Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-7cvxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T07:54:01.879Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Nonconvergence on the native speaker grammar: Defining L2 success

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 February 2018

LYDIA WHITE*
Affiliation:
McGill University
*
Address for correspondence: Dept. of Linguistics, McGill University, 1085 Dr. Penfield Avenue, Montreal, QC, H3A 1A7, Canada[email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Extract

The issue of critical or sensitive periods affecting the outcome of second language (L2) acquisition has been the subject of intense investigation and debate for many years, with people arguing for or against maturational effects on ultimate attainment. In their influential paper, Johnson and Newport (1989) identify two hypotheses: the exercise hypothesis and the maturational state hypothesis. According to the former, if the capacity for acquiring language is exercised early in life (in first language acquisition), then language learning abilities will remain intact throughout life: in other words, permitting successful L2 acquisition regardless of age. In contrast, according to the maturational state hypothesis, the language learning capacity declines with age, affecting L2 acquisition as well as late L1. Johnson and Newport take their results, which show an age-related decline in performance during childhood and adolescence, to support the maturational state hypothesis. Many L2 researchers have adopted a maturational perspective and have reached similar conclusions as to the presence of critical or sensitive periods (e.g., Abrahamsson, 2012; DeKeyser & Larson-Hall, 2005; Long, 1990; Oyama, 1976; Patkowski, 1980). Some researchers have pointed out that age effects continue into adulthood, contrary to the claim for a critical period (e.g., Birdsong & Mollis, 2001). Others have suggested that what looks like an age-related maturational decline may be accounted for by confounding factors, such as task effects, effects of L1, amount of L2 use, education or input (e.g., Bialystok & Miller, 1999; Flege, Yeni-Komshian & Liu, 1999).

Type
Peer Commentaries
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2018 

The issue of critical or sensitive periods affecting the outcome of second language (L2) acquisition has been the subject of intense investigation and debate for many years, with people arguing for or against maturational effects on ultimate attainment. In their influential paper, Johnson and Newport (Reference Johnson and Newport1989) identify two hypotheses: the exercise hypothesis and the maturational state hypothesis. According to the former, if the capacity for acquiring language is exercised early in life (in first language acquisition), then language learning abilities will remain intact throughout life: in other words, permitting successful L2 acquisition regardless of age. In contrast, according to the maturational state hypothesis, the language learning capacity declines with age, affecting L2 acquisition as well as late L1. Johnson and Newport take their results, which show an age-related decline in performance during childhood and adolescence, to support the maturational state hypothesis. Many L2 researchers have adopted a maturational perspective and have reached similar conclusions as to the presence of critical or sensitive periods (e.g., Abrahamsson, Reference Abrahamsson2012; DeKeyser & Larson-Hall, Reference DeKeyser, Larson-Hall, Kroll and de Groot2005; Long, Reference Long1990; Oyama, Reference Oyama1976; Patkowski, Reference Patkowski1980). Some researchers have pointed out that age effects continue into adulthood, contrary to the claim for a critical period (e.g., Birdsong & Mollis, Reference Birdsong and Molis2001). Others have suggested that what looks like an age-related maturational decline may be accounted for by confounding factors, such as task effects, effects of L1, amount of L2 use, education or input (e.g., Bialystok & Miller, Reference Bialystok and Miller1999; Flege, Yeni-Komshian & Liu, Reference Flege, Yeni-Komshian and Liu1999).

Mayberry and Kluender (Reference Mayberry and Kluender2017), as well as Mayberry and colleagues in earlier work (e.g., Mayberry, Reference Mayberry1993; Mayberry & Lock, Reference Mayberry and Lock2003), have been amongst the few to explore the other possibility, namely the exercise hypothesis. In a number of studies, Mayberry and colleagues have compared L1 and L2 learners of ASL, showing that, when age of acquisition is controlled for, L2ers outperform late L1 learners in a number of respects. In addition, native signers acquiring (written) L2 English perform like hearing learners in demonstrating considerable success in L2 acquisition, in contrast to late L1 acquirers learning an L2. In other words, as these researchers have demonstrated, a crucial predictor of L2 success is timing of L1 acquisition.

A central issue that faces both the exercise and the maturational approaches is how to define successful outcomes, how to determine the nature of ultimate attainment. In the literature on age effects in L2, it is usually taken for granted that success is to be defined in terms of convergence on the L2 grammar (or ‘target’ language); hence, monolingual native speakers provide the comparison group. Typically, researchers have picked a number of linguistic phenomena and have compared the performance of L2ers to native speakers, showing that L2ers either do or do not converge on native speaker performance on the chosen properties. Failure to converge is taken to indicate some kind of deficiency in the L2ers’ ultimate attainment.

However, it is important to bear in mind that the acquisition task involves coming up with a linguistic system that allows the learner to use the L2 (in comprehension and production). The task is not to arrive at a grammar identical to that of a native speaker. As a number of researchers have pointed out, comparing L2ers to monolingual native speakers is not necessarily the most appropriate way to establish their unconscious knowledge of the L2 or the nature of their ultimate attainment. Rather, the linguistic systems achieved by L2ers need to be considered in their own right (e.g., Bley-Vroman, Reference Bley-Vroman1983; Cook, Reference Cook1997). While interlanguage grammars may differ from those of native speakers, this does not make them defective or any less systematic than a native speaker grammar. One interesting possibility, then, which the exercise hypothesis allows one to entertain, is that late acquired L1s are indeed in some sense defective (or incomplete, a term sometimes used in the context of heritage language acquisition (e.g., Montrul, Reference Montrul, Lefebvre, White and Jourdan2006)) whereas late acquired L2s are not. Constructing a complex linguistic system may not be possible in late L1 acquisition but it certainly is possible in late L2.

References

Abrahamsson, N. (2012). Age of onset and nativelike L2 ultimate attainment of morphosyntactic and phonetic intuition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 34, 187214.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., & Miller, B. (1999). The problem of age in second-language acquisition: Influences from language, structure and task. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 2, 127145.Google Scholar
Birdsong, D., & Molis, M. (2001). On the evidence for maturational constraints in second-language acquisition. Journal of Memory and Language, 44, 235249.Google Scholar
Bley-Vroman, R. (1983). The comparative fallacy in interlanguage studies: the case of systematicity. Language Learning, 33, 117.Google Scholar
Cook, V. (1997). Monolingual bias in second language acquisition research. Revista Canaria de Estudios Ingleses, 34, 3550.Google Scholar
DeKeyser, R., & Larson-Hall, J. (2005). What does the critical period really mean? In Kroll, J. & de Groot, A. (eds.), Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches, pp. 88108. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Flege, J. E., Yeni-Komshian, G. H., & Liu, S. (1999). Age constraints on second-language acquisition. Journal of Memory and Language, 41, 78104.Google Scholar
Johnson, J., & Newport, E. (1989). Critical period effects in second language learning: The influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language. Cognitive Psychology, 21, 6099.Google Scholar
Long, M. (1990). Maturational constraints on language development. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 12, 251285.Google Scholar
Mayberry, R. (1993). First language acquisition after childhood differs from second language acquisition: The case of American Sign Language. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 36, 12581270.Google Scholar
Mayberry, R. I., & Kluender, R. (2017). Rethinking the critical period for language: New insights into an old question from American Sign Language. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition doi:10.1017/S1366728917000724Google Scholar
Mayberry, R., & Lock, E.. (2003). Age constraints on first versus second language acquisition: Evidence of linguistic plasticity and epigenesis. Brain and Language, 87, 369384.Google Scholar
Montrul, S. (2006). Incomplete acquisition in bilingualism as an instance of language change. In Lefebvre, C., White, L. & Jourdan, C. (eds.), L2 acquisition and creole genesis, pp. 379400. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Oyama, S. (1976). A sensitive period for the acquisition of a nonnative phonological system. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 5, 261472.Google Scholar
Patkowski, M. (1980). The sensitive period for the acquisition of syntax in a second language. Language Learning, 30, 449472.Google Scholar