Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-fscjk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-28T05:23:16.821Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Attention improvement to transcranial alternating current stimulation at gamma frequency over the right frontoparietal network: a preliminary report

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 October 2024

Tien-Wen Lee
Affiliation:
The NeuroCognitive Institute (NCI) Clinical Research Foundation, Mount Arlington, NJ, USA
Sergio Almeida
Affiliation:
The NeuroCognitive Institute (NCI) Clinical Research Foundation, Mount Arlington, NJ, USA
Gerald Tramontano*
Affiliation:
The NeuroCognitive Institute (NCI) Clinical Research Foundation, Mount Arlington, NJ, USA
*
Corresponding author: Gerald Tramontano; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Applying transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) at 40 Hz to the frontal and parietal regions, either unilaterally (left or right) or bilaterally, can improve cognitive dysfunctions. This study aimed to explore the influence of tACS at gamma frequency over right fronto-parietal (FP) region on attention. The analysis is based on retrospective data from a clinical intervention. We administered test of variables of attention (TOVA; visual mode) to 44 participants with various neuropsychiatric diagnoses before and after 12 sessions of tACS treatment. Alternating currents at 2.0 mA were delivered to the electrode positions F4 and P4, following the 10–20 EEG convention, for 20 mins in each session. We observed significant improvement across 3 indices of the TOVA, including reduction of variability in reaction time (p = 0.0002), increase in d-Prime (separability of targets and non-targets; p = 0.0157), and decrease in commission error rate (p = 0.0116). The mean RT and omission error rate largely remained unchanged. Artificial injection of tACS at 40 Hz over right FP network may improve attention function, especially in the domains of consistency in performance, target/non-target discrimination, and inhibitory control.

Type
Short Communication
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Scandinavian College of Neuropsychopharmacology

Significant outcomes

  • Transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) at 40 Hz applied to F4 and P4 EEG lead positions may enhance attention in diverse neuropsychiatric conditions.

  • The tACS protocol comprised 12 sessions, each lasting 20 minutes, with a current of 2 mA.

  • Significant improvements were observed in attention consistency, target/non-target discrimination, and inhibitory control.

Limitations

  • The results of this preliminary report may be influenced by practice effects.

  • Double-blind, sham-controlled studies with well-defined participant selection criteria are needed to validate the reported efficacy.

Introduction

Cortical oscillations at various frequencies have been linked to diverse cognitive functions. For instance, working memory and semantic memory functions have been, respectively, associated with power changes in the theta and upper alpha bands (Klimesch, Reference Klimesch1999; Brzezicka et al., Reference Brzezicka, Kaminski, Reed, Chung, Mamelak and Rutishauser2019). Gamma rhythms have been associated with the functioning of extensive brain networks and cognitive processes such as attention, perceptual grouping, memory, and working memory (Fell et al., Reference Fell, Klaver, Elger and Fernandez2002; Herrmann et al., Reference Herrmann, Fründ and Lenz2010; Brookes et al., Reference Brookes, Wood, Stevenson, Zumer, White, Liddle and Morris2011; Lundqvist et al., Reference Lundqvist, Herman, Warden, Brincat and Miller2018). With the introduction of transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS), it becomes possible to inject artificial oscillation into the brain and study its causal influence on neural rhythmicity and cognition.

Correspondent with neuroanatomical understanding, tACS at theta over anterior frontal and medial temporal and tACS at gamma over occipitoparietal regions lead to improvement in working memory task performance (Park et al., Reference Park, Lee, Lee and Im2022; Wischnewski et al., Reference Wischnewski, Berger, Opitz and Alekseichuk2024; Manippa et al., Reference Manippa, Filardi, Vilella, Logroscino and Rivolta2024a). The tACS at theta/gamma ranges with anterior/posterior montages may improve episodic memory (Varastegan et al., Reference Varastegan, Kazemi, Rostami, Khomami, Zandbagleh and Hadipour2023), while the theta-gamma cross-frequency tACS targeting the prefrontal cortex may enhance visuomotor learning (Diedrich et al., Reference Diedrich, Kolhoff, Chakalov, Vékony, Németh and Antal2024). The tACS at gamma frequency over right parietal and left temporo-parietal regions may, respectively, modulate endogenous attention and auditory spatial attention (Hopfinger et al., Reference Hopfinger, Parsons and Frohlich2017; Wostmann et al., Reference Wostmann, Vosskuhl, Obleser and Herrmann2018). The promising potential of gamma tACS in addressing mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and early stages of Alzheimer’s disease (AD) has been suggested (Manippa et al., Reference Manippa, Palmisano, Nitsche, Filardi, Vilella, Logroscino and Rivolta2024b). Positive evidence regarding the application of tACS to other neuropsychiatric conditions, such as dyslexia, anxiety, depression, and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), is beginning to accumulate (Dallmer-Zerbe et al., Reference Dallmer-Zerbe, Popp, Lam, Philipsen and Herrmann2020; Lee et al., Reference Lee, Li and Tramontano2024).

The clinical effects of tACS appear to be frequency- and region-specific as briefly summarised above. It is noteworthy, however, that sometimes tACS may be detrimental to neuropsychological performance. For example, prefrontal gamma modulation may impair working memory (Wischnewski et al., Reference Wischnewski, Berger, Opitz and Alekseichuk2024), and beta tACS may reduce motor cortex excitability (Zaghi et al., Reference Zaghi, De Freitas Rezende, De Oliveira, El-Nazer, Menning, Tadini and Fregni2010). Our recent report suggested that tACS may desynchronise underlying neural oscillation (Lee and Tramontano, Reference Lee and Tramontano2024). It is thus imperative to empirically examine if a specific tACS protocol is beneficial. It is acknowledged that attention function engages broad neural substrates, relatively right-lateralised for visuospatial processing, particularly fronto-parietal (FP) network (Shipp, Reference Shipp2004, Thiebaut de Schotten et al., Reference De Schotten, Dell’acqua, Forkel, Simmons, Vergani, Murphy and Catani2011). To the best of our knowledge, the influence of gamma tACS over right FP regions on the attention function has never been specifically examined. The importance of investigating transcranial electrical stimulation (tES) on attention cannot be overstated, as attention is essential for the effective execution of other cognitive processes. If tACS does indeed influence attention, it would impact the neuropsychological interpretation of pertinent research.

Among the frequency bands of brain waves, we are particularly interested in gamma, given its coupling with downward spectra (delta to beta), co-bursts with beta in volitional control, close relevance to attention, and its roles in cognitive impairment in various neurocognitive conditions (Fell et al., Reference Fell, Klaver, Elger and Fernandez2002; Fan et al., Reference Fan, Byrne, Worden, Guise, Mccandliss, Fossella and Posner2007; Foster and Parvizi, Reference Foster and Parvizi2012; Goodman et al., Reference Goodman, Kumar, Zomorrodi, Ghazala, Cheam, Barr, Daskalakis, Blumberger, Fischer and Flint2018). This study was designed to trace the changes in attention function via the test of variables of attention (TOVA) following 12 sessions of tACS treatment (Leark et al., Reference Leark, Greenberg, Kindschi, Dupuy and Hughes2008).

Materials and methods

Participants

The research protocol centred on patients exhibiting attention and other cognitive deficits who underwent 40 Hz tACS treatment applied to the right frontal and parietal regions. Participants were recruited through our clinics and included patients who were referred for neuropsychiatric evaluation and treatment. A consent form was acquired for each patient before the commencement of treatment. We conducted a review of data collected from our clinics between 2018 and 2022, with prior approval from a private Review Board (Pearl IRB; https://www.pearlirb.com/). To be enlisted in this study, patients need to have a diagnosis characterised by cognitive deficits (e.g. ADHD and MCI), or have a chief complaint of cognitive decline. Forty-four cognitively impaired patients were identified who underwent full tACS treatment as well as pre- and post-treatment TOVA assessments. Individuals with epilepsy, skull defects, intracranial electrodes, brain lesions, vascular clips or shunts in the brain, cardiac pacemakers, or other implanted biomedical devices, as well as those who are pregnant or lactating, were deemed ineligible for tES in accordance with safety guidelines (Antal et al., Reference Antal, Alekseichuk, Bikson, Brockmoller, Brunoni, Chen, Cohen, Dowthwaite, Ellrich, Floel, Fregni, George, Hamilton, Haueisen, Herrmann, Hummel, Lefaucheur, Liebetanz, Loo, Mccaig, Miniussi, Miranda, Moliadze, Nitsche, Nowak, Padberg, Pascual-Leone, Poppendieck, Priori, Rossi, Rossini, Rothwell, Rueger, Ruffini, Schellhorn, Siebner, Ugawa, Wexler, Ziemann, Hallett and Paulus2017; Matsumoto and Ugawa, Reference Matsumoto and Ugawa2017). Participants were instructed to maintain their current treatments, including medications, without any dosage adjustments during the tES treatment.

Administration of tACS

We employed the FDA-approved neuromodulation device called Starstim-8, developed by Neuroelectrics, Inc. (Barcelona, Spain), to address cognitive deficits arising from different causes. The device utilises electrodes connected through wires to a rechargeable battery, delivering electric currents directly to the scalp and brain. Prior to attaching the electrodes, the scalp underwent a gentle cleansing with skin preparation gel. Subsequently, conductive gel was applied to facilitate proper electrode-to-scalp contact. These steps were essential to maintain optimal electrode contact and ensure that the impedance level remained below 5 kΩ (DaSilva et al., Reference Dasilva, Volz, Bikson and Fregni2011).

The montage of electrodes covered the right lateral side of the head at F4 and P4 positions in terms of the 10–20 EEG convention. The peak current intensity was 2.0 mA, with sinewave currents oscillating at 40 Hz and alternating between electrodes F4 and P4, see Figure 1A. The simulation of electric field distribution is illustrated in Figure 1B. During the neuromodulation session, the subjects were asked to practice computerised cognitive training games (https://www.happyneuronpro.com/). To familiarise patients with tES, we implemented a gradual dose escalation strategy during the initial three sessions. This strategy involved the following current levels: 1.0 mA for the first session, 1.5 mA for the second session, and 2.0 mA for the third session. Each stimulation session had a duration of 20 mins, commencing with a 1 min ramp-up phase and concluding with a 30 s ramp-down phase to minimise skin irritation. The 12 treatment sessions were completed over 3 weeks, with sessions interrupted by weekends.

Figure 1. (A) An illustration of the F4 and P4 positions on the scalp. (B) simulation of tACS at 40 Hz over right FP regions.

TOVA administration

Within one week before the initial treatment and after the last neuromodulation session, TOVA was administered via the platform Inquisit (https://www.millisecond.com/). TOVA is an individually administered computerised test designed to evaluate attention and impulse control in both normal and clinical populations (Leark et al., Reference Leark, Greenberg, Kindschi, Dupuy and Hughes2008). This study exclusively reported findings related to the visual mode, although acknowledging the availability of both visual and auditory modes for assessment purposes. The visual TOVA presents two easily distinguishable geometric figures (target and non-target) at the centre of the computer screen. These stimuli appear for 100 milliseconds at intervals of 2000 milliseconds. Participants are instructed to respond to the target stimulus as rapidly as possible. During the first half of the test (stimulus infrequent condition), the target stimulus is presented in 22.5% of the trials (n = 72), whereas during the second half (stimulus frequent condition), it appears in 77.5% of the trials (n = 252). By manipulating the ratio of target to non-target stimuli, the design allows investigating the impact of varying response demands on the performance. Lumping together all the trials, this preliminary report focuses on five overarching indices: mean reaction time (RT), variability of RT, d-Prime (an indicator of the discriminability between target and non-target), commission errors, and omission errors. The TOVA manual provides age- and gender-matched norms (mean and standard deviation) so that the severity of impairment before treatment can be appraised by the converted z-scores.

Results

The age of the 44 patients ranged from 6.2 to 70.3 years (median 19.2), with a mean ± SD of 25.1 ± 17.6 years. The average education was 9.42 years (SD = 5.4). Among them, there were 28 males and 16 females. All patients tolerated the maximum tES current at 2.0 mA. Their diagnoses include ADHD (n = 13), mild to moderate intellectual disability (9), learning disability (4), autism (3), non-amnestic MCI (9), AD-MCI (2), traumatic brain injury (2), and mood disorder (2). The most common co-morbidities for ADHD and non-amnestic MCI were learning disabilities (8 out of 13) and traumatic brain injuries (2 out of 9), respectively. At baseline, the average RT variability and d-Prime are worse than age-matched norms by z-scores 3.3 and 2.3, exceeding the cut-off z-score of 1.645 (RT variability; the 95th cumulative percentile). The z-score of the patient with the least affected attention was 1.46 (the 92.8th cumulative percentile), indicating that all analysed participants had impaired attention. Commission and omission errors are also higher than normal population (z-scores are absent due to deviations of the raw scores from a normal distribution), see Table 1 for detail. The results for the ADHD subgroup are summarised in Table 2, showing a statistical trend similar to that in Table 1. The concurrent psychotropic medications, maintained at consistent dosages throughout the treatment, are as follows: 7 patients were on antipsychotics, 3 on central stimulants, 8 on antidepressants, and 2 on anxiolytics. One-fourth (11 out of 44) of patients were receiving psychotropic medications. The side effects were minimal, with the most common being itching or tingling sensations, consistent with another tACS study we conducted (Lee et al., Reference Lee, Li and Tramontano2024).

Table 1. Paired t-tests of TOVA profiles before and after gamma tACS treatment

RT, reaction time; ms, millisecond; Z-scores are referred to the age-matched norm. One subject has missing data of d-Prime.

Table 2. Paired t-tests of TOVA profiles before and after gamma tACS treatment for ADHD

RT, reaction time; ms, millisecond; Z-scores are referred to the age-matched norm. One subject has missing data of d-Prime.

Discussion

tES has been widely applied as a tool for basic and clinical research (Guleyupoglu et al., Reference Guleyupoglu, Schestatsky, Edwards, Fregni and Bikson2013). In addition to offering a research gateway to modulate brain rhythms and cognitive processes, studies have begun to support tACS as a treatment option for neuropsychiatric conditions (Lee et al., Reference Lee, Li and Tramontano2024; Manippa et al., Reference Manippa, Palmisano, Nitsche, Filardi, Vilella, Logroscino and Rivolta2024b). Previous research has investigated the potential of tACS at the gamma range to ameliorate various cognitive dysfunctions, predominantly episodic memory and working memory (Park et al., Reference Park, Lee, Lee and Im2022; Varastegan et al., Reference Varastegan, Kazemi, Rostami, Khomami, Zandbagleh and Hadipour2023; Wischnewski et al., Reference Wischnewski, Berger, Opitz and Alekseichuk2024; Manippa et al., Reference Manippa, Filardi, Vilella, Logroscino and Rivolta2024a). Relatively few studies have delved into its impact on attention function, primarily focusing on healthy volunteers (Hopfinger et al., Reference Hopfinger, Parsons and Frohlich2017; Wostmann et al., Reference Wostmann, Vosskuhl, Obleser and Herrmann2018). In addition, tACS has also been reported to modulate the frontoparietal attention network and, consequently, influence emotional attention (Hu et al., Reference Hu, He, Liu, Ren and Liu2021). Using TOVA, we evaluated attention function changes subsequent to 12 treatment sessions of 2mA tACS at 40 Hz over right frontal and parietal regions, aiming to ameliorate cognitive deficits stemming from diverse diagnoses. Through the comparisons of post-treatment minus baseline TOVA indices, our results showed that the treatment protocol benefited several, not all, aspects of attention function, including reduction of variability in RT, decrease of commission error, and improvement in separability of target and non-target, that is, d-Prime. The mean RT and omission error rate largely remained unchanged.

Each of the five selected indices from TOVA addresses a different domain of attention function (Leark et al., Reference Leark, Greenberg, Kindschi, Dupuy and Hughes2007). RT provides a general measure of processing speed and efficiency, whereas the variability of RT indicates sustained attention or the stability of performance. Commission and omission error rates, respectively, reflect impulsivity (tendency to respond to non-target stimuli) and attentional lapses (failure to respond to target stimuli). The former is relevant to deficits in response inhibition or inhibitory control and the latter to deficits in vigilance or sustained attention. Overall, our results implied that gamma tACS over right FP network may benefit the consistency in performance and inhibitory control but has little impact on processing speed and attentional lapses. It is not surprising that the mean RT was resistant to the impact of tACS since the task instruction requires the participants to respond as quickly as possible (p.3 in professional manual (Leark et al., Reference Leark, Greenberg, Kindschi, Dupuy and Hughes2008)). The improvement in discriminating between targets and non-targets may be more closely linked to the decrease in commission errors rather than omission errors, which appeared to be unaffected by the treatment.

Complicated neural substrates participate in the attention functioning. For example, previous functional MRI research revealed that greater pre-stimulus brain activities in the default-mode network were associated with longer RT, and the BOLD responses in the posterior cingulate, left inferior frontal gyrus, and left middle temporal gyrus increased proportionally with RT (Tam et al., Reference Tam, Luedke, Walsh, Fernandez-Ruiz and Garcia2015). In contrast, RT variability (intra-individual, as in TOVA) was predicted by the activity in the left anterior cingulate cortex (Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Pinar, Fornito, Nandam, Hester and Bellgrove2015). Error-related processing involved a broad neural matrix, including anterior cingulate cortex, pre-supplementary motor area, bilateral insula, thalamus, and inferior parietal lobule (Hester et al., Reference Hester, Fassbender and Garavan2004). The neural mechanisms behind the therapeutic benefits of tACS are still being investigated. Nevertheless, with tES initiating low-resolution and wide-ranging neuromodulation (see Figure 1.), we posit that our tACS targeting the FP network may influence extensive neural substrates beyond those underneath F4 and P4, thereby modulating neural networks/nodes related to attentional function.

Our research samples were drawn from a heterogeneous mix of neuropsychiatric populations. In conjunction with sporadic reports supporting the efficacy of gamma tACS in enhancing attention (Hopfinger et al., Reference Hopfinger, Parsons and Frohlich2017; Wostmann et al., Reference Wostmann, Vosskuhl, Obleser and Herrmann2018), we propose that gamma tACS may serve as a potential therapeutic intervention for a wide range of neuropsychiatric conditions, irrespective of specific diagnoses. Including heterogeneous populations in the design may unveil the utility of tACS as a generic attention modulator. However, due to significant variations in the underlying mechanisms of attention and cognitive impairments across different neuropsychiatric disorders, the efficacy of tACS in enhancing attention and cognition is likely to vary among different diagnoses. Attention, a fundamental cognitive capacity, enables individuals to allocate resources and maintain focus on cognitive tasks to facilitate various cognitive functions. This study offers a new caveat for research on the use of gamma tACS to improve cognitive impairment, prompting consideration of whether the cognitive enhancement observed with tACS treatment is attributable to improved attention. Although this study includes only 12 sessions based on previous reports (Lee et al., Reference Lee, Li and Tramontano2024; Manippa et al., Reference Manippa, Palmisano, Nitsche, Filardi, Vilella, Logroscino and Rivolta2024b), extended maintenance treatment may be necessary to solidify the beneficial responses. We acknowledged that the results of this preliminary report could be contaminated by practice effects. In addition, including a severity criterion to better stratify participants may help in reducing heterogeneity and allow for more precise conclusions. Double-blind and sham-controlled research with well-defined participant selection guidelines is warranted to verify its efficacy and to clarify its role as an adjunct treatment for various conditions of attention and cognitive impairment.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/neu.2024.35.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by NeuroCognitive Institute (NCI) and NCI Clinical Research Foundation Inc.

Author contributions

All authors contributed intellectually to this work. TW Lee and A Sergio carried out the analysis and wrote the first draft together. All authors revised and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Funding statement

None.

Competing interests

All authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Ethical standards

This IRB-approved research analysed the databank collected from 2018 to 2022. The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

Footnotes

Tien-Wen Lee and Sergio Almeida contributed equally to this research as co-first authors.

References

Antal, A, Alekseichuk, I, Bikson, M, Brockmoller, J, Brunoni, AR, Chen, R, Cohen, LG, Dowthwaite, G, Ellrich, J, Floel, A, Fregni, F, George, MS, Hamilton, R, Haueisen, J, Herrmann, CS, Hummel, FC, Lefaucheur, JP, Liebetanz, D, Loo, CK, Mccaig, CD, Miniussi, C, Miranda, PC, Moliadze, V, Nitsche, MA, Nowak, R, Padberg, F, Pascual-Leone, A, Poppendieck, W, Priori, A, Rossi, S, Rossini, PM, Rothwell, J, Rueger, MA, Ruffini, G, Schellhorn, K, Siebner, HR, Ugawa, Y, Wexler, A, Ziemann, U, Hallett, M and Paulus, W (2017) Low intensity transcranial electric stimulation: safety, ethical, legal regulatory and application guidelines. Clinical Neurophysiology 128, 17741809.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brookes, MJ, Wood, JR, Stevenson, CM, Zumer, JM, White, TP, Liddle, PF and Morris, PG (2011) Changes in brain network activity during working memory tasks: a magnetoencephalography study. Neuroimage 55, 18041815.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brzezicka, A, Kaminski, J, Reed, CM, Chung, JM, Mamelak, AN and Rutishauser, U (2019) Working memory load-related theta power decreases in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex predict individual differences in performance. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 31, 12901307.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dallmer-Zerbe, I, Popp, F, Lam, AP, Philipsen, A and Herrmann, CS (2020) Transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) as a tool to modulate P300 amplitude in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): preliminary findings. Brain Topography 33, 191207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dasilva, AF, Volz, MS, Bikson, M and Fregni, F (2011) Electrode positioning and montage in transcranial direct current stimulation. Journal of Visualized Experiments 23, 2744.Google Scholar
Diedrich, L, Kolhoff, HI, Chakalov, I, Vékony, T, Németh, D and Antal, A (2024) Prefrontal theta-gamma transcranial alternating current stimulation improves non-declarative visuomotor learning in older adults. Scientific Reports 14, 4955.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fan, J, Byrne, J, Worden, MS, Guise, KG, Mccandliss, BD, Fossella, J and Posner, MI (2007) The relation of brain oscillations to attentional networks. Journal of Neuroscience 27, 61976206.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fell, J, Klaver, P, Elger, CE and Fernandez, G (2002) Suppression of EEG gamma activity may cause the attentional blink. Consciousness and Cognition 11, 114122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foster, BL and Parvizi, J (2012) Resting oscillations and cross-frequency coupling in the human posteromedial cortex. Neuroimage 60, 384391.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodman, MS, Kumar, S, Zomorrodi, R, Ghazala, Z, Cheam, AS, Barr, MS, Daskalakis, ZJ, Blumberger, DM, Fischer, C and Flint, A (2018) Theta-gamma coupling and working memory in Alzheimer’s dementia and mild cognitive impairment. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience 10, 101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guleyupoglu, B, Schestatsky, P, Edwards, D, Fregni, F and Bikson, M (2013) Classification of methods in transcranial electrical stimulation (tES) and evolving strategy from historical approaches to contemporary innovations. Journal of Neuroscience Methods 219, 297311.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herrmann, CS, Fründ, I, Lenz, D (2010) Human gamma-band activity: a review on cognitive and behavioral correlates and network models. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 34, 981992.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hester, R, Fassbender, C and Garavan, H (2004) Individual differences in error processing: a review and reanalysis of three event-related fMRI studies using the GO/NOGO task. Cerebral Cortex 14, 986994.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hopfinger, JB, Parsons, J and Frohlich, F (2017) Differential effects of 10-Hz and 40-Hz transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) on endogenous versus exogenous attention. Cognitive Neuroscience 8, 102111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hu, P, He, Y, Liu, X, Ren, Z and Liu, S (2021)Modulating emotion processing using transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) - a sham-controlled study in healthy human participants. Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society 2021, 66676670.Google ScholarPubMed
Johnson, BP, Pinar, A, Fornito, A, Nandam, LS, Hester, R and Bellgrove, MA (2015) Left anterior cingulate activity predicts intra-individual reaction time variability in healthy adults. Neuropsychologia 72, 2226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klimesch, W (1999) EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: a review and analysis. Brain Research Reviews 29, 169195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leark, RA, Greenberg, LM, Kindschi, C, Dupuy, T and Hughes, SJ (2008) TOVA professional manual. Los Alamitos, CA: TOVA Company.Google Scholar
Leark, RA, Greenberg, LM, Kindschi, CL, Dupuy, TR and Hughes, SJ (2007) Test of Variables of Attention Continuous Performance Test. Los Alamitos, CA: The TOVA Company.Google Scholar
Lee, TW, Li, CS and Tramontano, G (2024) Tripod transcranial alternating current stimulation at 5-Hz over right hemisphere may relieve anxiety symptoms: a preliminary report. Journal of Affective Disorders 360, 156162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, TW and Tramontano, G (2024) Neural consequences of 5-Hz transcranial alternating current stimulation over right hemisphere: an eLORETA EEG study. Neuroscience Letters 835, 137849.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lundqvist, M, Herman, P, Warden, MR, Brincat, SL and Miller, EK (2018) Gamma and beta bursts during working memory readout suggest roles in its volitional control. Nature Communications 9, 394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manippa, V, Filardi, M, Vilella, D, Logroscino, G and Rivolta, D (2024a) Gamma (60 Hz) auditory stimulation improves intrusions but not recall and working memory in healthy adults. Behavioural Brain Research 456, 114703.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manippa, V, Palmisano, A, Nitsche, MA, Filardi, M, Vilella, D, Logroscino, G and Rivolta, D (2024b) Cognitive and neuropathophysiological outcomes of gamma-tACS in dementia: a systematic review. Neuropsychology Review 34, 338361.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matsumoto, H and Ugawa, Y (2017) Adverse events of tDCS and tACS: a review. Clinical Neurophysiology Practice 2, 1925.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Park, J, Lee, C, Lee, S and Im, CH (2022) 80 Hz but not 40 Hz, transcranial alternating current stimulation of 80 Hz over right intraparietal sulcus increases visuospatial working memory capacity. Scientific Reports 12, 13762.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shipp, S (2004) The brain circuitry of attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8, 223230.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tam, A, Luedke, AC, Walsh, JJ, Fernandez-Ruiz, J and Garcia, A (2015) Effects of reaction time variability and age on brain activity during Stroop task performance. Brain Imaging and Behavior 9, 609618.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Schotten, MThiebaut, Dell’acqua, F, Forkel, SJ, Simmons, A, Vergani, F, Murphy, DG and Catani, M (2011) A lateralized brain network for visuospatial attention. Nature Neuroscience 14, 12451246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Varastegan, S, Kazemi, R, Rostami, R, Khomami, S, Zandbagleh, A and Hadipour, AL (2023) Remember NIBS? tACS improves memory performance in elders with subjective memory complaints. GeroScience 45, 851869.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wischnewski, M, Berger, TA, Opitz, A and Alekseichuk, I (2024) Causal functional maps of brain rhythms in working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 121, e2318528121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wostmann, M, Vosskuhl, J, Obleser, J and Herrmann, CS (2018) Opposite effects of lateralised transcranial alpha versus gamma stimulation on auditory spatial attention. Brain Stimulation 11, 752758.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zaghi, S, De Freitas Rezende, L, De Oliveira, LM, El-Nazer, R, Menning, S, Tadini, L and Fregni, F (2010) Inhibition of motor cortex excitability with 15Hz transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS). Neuroscience Letters 479, 211214.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. (A) An illustration of the F4 and P4 positions on the scalp. (B) simulation of tACS at 40 Hz over right FP regions.

Figure 1

Table 1. Paired t-tests of TOVA profiles before and after gamma tACS treatment

Figure 2

Table 2. Paired t-tests of TOVA profiles before and after gamma tACS treatment for ADHD

Supplementary material: File

Lee et al. supplementary material 1

Lee et al. supplementary material
Download Lee et al. supplementary material 1(File)
File 293.8 KB
Supplementary material: File

Lee et al. supplementary material 2

Lee et al. supplementary material
Download Lee et al. supplementary material 2(File)
File 358.7 KB