objective was to promote malaria prevention behaviors, particularly caregivers using bednets for their children, as malaria continues to be a leading cause of child morbidity and mortality (National Malaria Control Program et al. 2012).

Methods

Setting and participants

This study was conducted in Lofa County, Liberia. During the Second Liberian Civil War (1999–2003) that killed between 150,000 and 200,000 people, many residents of this county fled to neighboring Guinea and Sierra Leone. The International Rescue Committee (IRC), an international humanitarian organization, implemented the PMD program approximately 10 years after refugees returned home to rural Lofa County.

The IRC recruited caregivers from five communities in Lofa. Program staff conducted information sessions at schools and invited families to register. To be eligible, adults had to be a caregiver to a child aged 3–7 years entering the first year of formal schooling. If a caregiver had more than one eligible child, staff enumerated all eligible children in the household and randomly selected a ‘target’ child.

Intervention

PMD is a 10-session intervention. IRC staff developed the program by reviewing existing evidence-based programming and soliciting feedback from content experts on the research team; an earlier iteration of the program was influenced in particular by concepts included in the Nurturing Parenting Program (Bavolek et al. Reference Bavolek, Comstock and McLaughlin1983). The local IRC office then held focus groups in all five communities with male and female caregivers, as well as activity-based groups with children, to gather information about common parenting practices and roles, including those related to education and health. To inform components of the intervention on harsh parenting, additional conversations with caregivers and community leaders were held to gather information on specific discipline practices. These data informed both the intervention and assessment tools.

A pair of lay Liberian facilitators was trained to lead each weekly 2-hour session with groups of 20–35 caregivers. This group size is larger than that of many other parenting programs but allowed the IRC to serve more people with fewer resources; this is important for scalability in low-resource settings without external funding. This group size also had proven feasible and acceptable in previous piloting of similar interventions by IRC. Sessions were highly interactive with an emphasis on discussion, modeling, and in-session skills practice. The program focused on positive parenting and included briefer components on building cognitive and educational skills and malaria prevention behaviors (see Table 1). Facilitators visited the home of each caregiver once during the program to reinforce skills. The IRC provided caregivers with an incentive of approximately $1.50 USD per session as is customary of programs offered by non-governmental organizations in this setting. Caregivers were permitted to attend the program with a spouse or co-caregiver.

Table 1. Summary of the ‘Parents Make the Difference’ curriculum

Note. Each session lasted approximately 2 hours. Sessions were designed to be highly interactive with a strong emphasis on discussion, modeling, and in-session practice of skills.

Research design

This was a pragmatic, parallel-group, individually-randomized superiority trial. Eligible primary caregivers were randomized into a treatment or wait-list control group with a 1:1 allocation to treatment ratio, stratified by community. A participant flow diagram is displayed in Fig. 1. With power of 0.80, α of 0.05, equal allocation of participants to treatment and control conditions, and an assumption of minimal attrition, this study was designed to detect a standardized effect of at least 0.30.

Fig. 1. Participant flow diagram.

Procedures

A team of trained Liberian enumerators conducted a baseline survey of caregivers and target children in September 2013. After the survey, we held a public lottery in each community and randomly assigned eligible caregivers (without replacement) to immediate or delayed treatment. IRC staff delivered the 10-session intervention to caregivers in the immediate treatment group over the course of 13 weeks in early 2013. Approximately 1 month after the final session, we surveyed caregivers in both groups prior to providing the intervention to the waitlist control group. Enumerators administered all surveys at participants’ homes and captured responses with Android smart-phones. During the same home visit, enumerators also asked caregivers and their children to play together for 5 min to complete an observational measure of parent–child interactions. Enumerators were blinded to treatment assignment unless the caregiver divulged this information.

The research protocol was reviewed and approved by local community advisory boards in Liberia from all communities and the Duke University Institutional Review Board. This study was pre-registered at ClinicalTrials.gov (registration number omitted).

Measures

Prior to data collection, the local research team evaluated the study instruments through systematic piloting and cognitive interviewing. To do this, the local research team administered the items to caregivers and children in the community and asked participants to explain their responses to assess understandability and to advise on clearer ways to ask or translate the items. This led to changes in the core content of the questions, as well as to changes in the translations from American to Liberian English.

Primary outcomes

Parenting behavior

We asked caregivers to self-report on their use of six harsh discipline practices – such as whipping, slapping, and yelling at the child – in the 4 weeks prior to the survey on a scale of 0 (‘never’) to 4 (‘almost every day’). Items were adapted from the Discipline Module of the Multiple Indicator Cluster Survey (UNICEF, 2005). Responses were averaged to create a composite index. We also asked caregivers to rate their use of four positive behavior management strategies: ‘time out’, teaching rules, praising compliant behavior, and asking the child to stop bad behavior. The ‘praise’ item was rated on a different 0–3 scale, so all positive behavior management items were standardized before summing to create a composite that was then standardized. Caregivers also provided an open-ended description of their response the last time the child misbehaved. Enumerators coded responses to match 15 possible behaviors; non-matching responses were coded as ‘other’.

Caregiver–child interactions

We asked caregivers and children to rate their experience of four behaviors: spending time together, playing together, talking together, and praise. Caregivers rated their tendency to do these behaviors by indicating their position on a 10-step ladder where the 10th step represents maximum engagement. Children were asked whether the caregiver did these things in the 7 days prior to the survey. To reduce difficulty, enumerators first asked about the behavior using simplified wording and phrasing in the form of a ‘yes/no’ question. If the child responded ‘yes’, the enumerator then asked how much: ‘a small amount’ (1), ‘a medium amount’ (2), or ‘a lot’ (3). ‘No’ responses were scored as 0. We created composite scores for caregivers and children by averaging responses to each set of items.

Secondary outcomes

Communication

Audio recordings from the 5 min caregiver-child play observation were transcribed by Liberians and coded by trained assistants (American) using the Dyadic Parent–Child Interaction Coding System (DPICS-III; Eyberg et al. Reference Eyberg, Nelson, Duke and Boggs2005). DPICS allows for the classification of parent verbalizations that serve as markers of parent-child relationship quality. Specific categories include: neutral talk, praise, reflection, behavior description, questions, commands, and negative talk. Assistants were trained according to the DPICS clinical coding manual (Chase & Eyberg, Reference Chase and Eyberg2005; Fernandez et al. Reference Fernandez, Chase Rhea and Eyberg2005) and reached 80% agreement with the trainer before coding independently; 10% of all transcripts were randomly selected to be re-coded by a second rater.

Child cognitive abilities

We developed a short battery of cognitive tasks similar to those on standardized developmental and cognitive measures. Domains included receptive language, expressive language, verbal comprehension, verbal fluency, and numeracy. Scores on language domains were standardized and summed to create an overall language ability composite that was then standardized.

Child wellbeing

The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) is a 25-item behavioral screening questionnaire about children 3–17 years old (Goodman, Reference Goodman1997). We administered three subscales of the SDQ to caregivers: hyperactivity/inattention, emotional symptoms, and conduct problems.

Malaria prevention behaviors

We also asked caregivers whether the household owned a mosquito net, whether the target child slept under the net the night before the survey, and other patterns of use.

Empirical strategy

We calculated intention-to-treat (ITT) estimates of the average treatment effect, θ ITT, using the following ordinary least squares specification:

$${Y_i} = \alpha + {\theta _{{\rm ITT}}}{T_i} + \beta {X'_i} + {\rm} {\varepsilon _i}$$

where Y i is an outcome of interest for person i measured 1 month after the treatment group completed the program; T i is an indicator for assignment to treatment (1 = treatment; 0 = wait-list control); X i is a vector of community strata dummies and baseline covariates. In the main specification, all missing data were imputed with median values.

In addition to looking at the average treatment effects, we also examined treatment heterogeneity via subgroup analyses (Longford, Reference Longford1999) and quantile regression (Koenker, Reference Koenker2005). Subgroup (moderator) analyses allow us to ask whether the treatment was more or less effective for some participants. We estimated treatment heterogeneity by interacting assignment to treatment with each baseline covariate we are testing as a potential moderator. Quantile regression allows us to examine the variation in impact across the distribution of outcomes. We did not pre-register heterogeneity analyses, so these results should be viewed as exploratory; however, our selection of potential moderators was guided by similar work (Gardner et al. Reference Gardner, Hutchings, Bywater and Whitaker2010).

To explore the mechanisms through which the intervention might impact more distal outcomes (e.g. child emotional symptoms), we considered two possible post-baseline mediator (or intervening) variables: caregiver-reported use of harsh discipline and caregiver–child positive interactions. We tested a multiple mediator model that includes both potential mediators simultaneously and compute bootstrapped 95% confidence intervals (CI) (adjusted bootstrap percentile method) of the total indirect effect and the component indirect effects of each proposed mediator (Preacher & Hayes, Reference Preacher and Hayes2008).

We conducted all analyses in R version 3.0.2 (R Core Team, 2013) except for the quantile regressions which we conducted in Stata MP version 12.0 (StataCorp, 2011). Our paper was compiled directly from the raw data and analysis code using RStudio version 0.98.932 (RStudio, 2013) and LATEX MacTeX-2013 distribution (LaTeX3 Project, 2013).

Results

Participant characteristics

Children (n = 269) and their caregivers (n = 270) completed a baseline survey. Figure 1 displays the participant flow diagram. 53% of the child sample was female, and the average child was 5.2 years old. The caregiver sample was also almost evenly divided between males and females (57%), and the average caregiver was 35.5 years old. Table 2 demonstrates that the random assignment to treatment and wait-list control resulted in pre-treatment balance.

Table 2. Participant baseline characteristics

SDQ, strengths and difficulties questionnaire

Note. aAn exchange rate of 74.2 Liberian Dollars per $1USD (12 September 2012) was used to convert to USD. Self-reported income top-coded at the 99th percentile.

Treatment compliance

Caregiver attendance was high; at least one caregiver from 98% of households attended all 10 sessions. In some cases, a co-caregiver who did not participate in the surveys attended sessions with, or in place of, the caregiver in the study. These patterns of attendance were not tracked, nor were completed home visits.

Attrition

The overall attrition rate was 6.7%; 18 children and their caregivers did not participate in the endline survey. As shown in Table A.1 in the Online Appendix, there are no statistically significant baseline differences between found and unfound participants.

Treatment effects

Program impacts on primary and secondary outcomes are listed in Table 3 and displayed graphically as standardized effect sizes (Glass's Delta) in Fig. 2. Full results are presented in Tables A.2–A.7 in Online Appendix A.

Fig. 2. This plot displays standardized results of intention-to-treat (ITT) ordinary least squares (OLS) regressions of each primary and secondary outcome on an indicator of assignment to treatment, stratum fixed effects, and baseline covariates. The point estimates were standardized by dividing the coefficient on assignment by the control group standard deviation (Glass's Δ). Black dots represent point estimates in the hypothesized direction (grey if not in the hypothesized direction). Dotted lines represent 95% confidence intervals (CI).

Table 3. Average treatment effects: primary and secondary outcomes

SDQ, strengths and difficulties questionnaire

˙p < 0.1, *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Note. This table reports average treatment effects (ATE) that are based on a comparison of caregivers assigned to the treatment and control groups. Column 1 lists the scale of each outcome. The character in parentheses indicates the valence of higher values: good (+) or bad (−). Columns 2 and 3 report unadjusted means and standard deviations (s.d.) among the control group. Columns 4 and 5 report the results from an ordinary least squares regression of each outcome on an indicator of assignment to treatment, community fixed effects (omitted), and a vector of baseline covariates (omitted). Column 6 reports the 95% confidence interval (CI) on the estimate reported in column 4. Column 7 reports Glass's Δ, a standardized effect size (ATE/control group s.d.).

Primary outcomes

Parenting behavior

The program led to a 55.5% reduction (−0.49 points on a scale of 0–4) in caregiver-reported use of harsh punishment practices. As shown in Table A.2 (Online Appendix), a greater proportion of caregivers in the treatment group compared with the wait-list control group reported abandoning harsh practices, such as whipping, slapping, beating, and shouting at their children. The standardized treatment effects (Glass's Delta) on harsh parenting practices range from −0.26 to −0.67. We also observe a corresponding significant increase in the use of positive behavior management strategies. This small effect appears to be driven by a significant increase in reported use of time out.

We see the same pattern of results in the open coding of caregivers’ descriptions of how they responded to their child's most recent episode of bad behavior (see Table A.3 in the Online Appendix). On average, a smaller proportion of caregivers in the treatment group described using harsh practices such as yelling and beating, and a larger proportion reported using time out.

Caregiver–child interactions

In addition to decreasing caregivers’ use of harsh punishment and increasing the use of positive behavior management strategies, the program also significantly increased positive interactions between caregivers and children. The average caregiver in the treatment group reported a 4.4% increase in positive interactions (0.34 points on a scale of 1–10), while the average child of a caregiver assigned to the treatment group reported a 17.5% increase (0.32 points on a scale of 0–3; standardized effect sizes of 0.22 and 0.38, respectively).

Secondary outcomes

The average caregiver assigned to the treatment group increased praise and decreased negative talk as a percentage of overall number of caregiver verbalizations during the timed observational play session, but neither effect is statistically significant at conventional levels. Counter to what we hypothesized, we observe a slight decrease in verbalizations among children of caregivers assigned to the treatment group, but this effect is also not statistically significant. We detect a small treatment effect at the 0.10 level on children's receptive vocabulary and numeracy (standardized effect sizes of 0.1 and 0.18, respectively). With the exception of caregiver-reported child conduct problems, child cognitive, and emotional wellbeing indicators move in the hypothesized direction; however, these effects are mostly small and non-significant. The program had no measurable impact on the uptake of malaria prevention behaviors.

Sensitivity

Bias could potentially result from unobserved baseline imbalance and systematic attrition, so we tested the sensitivity of impacts on primary outcomes. As shown in Table A.8 of the Online Appendix, findings are robust to an alternate specification that excludes baseline covariates. We also created an extreme bound on treatment effects that assumes the ‘worst’ case for missing treatment observations and the ‘best’ case for missing control observations (see table notes for more details). The impact on harsh parenting holds under this extreme assumption, but the impacts on caregiver–child positive interactions and positive behavior management shrink and become non-significant.

Treatment heterogeneity

We examine treatment heterogeneity in three outcomes – harsh parenting, positive behavior management, and positive interaction – according to four characteristics measured at baseline: caregiver gender, child gender, child age, and child conduct problems. Results are presented in Table 4.

Table 4. Treatment heterogeneity

˙p < 0.1, *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Note. This table displays the results of a moderation (subgroup) analysis. We examine treatment heterogeneity in three outcomes – harsh parenting, positive behavior management, and positive interaction – according to four characteristics measured at baseline: (i) caregiver gender, (ii) child gender, (iii) child age, and (iv) child conduct problems. We estimate treatment heterogeneity by interacting assignment to treatment with each baseline covariate.

With respect to harsh parenting, female caregivers report significantly greater use of harsh punishments, but benefitted twice as much from the intervention compared with male caregivers. Caregivers of boys also reported significantly greater use of harsh punishment, but child gender was not a significant moderator of treatment impact. Child age, however, was a moderator, with caregivers of younger children benefitting more from the intervention. There is also evidence that caregivers of children with more conduct problems at baseline responded more positively. With respect to positive behavior management and children's reports of positive interactions, there was only possible moderation (p < 0.10) of caregiver gender on effects on positive behavior management with males showing larger improvements.

Quantile regression results are displayed in Fig. A.1 in the Online Appendix. As shown in Panel B, it appears that the intervention was most effective among caregivers who reported the most frequent use of harsh punishment (−1.31 s.d.)

Mediation analysis

The intervention's theory of change suggests that teaching caregivers the importance of positive caregiver–child interaction, the ineffectiveness and potential negative impact of harsh discipline strategies, and the utility of positive behavior management approaches – all in the context of modeling and practice – will lead to measureable change on these intermediate caregiver/relationship outcomes, which in turn will improve child outcomes. As reported above, we observe statistically significant impacts on the intermediate outcomes, but small and generally non-significant results for child outcomes such as conduct problems, language ability, and emotional symptoms. While the program impact was non-significant, we can use mediation analysis to explore the mechanism through which the program may impact child outcomes (O'Rourke & MacKinnon, Reference O'Rourke and MacKinnon2015).

As shown in Fig. A.2 in the Online Appendix, child emotional problems appear to be partially mediated by harsh discipline and positive caregiver–child interactions. The total indirect effect is −0.13 s.d. and the 95% CI excludes 0. Both mediators appear to contribute roughly the same amount to the indirect effect. There is also a significant indirect effect of these two mediators on child language. The total indirect effect is 0.13 s.d. We also tested for moderated mediation since we found that caregiver gender is a potential moderator, but we found no evidence for this.

Discussion

This randomized trial examined the feasibility and impact of a parenting skills intervention for caregivers of young children in Liberia. Findings add to the scarce but growing literature on the impacts of parenting interventions in low-income countries and post-conflict settings. This study focuses on children ages 3–7, an age range well-represented in parenting literature in high-income countries but less so in other parts of the world.

Results show that the PMD reduced caregivers’ use of harsh physical and verbal punishment strategies and increased their use of positive behavior management at 1-month follow-up. The program also improved caregiver–child interactions – according to caregiver and child report – but no significant effects were found on child-level outcomes or malaria prevention behaviors.

The effects of PMD on harsh punishment were the most robust findings, with medium-sized effects on common discipline strategies, including beating, whipping, and shouting. This is comparable with effects of similar programs in the USA. In their review of parenting interventions in low- and middle-income countries, Knerr et al. (Reference Knerr, Gardner and Cluver2013) identified three studies specifically targeting reductions in harsh parenting behaviors. As examples, results were consistent with those of Oveisi et al. (Reference Oveisi, Ardabili, Dadds, Majdzadeh, Mohammadkhani, Rad and Shahrivar2010) who documented moderate effects on harsh discipline following a physician-led parenting intervention in Iran and larger than the small long-term effect on harsh discipline among Turkish mothers after an early enrichment program Kagitcibasi et al. (Reference Kagitcibasi, Sunar and Bekman2001). Thus, our results suggest that a parenting intervention delivered by lay facilitators in a post-conflict setting can be as effective for reducing harsh discipline as many similar programs in other contexts.

PMD also had significant small effects on positive behavior management strategies, such as praising positive behavior and using time out. These were smaller than effects of parenting interventions in high-income countries, though studies reporting the largest effects evaluated interventions for children with documented disruptive behavior problems (Thomas & Zimmer-Gembeck, Reference Thomas and Zimmer-Gembeck2007). Thus, the small impact of PMD on positive behavior management could reflect the fact that we studied the impact of a smaller intervention dose on a non-clinical sample. To bolster effects, interventions like PMD may need to include more intense skills modeling and practice, focus on fewer skills, and take into account more of the potential environmental or cultural factors that may point to additional helpful adaptations.

For positive parent–child interactions, such as playing, talking, and time together, PMD led to modest improvements. This is consistent with the study by Kagitcibasi et al. (Reference Kagitcibasi, Sunar and Bekman2001) in Turkey documenting small but significant effects of a parenting intervention on mother involvement and parent-child communication. Several studies in low-income countries also have documented benefits of parenting interventions on positive caregiver–infant interactions (Aboud et al. Reference Aboud, Shafique and Akhter2009; Knerr et al. Reference Knerr, Gardner and Cluver2013).

Despite having a positive impact on these proximal caregiver-level outcomes, the PMD program did not lead to significant improvements on the secondary outcomes of children's cognitive and communication skills or emotional or behavioral well-being. This is not entirely surprising given the non-clinical sample, as higher levels of distress and emotional dysregulation among oppositional children at baseline predict greater improvements in child outcomes after parent training (Scott & O'Connor, Reference Scott and O'Connor2012). These studies have led to the ‘differential susceptibility hypothesis’, which posits that children with higher levels of irritability and emotional lability are more impacted by changes in parenting (Bakermans-Kranenburg et al. Reference Bakermans-Kranenburg, Van IJzendoorn, Pijlman, Mes-man and Juffer2008; Scott & O'Connor, Reference Scott and O'Connor2012). Thus, while the theory underlying parenting interventions is that changes in parenting will positively impact children's development, in non-clinical samples, parenting changes may not have immediate effects; child outcomes may emerge over time – especially the prevention of negative mental health outcomes (Hermanns et al. Reference Hermanns, Asscher, Zijlstra, Hoffenaar and Dekovic2013). If child-level effects are to emerge over time, the mechanism of change might be through decreased harsh parenting and increased positive caregiver–child interactions. The results of our mediation analysis suggest that both might mediate child emotional problems. More positive interactions may also mediate the development of children's cognitive abilities, such as language.

Finally, a foundational goal of this study was to determine the feasibility of implementing a parenting skills intervention in a post-conflict, low-resource setting. Attendance and monitoring data supported both the feasibility and acceptability across the communities. This is encouraging because the intervention was implemented in community settings by lay facilitators – a delivery model that is replicable across diverse settings, including those affected by conflict and scarcity of health professionals.

Limitations

Limitations are related to measurement challenges, length of follow-up, study design, and implementation. First, the broad age range in child age (3–7 years) meant that we could not rely on standardized cognitive measures, and the study was not powered to examine outcomes by age group. This study was also among the first to use an observational measure of parent–child interactions in this context. American research assistants coded the data, and their unfamiliarity with Liberian English vernacular may have contributed to the limited results in this domain. Second, endline measurement 1-month post-intervention may not have allowed sufficient time for potential positive child outcomes to emerge and did not allow for examination of the maintenance of change over time. Third, related to design, it is possible that there was contamination between the groups, which could have attenuated the observed treatment effects. Also, because we used a waitlist control condition, we do not have a time and attention control condition to conclude that effects were due to this specific intervention. Lastly, attendance tracking did not specify whether another family member replaced the enrolled participant at any given session, meaning that attendance rates are at the household level.

Conclusions and future directions

Results support the feasibility of implementing PMD in a low-income post-conflict setting to reduce harsh treatment of children. PMD was implemented in community-based settings with lay facilitators, a model that could be replicated by aid organizations, governments, or community-level mechanisms.

Future implementations of PMD or similar programs should evaluate modifications that may further increase impact. Live practice with children has emerged as one of the most effective treatment components for children with disruptive behavior disorders (Kaminski et al. Reference Kaminski, Valle, Filene and Boyle2008) and could also enhance outcomes in non-clinical populations. This could be integrated into PMD home visits. However, as home visits are a relatively expensive component, their contribution to the intervention effects and cost-effectiveness would need to be explored.

Given the null results on malaria prevention and cognitive outcomes, it is clear that more work is needed to determine whether and how to integrate parenting, health, and early education interventions. These are key outcomes for young children, and all are caregiver-driven. The challenge is to develop interventions that have enough depth, while also maintaining a reasonable scope and length.

A priority for future study is to evaluate the long-term effects of prevention-focused parenting interventions to determine whether they influence the future development of emotional, behavioral, or cognitive problems throughout childhood and adolescence. Future studies also should examine impacts of PMD with caregivers of children with clinical levels of behavioral or emotional difficulties. These could compare effects of the intervention for clinical v. non-clinical samples or compare variants of the program for children and caregivers with varying levels of concerns.

Supplementary material

For supplementary material accompanying this paper visit http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/gmh.2015.12.

Acknowledgement

For their input into project design and implementation, the authors would like to acknowledge Paul Amendola, Julia Frazier, Geoffrey Kirenga, Ruth Z.B. Korgba-Siafa, Rufus J. Kuku, Justine Landegger, Sandra Maignant, and Sarah Smith.

References

Aboud, FE, Shafique, S, Akhter, S (2009). A responsive feeding intervention increases children's self-feeding and maternal responsiveness but not weight gain. The Journal of Nutrition 139, 17381743.Google Scholar
Akinsulure-Smith, AM, Smith, HE (2012). Evolution of family policies in post-conflict Sierra Leone. Journal of Child and Family Studies 21, 413.Google Scholar
Akmatov, MK (2011). Child abuse in 28 developing and transitional countries – results from the multiple indicator cluster surveys. International Journal of Epidemiology 40, 219227.Google Scholar
Bakermans-Kranenburg, MJ, Van IJzendoorn, MH, Pijlman, FT, Mes-man, J, Juffer, F (2008). Experimental evidence for differential susceptibility: dopamine d4 receptor polymorphism (DRD4 VNTR) moderates intervention effects on toddlers’ externalizing behavior in a randomized controlled trial. Developmental Psychology 44, 293.Google Scholar
Bavolek, SJ, Comstock, CM, McLaughlin, JA (1983). The Nurturing Program: a Validated Approach to Reducing Dysfunctional Family Interactions. Final Report, Project (1R01MH34862).Google Scholar
Betancourt, TS, Khan, KT (2008). The mental health of children affected by armed conflict: protective processes and pathways to resilience. International Review of Psychiatry 20, 317328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bolton, P, Betancourt, TS (2004). Mental health in postwar Afghanistan. JAMA 292, 626628.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boudreault-Bouchard, A-M, Dion, J, Hains, J, Vandermeerschen, J, Laberge, L, Perron, M (2013). Impact of parental emotional support and coercive control on adolescents’ self-esteem and psychological distress: results of a four-year longitudinal study. Journal of Adolescence 36, 695704.Google Scholar
Catani, C, Schauer, E, Neuner, F (2008). Beyond individual war trauma: domestic violence against children in Afghanistan and Sri Lanka. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy 34, 165176.Google Scholar
Chase, R, Eyberg, S (2005). Abridged Manual for the Dyadic Parent-Child Interaction Coding System, 3rd edn. Tech. Report. PCIT International: Gainesville, FL.Google Scholar
Chase, RM, Eyberg, SM (2008). Clinical presentation and treatment outcome for children with comorbid externalizing and internalizing symptoms. Journal of Anxiety Disorders 22, 273282.Google Scholar
Eisenstadt, TH, Eyberg, S, McNeil, CB, Newcomb, K, Funderburk, B (1993). Parent-child interaction therapy with behavior problem children: relative effectiveness of two stages and overall treatment outcome. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology 22, 4251.Google Scholar
Engle, PL, Black, MM, Behrman, JR, Cabral de Mello, M, Gertler, PJ, Kapiriri, L, Martorell, R, Young, ME (2007). Strategies to avoid the loss of developmental potential in more than 200 million children in the developing world. The Lancet 369, 229242.Google Scholar
Eyberg, S, Nelson, M, Duke, M, Boggs, S (2005). Manual for the Dyadic Parent-Child Interaction Coding System, (DPICS-III), 3rd edn. Tech. Report. PCIT International: Gainesville, FL.Google Scholar
Eyberg, SM, Nelson, MM, Boggs, SR (2008). Evidence-based psychosocial treatments for children and adolescents with disruptive behavior. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology 37, 215237.Google Scholar
Fernandez, M, Chase Rhea, M, Eyberg, S (2005). The Workbook: Coder Training Manual for the Dyadic Parent-Child Interaction Coding System, 3rd edn. Tech. Report. PCIT International: Gainesville, FL. Google Scholar
Gardner, F, Hutchings, J, Bywater, T, Whitaker, C (2010). Who benefits and how does it work? Moderators and mediators of outcome in an effectiveness trial of a parenting intervention. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology 39, 568580.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodman, R (1997). The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: a research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 38, 581586.Google Scholar
Hermanns, J, Asscher, JJ, Zijlstra, BJ, Hoffenaar, PJ, Dekovic, M (2013). Long-term changes in parenting and child behavior after the home-start family support program. Children and Youth Services Review 35, 678684.Google Scholar
Jin, X, Sun, Y, Jiang, F, Ma, J, Morgan, C, Shen, X (2007). ‘Care for development’ intervention in rural China: a prospective follow-up study. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics 28, 213218.Google Scholar
Kagitcibasi, C, Sunar, D, Bekman, S (2001). Long-term effects of early intervention: Turkish low-income mothers and children. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 22, 333361.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kakuma, R, Minas, H, van Ginneken, N, Dal Poz, MR, Desiraju, K, Morris, JE, Saxena, S, Scheffier, RM (2011). Human resources for mental health care: current situation and strategies for action. The Lancet 378, 16541663.Google Scholar
Kaminski, JW, Valle, LA, Filene, JH, Boyle, CL (2008). A meta-analytic review of components associated with parent training program effectiveness. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 36, 567589.Google Scholar
Kieling, C, Baker-Henningham, H, Belfer, M, Conti, G, Ertem, I, Omigbo-dun, O, Rohde, LA, Srinath, S, Ulkuer, N, Rahman, A (2011). Child and adolescent mental health worldwide: evidence for action. The Lancet 378, 15151525.Google Scholar
Klein, PS, Rye, H (2004). Interaction-oriented early intervention in Ethiopia: the MISC approach. Infants & Young Children 17, 340354.Google Scholar
Knerr, W, Gardner, F, Cluver, L (2013). Improving positive parenting skills and reducing harsh and abusive parenting in low-and middle-income countries: a systematic review. Prevention Science 14, 352363.Google Scholar
Koenker, R (2005). Quantile Regression. No. 38. Cambridge University Press. LaTeX3 Project, 2013. Latex: A Document Preparation System.Google Scholar
Longford, NT (1999). Selection bias and treatment heterogeneity in clinical trials. Statistics in Medicine 18, 14671474.3.0.CO;2-H>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maher, EJ, Marcynyszyn, LA, Corwin, TW, Hodnett, R (2011). Dosage matters: the relationship between participation in the nurturing parenting program for infants, toddlers, and preschoolers and subsequent child maltreatment. Children and Youth Services Review 33, 14261434.Google Scholar
Mejia, A, Calam, R, Sanders, MR (2012). A review of parenting programs in developing countries: opportunities and challenges for preventing emotional and behavioral difficulties in children. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review 15, 163175.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mels, C, Derluyn, I, Broekaert, E, Rosseel, Y (2010). The psychological impact of forced displacement and related risk factors on eastern congolese adolescents affected by war. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 51, 10961104.Google Scholar
Miller, KE, Rasmussen, A (2010). War exposure, daily stressors, and mental health in conflict and post-conflict settings: bridging the divide between trauma-focused and psychosocial frameworks. Social Science & Medicine 70, 716.Google Scholar
Mills, R, Alati, R, O'Callaghan, M, Najman, JM, Williams, GM, Bor, W, Strathearn, L (2011). Child abuse and neglect and cognitive function at 14 years of age: findings from a birth cohort. Pediatrics 127, 410.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moylan, CA, Herrenkohl, TI, Sousa, C, Tajima, EA, Herrenkohl, RC, Russo, MJ (2010). The effects of child abuse and exposure to domestic violence on adolescent internalizing and externalizing behavior problems. Journal of Family Violence 25, 5363.Google Scholar
National Malaria Control Program, Ministry of Health and Social Welfare, Liberia Institute of Statistics and Geo-Information Services, ICF International (2012). Liberia Malaria Indicator Survey 2011, Tech. Report, NMCP, LIS-GIS, and ICF International: Monrovia, Liberia.Google Scholar
Nowak, C, Heinrichs, N (2008). A comprehensive meta-analysis of triple p-positive parenting program using hierarchical linear modeling: effectiveness and moderating variables. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review 11, 114144.Google Scholar
O'Rourke, H, MacKinnon, D (2015). When the test of mediation is more powerful than the test of the total effect. Behavioral Research Methods 47, 424442.Google Scholar
Oveisi, S, Ardabili, HE, Dadds, MR, Majdzadeh, R, Mohammadkhani, P, Rad, JA, Shahrivar, Z (2010). Primary prevention of parent-child conflict and abuse in Iranian mothers: a randomized-controlled trial. Child Abuse & Neglect 34, 206213.Google Scholar
Panter-Brick, C, Goodman, A, Tol, W, Eggerman, M (2011). Mental health and childhood adversities: a longitudinal study in Kabul, Afghanistan. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 50, 349363.Google Scholar
Preacher, KJ, Hayes, AF (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods 40, 879891.Google Scholar
R Core Team (2013). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria.Google Scholar
Reed, RV, Fazel, M, Jones, L, Panter-Brick, C, Stein, A (2012). Mental health of displaced and refugee children resettled in low-income and middle-income countries: risk and protective factors. The Lancet 379, 250265.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
RStudio (2013). Rstudio: Integrated development environment for R. Boston, MA. Version 3.2.1.Google Scholar
Scott, S, O'Connor, TG (2012). An experimental test of differential susceptibility to parenting among emotionally-dysregulated children in a randomized controlled trial for oppositional behavior. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 53, 11841193.Google Scholar
StataCorp (2011). Stata Statistical Software: Release 12. StataCorp LP: College Station, TX.Google Scholar
Stoltenborgh, M, van IJzendoorn, MH, Euser, EM, Bakermans-Kranenburg, MJ (2011). A global perspective on child sexual abuse: meta-analysis of prevalence around the world. Child Maltreatment 16, 79101.Google Scholar
Thomas, R, Zimmer-Gembeck, MJ (2007). Behavioral outcomes of parent-child interaction therapy and triple P – positive parenting program: a review and meta-analysis. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 35, 475495.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tol, WA, Barbui, C, Galappatti, A, Silove, D, Betancourt, TS, Souza, R, Golaz, A, Van Ommeren, M (2011). mental health and psychosocial support in humanitarian settings: linking practice and research. The Lancet 378, 15811591.Google Scholar
Uji, M, Sakamoto, A, Adachi, K, Kitamura, T (2014). The impact of authoritative, authoritarian, and permissive parenting styles on children's later mental health in Japan: focusing on parent and child gender. Journal of Child and Family Studies 23, 293302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
UNICEF (2005). Multiple Indicator Cluster Survey, Tech. Report, UNICEF. New York, NY.Google Scholar
Webster-Stratton, C, Rinaldi, J, Reid, JM (2011). Long-term outcomes of incredible years parenting program: predictors of adolescent adjustment*. Child and Adolescent Mental Health 16, 3846.Google Scholar
Widom, CS, Czaja, SJ, Bentley, T, Johnson, MS (2012). A prospective investigation of physical health outcomes in abused and neglected children: new findings from a 30-year follow-up. American Journal of Public Health 102, 11351144.Google Scholar
Yap, MBH, Pilkington, PD, Ryan, SM, Jorm, AF (2013). Parental factors associated with depression and anxiety in young people: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders 156, 823.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Summary of the ‘Parents Make the Difference’ curriculum

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Participant flow diagram.

Figure 2

Table 2. Participant baseline characteristics

Figure 3

Fig. 2. This plot displays standardized results of intention-to-treat (ITT) ordinary least squares (OLS) regressions of each primary and secondary outcome on an indicator of assignment to treatment, stratum fixed effects, and baseline covariates. The point estimates were standardized by dividing the coefficient on assignment by the control group standard deviation (Glass's Δ). Black dots represent point estimates in the hypothesized direction (grey if not in the hypothesized direction). Dotted lines represent 95% confidence intervals (CI).

Figure 4

Table 3. Average treatment effects: primary and secondary outcomes

Figure 5

Table 4. Treatment heterogeneity

Supplementary material: PDF

Puffer supplementary material S1

Appendix and figures

Download Puffer supplementary material S1(PDF)
PDF 468.8 KB